首页 > 最新文献

Contemporary Educational Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Revisiting the relation between academic buoyancy and coping: A network analysis 重新审视学业浮力与应对之间的关系:网络分析
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102283
David W. Putwain , Martin Daumiller , Tahrim Hussain , Reinhard Pekrun

Academic buoyancy, the capacity to respond to minor academic adversities, is expected to enable students to effectively deal with failure. Prior research, however, has shown negligible relations between buoyancy and coping, but only considered a limited set of coping strategies. In addition, academic buoyancy and effective coping are expected to positively relate to higher academic achievement. However, studies examining how coping could mediate relations from academic buoyancy to achievement are lacking. In the present study (N = 535 upper secondary students, mean age 16.4 years), we examined relations between students’ buoyancy, coping with an examination failure, and academic achievement. We considered an extensive set of nine coping strategies (five adaptive, four maladaptive) and used a novel network analysis, alongside traditional analytic approaches (correlation, structural equation modelling). Buoyancy and coping were assessed with self-report, and achievement from an end-of-year examination. Buoyancy was positively related with adaptive, and negatively with maladaptive, coping strategies both in structural equation modeling and in the network analysis. In addition, structural equation modeling showed positive and negative indirect relations between buoyancy and achievement that were mediated by adaptive coping strategies. Our findings suggest that buoyancy interventions to enhance adaptive, and reduce maladaptive, coping strategies could be suitable ways to help students overcome examination failure.

学业浮力,即应对学业小挫折的能力,有望使学生有效应对失败。然而,先前的研究表明,浮力和应对之间的关系微乎其微,但只考虑了有限的应对策略。此外,学业浮力和有效的应对方法有望与更高的学业成绩产生积极的联系。然而,目前还缺乏研究来探讨应对如何从学业浮力到学业成绩之间的关系。在本研究中(研究对象为 535 名高中生,平均年龄为 16.4 岁),我们考察了学生的学习积极性、应对考试失败的方法和学业成绩之间的关系。我们考虑了广泛的九种应对策略(五种适应性策略和四种适应不良策略),并在使用传统分析方法(相关、结构方程模型)的同时,还使用了新颖的网络分析方法。通过自我报告和年终考试成绩来评估浮力和应对策略。在结构方程模型和网络分析中,浮力与适应性应对策略呈正相关,与适应性不良应对策略呈负相关。此外,结构方程模型还显示,浮力与成绩之间存在正负间接关系,而这种关系又以适应性应对策略为中介。我们的研究结果表明,采取浮力干预措施来增强适应性应对策略和减少适应性应对策略,可能是帮助学生克服考试失败的合适方法。
{"title":"Revisiting the relation between academic buoyancy and coping: A network analysis","authors":"David W. Putwain ,&nbsp;Martin Daumiller ,&nbsp;Tahrim Hussain ,&nbsp;Reinhard Pekrun","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102283","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102283","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Academic buoyancy, the capacity to respond to minor academic adversities, is expected to enable students to effectively deal with failure. Prior research, however, has shown negligible relations between buoyancy and coping, but only considered a limited set of coping strategies. In addition, academic buoyancy and effective coping are expected to positively relate to higher academic achievement. However, studies examining how coping could mediate relations from academic buoyancy to achievement are lacking. In the present study (N = 535 upper secondary students, mean age 16.4 years), we examined relations between students’ buoyancy, coping with an examination failure, and academic achievement. We considered an extensive set of nine coping strategies (five adaptive, four maladaptive) and used a novel network analysis, alongside traditional analytic approaches (correlation, structural equation modelling). Buoyancy and coping were assessed with self-report, and achievement from an end-of-year examination. Buoyancy was positively related with adaptive, and negatively with maladaptive, coping strategies both in structural equation modeling and in the network analysis. In addition, structural equation modeling showed positive and negative indirect relations between buoyancy and achievement that were mediated by adaptive coping strategies. Our findings suggest that buoyancy interventions to enhance adaptive, and reduce maladaptive, coping strategies could be suitable ways to help students overcome examination failure.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0361476X24000286/pdfft?md5=c6c430566403f129a9b5f9160074e202&pid=1-s2.0-S0361476X24000286-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141278482","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of element interactivity and mental rehearsal on working memory resource depletion and the spacing effect 元素互动性和心理预演对工作记忆资源耗竭和间隔效应的影响
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102281
Ouhao Chen , Bobo Kai Yin Chan , Ellie Anderson , Rory O’sullivan , Tim Jay , Kim Ouwehand , Fred Paas , John Sweller

The spacing effect occurs when learning with rest periods is superior to learning without rest periods. Cognitive load theory has explained this superiority by working memory resource depletion, under which resources are depleted during cognitive activity but restored with rest. In a series of four experiments involving 341 participants, we explored the relationships between the spacing effect, depletion of working memory resources, and mental rehearsal, particularly focusing on how these dynamics are influenced by task complexity as defined by element interactivity. Experiment 1 showed that materials with higher element interactivity had a greater impact on working memory resource depletion. In Experiment 2, using materials low in element interactivity, a spacing effect was obtained with no evidence of working memory resource depletion. Instead, results suggested that the effect might be due to mental rehearsal occurring during rest periods. Experiment 3, using more complex information, obtained both the spacing and working memory resource depletion effects for less knowledgeable learners for whom the information was high in element interactivity. In Experiment 4, testing more knowledgeable learners for whom the same information was lower in element interactivity, both effects disappeared. The results indicated that working memory resource depletion and recovery may be more sensitive to materials high in element interactivity and suggest that it is only one of multiple causes of the spacing effect.

当有休息时间的学习优于没有休息时间的学习时,就会产生间隔效应。认知负荷理论用工作记忆资源耗竭来解释这种优越性,即在认知活动中工作记忆资源会耗竭,但在休息时工作记忆资源会恢复。在由 341 名参与者参与的四个系列实验中,我们探讨了间距效应、工作记忆资源耗竭和心理预演之间的关系,尤其关注了这些动态变化如何受到由元素互动性定义的任务复杂性的影响。实验 1 显示,元素互动性越高的材料对工作记忆资源消耗的影响越大。在实验 2 中,使用元素交互性较低的材料,获得了间隔效应,但没有工作记忆资源耗竭的证据。相反,实验结果表明,这种效应可能是由于在休息期间进行的心理预演造成的。实验 3 使用了更复杂的信息,对于知识较少的学习者来说,信息的元素交互性较高,因此获得了间距效应和工作记忆资源耗竭效应。在实验 4 中,对知识水平较高的学习者进行测试,同样的信息对他们来说元素互动性较低,这两种效应都消失了。结果表明,工作记忆资源耗竭和恢复可能对元素互动性高的材料更为敏感,并表明这只是间距效应的多种原因之一。
{"title":"The effect of element interactivity and mental rehearsal on working memory resource depletion and the spacing effect","authors":"Ouhao Chen ,&nbsp;Bobo Kai Yin Chan ,&nbsp;Ellie Anderson ,&nbsp;Rory O’sullivan ,&nbsp;Tim Jay ,&nbsp;Kim Ouwehand ,&nbsp;Fred Paas ,&nbsp;John Sweller","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102281","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102281","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>The spacing effect occurs when learning with rest periods is superior to learning without rest periods. Cognitive load theory has explained this superiority by working memory resource depletion, under which resources are depleted during cognitive activity but restored with rest. In a series of four experiments involving 341 participants, we explored the relationships between the spacing effect, depletion of working memory resources, and mental rehearsal, particularly focusing on how these dynamics are influenced by task complexity as defined by element interactivity. Experiment 1 showed that materials with higher element interactivity had a greater impact on working memory resource depletion. In Experiment 2, using materials low in element interactivity, a spacing effect was obtained with no evidence of working memory resource depletion. Instead, results suggested that the effect might be due to mental rehearsal occurring during rest periods. Experiment 3, using more complex information, obtained both the spacing and working memory resource depletion effects for less knowledgeable learners for whom the information was high in element interactivity. In Experiment 4, testing more knowledgeable learners for whom the same information was lower in element interactivity, both effects disappeared. The results indicated that working memory resource depletion and recovery may be more sensitive to materials high in element interactivity and suggest that it is only one of multiple causes of the spacing effect.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0361476X24000262/pdfft?md5=4753d694ca756c0dd36842ab8cf04d41&pid=1-s2.0-S0361476X24000262-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141137721","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Development in context: Comparing short-term trajectories of expectancy, task values, and costs in four university STEM courses 背景下的发展:比较四门大学 STEM 课程中期望值、任务价值和成本的短期轨迹
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102282
So Yeon Lee, Ella Christiaans, Kristy A. Robinson

University students studying science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) tend to lose motivation, on average, over time. However, detailed understanding of when and how students are most likely to lose motivation, as well as whether motivational trajectories differ across contexts, is lacking. Accordingly, we investigated trajectories of expectancy, task values, and perceived costs in four introductory STEM courses (N = 2,104). Latent change score models revealed nuanced patterns of change, indicating average motivational declines in expectancy and task values and average increases in cost perceptions observed in prior research tend to occur primarily at the beginning of the semester. Motivational trajectories also differed in the shape and timing of mean changes across the four STEM courses, suggesting motivational loss trends may not be universal. Variation in trajectories also related to course performance and major persistence, with different motivational beliefs playing different roles at various times in the semester. Findings highlighted the importance of early course performance in predicting motivational change, as well as the importance of motivational changes for short- and long-term outcomes. Findings underscore the contextualized nature of motivational constructs, as both change patterns and relations to correlates differed across settings, indicating a need for researchers to carefully consider how samples are aggregated and further uncover contextual features that shape motivation. Overall, our study findings add essential understanding of motivational strengths and needs in real-world classroom contexts, with implications for the design and timing of intervention for instructors by examining what developmental patterns are common across settings and which are more particular to specific settings.

学习科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)的大学生往往会随着时间的推移而失去学习动力。然而,对于学生何时和如何最有可能丧失学习动机,以及不同情境下的学习动机轨迹是否存在差异,我们还缺乏详细的了解。因此,我们调查了四门 STEM 入门课程(人数 = 2,104)中的期望值、任务价值和感知成本的轨迹。潜在变化得分模型揭示了细微的变化模式,表明先前研究中观察到的期望值和任务价值的平均动机下降以及成本感知的平均上升主要发生在学期初。四门 STEM 课程的动机轨迹在平均变化的形状和时间上也有所不同,这表明动机丧失的趋势可能并不普遍。动机轨迹的变化还与课程成绩和主修课程的持续性有关,不同的动机信念在学期的不同时期发挥着不同的作用。研究结果强调了早期课程成绩对预测动机变化的重要性,以及动机变化对短期和长期结果的重要性。研究结果强调了动机建构的情境性,因为在不同的情境下,动机的变化模式以及与相关因素的关系都不尽相同,这表明研究人员需要仔细考虑如何汇总样本,并进一步揭示形成动机的情境特征。总之,我们的研究结果增加了对真实世界课堂环境中动机优势和需求的基本了解,通过研究哪些发展模式在不同环境中是共同的,哪些是特定环境中更为特殊的,从而对教师干预的设计和时机选择产生影响。
{"title":"Development in context: Comparing short-term trajectories of expectancy, task values, and costs in four university STEM courses","authors":"So Yeon Lee,&nbsp;Ella Christiaans,&nbsp;Kristy A. Robinson","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102282","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102282","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>University students studying science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) tend to lose motivation, on average, over time. However, detailed understanding of when and how students are most likely to lose motivation, as well as whether motivational trajectories differ across contexts, is lacking. Accordingly, we investigated trajectories of expectancy, task values, and perceived costs in four introductory STEM courses (<em>N</em> = 2,104). Latent change score models revealed nuanced patterns of change, indicating average motivational declines in expectancy and task values and average increases in cost perceptions observed in prior research tend to occur primarily at the beginning of the semester. Motivational trajectories also differed in the shape and timing of mean changes across the four STEM courses, suggesting motivational loss trends may not be universal. Variation in trajectories also related to course performance and major persistence, with different motivational beliefs playing different roles at various times in the semester. Findings highlighted the importance of early course performance in predicting motivational change, as well as the importance of motivational changes for short- and long-term outcomes. Findings underscore the contextualized nature of motivational constructs, as both change patterns and relations to correlates differed across settings, indicating a need for researchers to carefully consider how samples are aggregated and further uncover contextual features that shape motivation. Overall, our study findings add essential understanding of motivational strengths and needs in real-world classroom contexts, with implications for the design and timing of intervention for instructors by examining what developmental patterns are common across settings and which are more particular to specific settings.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-06-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141282108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
PhD students’ motivation profiles: A self-determination theory perspective 博士生的学习动机概况:自我决定理论视角
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-11 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102279
David Litalien , István Tóth-Király , Frédéric Guay , Alexandre J.S. Morin

Previous research has underscored the importance of understanding the mechanisms that underpin the academic motivation of PhD students. This understanding is crucial for enhancing their educational experience and program completion rates. Based on self-determination theory, this person-centered study first investigated PhD students’ motivation profiles defined on their types of academic motivations. Second, we explored associations between these profiles and various predictors (need satisfaction and support) and educational outcomes (persistence, satisfaction, future intentions, and performance). Third, we systematically tested the similarity of these profiles and their associations with predictors and outcomes across subgroups of students based on sex, field of study, citizenship, and program progression. Using a sample of 1060 Canadian PhD students, four distinct profiles emerged from the latent profile analyses: Low self-determined, Introjected, Identified, and High self-determined. Profile membership was predicted by need satisfaction and perceived support from faculty members. The most desirable outcome levels were associated with the High self-determination profile, followed by the Identified, Introjected and Low self-determined profiles. These profiles and their associations with predictors and outcomes were highly similar across the different subgroups. From a practical perspective, our results allowed us to identify students with less optimal motivation configurations and to propose intervention strategies, particularly focused on students’ need for autonomy, to support more desirable motivational profiles.

以往的研究强调了了解支撑博士生学术动机的机制的重要性。这种理解对于提高他们的教育体验和课程完成率至关重要。基于自我决定理论,这项以人为本的研究首先调查了博士生的学习动机类型。其次,我们探讨了这些特征与各种预测因素(需求满足和支持)和教育结果(坚持性、满意度、未来意向和表现)之间的关联。第三,我们根据性别、研究领域、公民身份和课程进度,系统地测试了这些特征的相似性,以及它们与不同学生子群体的预测因素和结果之间的关联。通过对 1060 名加拿大博士生进行抽样调查,潜在特征分析得出了四种不同的特征:低自我决定型、注入型、认同型和高自我决定型。需求满意度和感知到的教职员工的支持可以预测学生的特征。最理想的结果水平与 "高自我决定 "特征相关,其次是 "认同"、"引入 "和 "低自我决定 "特征。在不同的分组中,这些特征及其与预测因素和结果的关系非常相似。从实用的角度来看,我们的研究结果使我们能够识别出动机配置不太理想的学生,并提出干预策略,特别是关注学生的自主需求,以支持更理想的动机配置。
{"title":"PhD students’ motivation profiles: A self-determination theory perspective","authors":"David Litalien ,&nbsp;István Tóth-Király ,&nbsp;Frédéric Guay ,&nbsp;Alexandre J.S. Morin","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102279","DOIUrl":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102279","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research has underscored the importance of understanding the mechanisms that underpin the academic motivation of PhD students. This understanding is crucial for enhancing their educational experience and program completion rates. Based on self-determination theory, this person-centered study first investigated PhD students’ motivation profiles defined on their types of academic motivations. Second, we explored associations between these profiles and various predictors (need satisfaction and support) and educational outcomes (persistence, satisfaction, future intentions, and performance). Third, we systematically tested the similarity of these profiles and their associations with predictors and outcomes across subgroups of students based on sex, field of study, citizenship, and program progression. Using a sample of 1060 Canadian PhD students, four distinct profiles emerged from the latent profile analyses: <em>Low self-determined</em>, <em>Introjected</em>, <em>Identified</em>, and <em>High self-determined</em>. Profile membership was predicted by need satisfaction and perceived support from faculty members. The most desirable outcome levels were associated with the <em>High self-determination</em> profile, followed by the <em>Identified</em>, <em>Introjected</em> and <em>Low self-determined</em> profiles. These profiles and their associations with predictors and outcomes were highly similar across the different subgroups. From a practical perspective, our results allowed us to identify students with less optimal motivation configurations and to propose intervention strategies, particularly focused on students’ need for autonomy, to support more desirable motivational profiles.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0361476X24000249/pdfft?md5=1d59f73d86131ec0f22e84ef57157351&pid=1-s2.0-S0361476X24000249-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141052698","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does the Jigsaw method improve motivation and self-regulation in vocational high schools? 拼图法能否提高职业高中的学习动机和自我调节能力?
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-07 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102278
Mathilde Riant , Anne-Laure de Place , Pascal Bressoux , Anatolia Batruch , Marinette Bouet , Marco Bressan , Genavee Brown , Fabrizio Butera , Carlos Cepeda , Anthony Cherbonnier , Céline Darnon , Marie Demolliens , Olivier Desrichard , Théo Ducros , Luc Goron , Brivael Hémon , Pascal Huguet , Eric Jamet , Ruben Martinez , Vincent Mazenod , Pascal Pansu

Although much has been written about the beneficial effects of the Jigsaw method, little is known about how it affects students' motivation and self-regulation processes. In this study, we tested its effects on students' trajectories of autonomous mathematics motivation and academic self-regulation. We also examined whether these effects could be moderated by the students’ cooperative attitudes and initial mathematics achievement level. 4,698 students from French vocational high schools participated in the study over two years. They were divided into three groups: 1,641 were assigned to a cooperative learning condition with the Jigsaw method, 1,602 to a weakly structured cooperative learning condition, and 1,455 to a business-as-usual learning condition. Self-reported mathematics motivation, academic self-regulation, and cooperative attitudes were collected three times during the study. Overall, the multilevel growth model results indicate a general decline in students’ motivation and self-regulation, and student-reported cooperative attitudes did not moderate these effects. However, the trajectories of motivation and self-regulation differed by condition for low-achieving students. While these trajectories decreased over time amongst low-achieving students in the Jigsaw method condition and in the weakly structured cooperation condition, they were stable in the business-as-usual learning condition. These results provide a new perspective since they seem to question the implementation conditions of the Jigsaw method for low-achieving students.

尽管有关拼图教学法的有益影响的文章已经很多,但对其如何影响学生的学习动机和自我调节过程却知之甚少。在本研究中,我们测试了拼图法对学生自主数学学习动机和学业自律的影响。我们还研究了这些影响是否会受到学生合作态度和初始数学成绩水平的调节。来自法国职业高中的 4,698 名学生参加了为期两年的研究。他们被分为三组:1,641 人被分配到采用拼图法的合作学习条件下,1,602 人被分配到结构薄弱的合作学习条件下,1,455 人被分配到一切照旧的学习条件下。在研究期间,对学生的数学学习动机、学业自我调节和合作态度进行了三次自我报告。总体而言,多层次成长模型的结果表明,学生的学习动机和自我调节能力普遍下降,而学生报告的合作态度并没有缓和这些影响。然而,对于成绩较差的学生来说,学习动机和自我调节能力的发展轨迹因条件而异。在拼图学习法条件和弱结构合作条件下,学习成绩差的学生的学习动机和自我调节能力随时间推移而下降,而在 "一切照旧 "学习条件下,学习动机和自我调节能力则保持稳定。这些结果提供了一个新的视角,因为它们似乎对拼图学习法在成绩差的学生中的实施条件提出了质疑。
{"title":"Does the Jigsaw method improve motivation and self-regulation in vocational high schools?","authors":"Mathilde Riant ,&nbsp;Anne-Laure de Place ,&nbsp;Pascal Bressoux ,&nbsp;Anatolia Batruch ,&nbsp;Marinette Bouet ,&nbsp;Marco Bressan ,&nbsp;Genavee Brown ,&nbsp;Fabrizio Butera ,&nbsp;Carlos Cepeda ,&nbsp;Anthony Cherbonnier ,&nbsp;Céline Darnon ,&nbsp;Marie Demolliens ,&nbsp;Olivier Desrichard ,&nbsp;Théo Ducros ,&nbsp;Luc Goron ,&nbsp;Brivael Hémon ,&nbsp;Pascal Huguet ,&nbsp;Eric Jamet ,&nbsp;Ruben Martinez ,&nbsp;Vincent Mazenod ,&nbsp;Pascal Pansu","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102278","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102278","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Although much has been written about the beneficial effects of the Jigsaw method, little is known about how it affects students' motivation and self-regulation processes. In this study, we tested its effects on students' trajectories of autonomous mathematics motivation and academic self-regulation. We also examined whether these effects could be moderated by the students’ cooperative attitudes and initial mathematics achievement level. 4,698 students from French vocational high schools participated in the study over two years. They were divided into three groups: 1,641 were assigned to a cooperative learning condition with the Jigsaw method, 1,602 to a weakly structured cooperative learning condition, and 1,455 to a business-as-usual learning condition. Self-reported mathematics motivation, academic self-regulation, and cooperative attitudes were collected three times during the study. Overall, the multilevel growth model results indicate a general decline in students’ motivation and self-regulation, and student-reported cooperative attitudes did not moderate these effects. However, the trajectories of motivation and self-regulation differed by condition for low-achieving students. While these trajectories decreased over time amongst low-achieving students in the Jigsaw method condition and in the weakly structured cooperation condition, they were stable in the business-as-usual learning condition. These results provide a new perspective since they seem to question the implementation conditions of the Jigsaw method for low-achieving students.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140917752","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“My drawing is quite different!” Drawbacks of comparing generative drawings to instructional visuals "我的画与众不同将生成性绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较的缺点
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-05-01 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102277
Logan Fiorella , Allison J. Jaeger , Alexis Capobianco , Anna Burnett

This study tested how prompting learners to compare their drawings to instructional visuals affects their perceived and actual performance. Undergraduates (n = 116) created two drawings while studying a text on the human circulatory system. Then they made a series of retrospective and prospective judgments of their drawing performance and prospective judgments of their comprehension. In a subsequent restudy phase, students were randomly assigned to either compare their drawings to instructional visuals (compare group; n = 56) or to restudy the text and review their drawings without receiving instructional visuals (control group; n = 60), followed by a series of new judgments of drawing and comprehension. All students then completed drawing and comprehension post-tests. Results indicated that comparing one’s drawings to instructional visuals caused students to become underconfident in the quality of their drawings (lower retrospective accuracy) and overconfident in their future drawing performance (lower prospective accuracy). Exploratory analyses indicated that the compare group tended to make surface-level (rather than conceptual) comparisons when processing the provided visuals, such as attending to the aesthetic style or conventions used in the instructional visuals. Furthermore, despite a strong link between drawing and comprehension performance, comparing drawings to instructional visuals did not significantly affect students’ judgments of comprehension. These findings highlight potential drawbacks of comparing generative drawings to instructional visuals in learning by drawing.

本研究测试了促使学习者将自己的绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较会如何影响他们的感知和实际表现。本科生(n = 116)在学习一篇关于人体循环系统的文章时创作了两幅图画。然后,他们对自己的绘画表现进行了一系列回顾性和前瞻性判断,并对自己的理解能力进行了前瞻性判断。在随后的复习阶段,学生被随机分配到将他们的绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较(比较组;n = 56),或重新学习课文并在不接受教学视觉效果的情况下复习他们的绘画(对照组;n = 60),然后对绘画和理解能力进行一系列新的判断。然后,所有学生都完成了绘画和理解能力的后测。结果表明,将自己的绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较会导致学生对自己的绘画质量不自信(降低了回顾的准确性),并对自己未来的绘画表现过于自信(降低了展望的准确性)。探索性分析表明,比较组在处理所提供的视觉图像时倾向于进行表面(而非概念)比较,如关注教学视觉图像中使用的美学风格或惯例。此外,尽管绘画与理解能力之间存在密切联系,但将绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较并不会对学生的理解能力判断产生显著影响。这些发现凸显了在绘画学习中将生成性绘画与教学视觉效果进行比较的潜在缺陷。
{"title":"“My drawing is quite different!” Drawbacks of comparing generative drawings to instructional visuals","authors":"Logan Fiorella ,&nbsp;Allison J. Jaeger ,&nbsp;Alexis Capobianco ,&nbsp;Anna Burnett","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102277","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102277","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>This study tested how prompting learners to compare their drawings to instructional visuals affects their perceived and actual performance. Undergraduates (<em>n</em> = 116) created two drawings while studying a text on the human circulatory system. Then they made a series of retrospective and prospective judgments of their drawing performance and prospective judgments of their comprehension. In a subsequent restudy phase, students were randomly assigned to either compare their drawings to instructional visuals (compare group; <em>n</em> = 56) or to restudy the text and review their drawings without receiving instructional visuals (control group; <em>n</em> = 60), followed by a series of new judgments of drawing and comprehension. All students then completed drawing and comprehension post-tests. Results indicated that comparing one’s drawings to instructional visuals caused students to become underconfident in the quality of their drawings (lower retrospective accuracy) and overconfident in their future drawing performance (lower prospective accuracy). Exploratory analyses indicated that the compare group tended to make surface-level (rather than conceptual) comparisons when processing the provided visuals, such as attending to the aesthetic style or conventions used in the instructional visuals. Furthermore, despite a strong link between drawing and comprehension performance, comparing drawings to instructional visuals did not significantly affect students’ judgments of comprehension. These findings highlight potential drawbacks of comparing generative drawings to instructional visuals in learning by drawing.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140822283","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between teachers’ stress and buoyancy from day to day: Two daily diary studies 教师每天的压力与浮力之间的关系:两项每日日记研究
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102276
Joost Jansen in de Wal , Thijmen van Alphen , Jaap Schuitema , Lucija Andre , Joran Jongerling , Thea Peetsma

Despite the pertinence of teachers’ buoyancy to ‘everyday work’, existing studies do not investigate buoyancy in close proximity to everyday experiences. Nor does existing research longitudinally investigate teachers’ buoyancy and how it relates to stress. This paper describes two quantitative daily diary studies on this relationship. Study 1 includes a relatively large sample of teachers (N = 151), compared to the number of days that they were followed (T = 15). Study 2 includes relatively few teachers (N = 10), but follows them for an extended period of time (T = 61). Both studies tested hypotheses regarding the extent to which teachers’ stress and buoyancy beliefs vary − and carry over − from day to day and the extent to which teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress experiences co-occur and predict each other from day to day and from teacher to teacher. Results showed that both teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress experiences varied and carried over significantly from day to day, although carryover effects were small. The relationship between buoyancy beliefs and stress was negative from teacher to teacher and concurrently from day to day. However, cross-lagged effects between both constructs from day to day were not significant. These results imply that both teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress are malleable, state-like constructs to a considerable extent, but their dynamics likely occur on a timescale that is smaller than daily.

尽管教师的浮力与 "日常工作 "密切相关,但现有的研究并没有对浮力与日常经 历的密切关系进行调查。现有研究也没有纵向调查教师的浮力及其与压力的关系。本文介绍了关于这种关系的两项每日日记定量研究。研究 1 包括了相对较多的教师样本(N = 151),而对他们进行跟踪调查的天数(T = 15)则相对较少。研究 2 的教师人数相对较少(N = 10),但对他们进行了较长时间的跟踪调查(T = 61)。这两项研究都检验了以下假设:教师的压力和浮力信念在多大程度上每天都会发生变化和延续,以及教师的浮力信念和压力体验在多大程度上每天都会同时出现并相互预测。结果表明,教师的浮力信念和压力体验每天都有显著的变化和延续,尽管延续效应很小。浮力信念与压力之间的关系在教师与教师之间是负相关的,在一天与一天之间也是同时存在的。然而,这两个构念之间每天的交叉滞后效应并不显著。这些结果表明,教师的浮力信念和压力在很大程度上都是可塑的、类似于状态的建构,但其动态变化的时间尺度可能小于每天。
{"title":"The relationship between teachers’ stress and buoyancy from day to day: Two daily diary studies","authors":"Joost Jansen in de Wal ,&nbsp;Thijmen van Alphen ,&nbsp;Jaap Schuitema ,&nbsp;Lucija Andre ,&nbsp;Joran Jongerling ,&nbsp;Thea Peetsma","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102276","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102276","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Despite the pertinence of teachers’ buoyancy to ‘everyday work’, existing studies do not investigate buoyancy in close proximity to everyday experiences. Nor does existing research longitudinally investigate teachers’ buoyancy and how it relates to stress. This paper describes two quantitative daily diary studies on this relationship. Study 1 includes a relatively large sample of teachers (N = 151), compared to the number of days that they were followed (T = 15). Study 2 includes relatively few teachers (N = 10), but follows them for an extended period of time (T = 61). Both studies tested hypotheses regarding the extent to which teachers’ stress and buoyancy beliefs vary − and carry over − from day to day and the extent to which teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress experiences co-occur and predict each other from day to day and from teacher to teacher. Results showed that both teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress experiences varied and carried over significantly from day to day, although carryover effects were small. The relationship between buoyancy beliefs and stress was negative from teacher to teacher and concurrently from day to day. However, cross-lagged effects between both constructs from day to day were not significant. These results imply that both teachers’ buoyancy beliefs and stress are malleable, state-like constructs to a considerable extent, but their dynamics likely occur on a timescale that is smaller than daily.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0361476X24000213/pdfft?md5=bff0ffa0f910d20eb78ed786e583ae5d&pid=1-s2.0-S0361476X24000213-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140558769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How trait and state positive Emotions, negative Emotions, and self-regulation relate to adolescents' perceived daily learning progress 积极情绪、消极情绪和自我调节的特质和状态如何与青少年感知到的日常学习进步相关联
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-05 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102275
Gaoxia Zhu , Juan Zheng , Kaylin Ratner , Qingyi Li , Melody Estevez , Anthony L. Burrow

Previous research is replete with evidence that emotions and self-regulation work together to influence learning performance, but distinct trait and state features of emotions and self-regulation are rarely considered. With an analytic sample comprising 9,501 daily diaries from 280 adolescents participating in a self-driven learning program, this study used multilevel modeling to examine how trait and state positive and negative emotions and self-regulation interact to predict adolescents' perceived daily learning progress. Results suggested that daily perceived learning progress was associated with trait and state positive emotions and self-regulation, as well as trait negative emotions. Furthermore, there was a significant positive interaction between state positive emotions and state self-regulation on perceived daily learning progress, such that when adolescents' state self-regulation was higher than usual, their perceived daily learning progress was more sensitive to state positive emotion. Results underscore the importance of enhancing adolescents' self-regulation and positive emotion, and the feasibility of facilitating adolescents' learning even if they are in a state of greater negative emotion.

以往的研究有大量证据表明,情绪和自我调节共同影响学习成绩,但情绪和自我调节的不同特质和状态特征却很少被考虑在内。本研究使用多层次模型分析了280名参加自我驱动学习计划的青少年的9501篇每日日记,研究了特质和状态的积极和消极情绪与自我调节如何相互作用,以预测青少年感知到的每日学习进度。结果表明,每日感知到的学习进步与特质和状态积极情绪、自我调节以及特质消极情绪有关。此外,状态积极情绪和状态自我调节对日常学习进步的感知有显著的正向交互作用,即当青少年的状态自我调节比平时高时,他们的日常学习进步感知对状态积极情绪更敏感。研究结果强调了加强青少年自我调节和积极情绪的重要性,以及即使青少年处于较强的消极情绪状态下也能促进其学习的可行性。
{"title":"How trait and state positive Emotions, negative Emotions, and self-regulation relate to adolescents' perceived daily learning progress","authors":"Gaoxia Zhu ,&nbsp;Juan Zheng ,&nbsp;Kaylin Ratner ,&nbsp;Qingyi Li ,&nbsp;Melody Estevez ,&nbsp;Anthony L. Burrow","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102275","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102275","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Previous research is replete with evidence that emotions and self-regulation work together to influence learning performance, but distinct trait and state features of emotions and self-regulation are rarely considered. With an analytic sample comprising 9,501 daily diaries from 280 adolescents participating in a self-driven learning program, this study used multilevel modeling to examine how trait and state positive and negative emotions and self-regulation interact to predict adolescents' perceived daily learning progress. Results suggested that daily perceived learning progress was associated with trait and state positive emotions and self-regulation, as well as trait negative emotions. Furthermore, there was a significant positive interaction between state positive emotions and state self-regulation on perceived daily learning progress, such that when adolescents' state self-regulation was higher than usual, their perceived daily learning progress was more sensitive to state positive emotion. Results underscore the importance of enhancing adolescents' self-regulation and positive emotion, and the feasibility of facilitating adolescents' learning even if they are in a state of greater negative emotion.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140549413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Multidimensional Student Well-being (MSW) instrument: Conceptualisation, measurement, and differences between Indigenous and non-Indigenous primary and secondary students 多维学生福祉(MSW)工具:土著和非土著中小学生的概念、测量和差异
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102274
Rhonda G. Craven , Herbert W. Marsh , Alexander S. Yeung , Diego Vasconcellos , Anthony Dillon , Richard M. Ryan , Janet Mooney , Alicia Franklin , Lily Barclay , Annalies van Westenbrugge

Enabling children’s and youth’s well-being is widely valued by families and communities worldwide. However, there is no general agreement about the structure and measurement of well-being in schooling contexts, nor in particular for Indigenous students who comprise some of the most educationally disadvantaged populations in the world. We theorised a multidimensional student well-being model and the Multidimensional Student Well-being (MSW) instrument, grounded on recent research. We investigated its structure, measurement, and relation to correlates of well-being for a matched sample of 1,405 Australian students (Indigenous, N = 764; non-Indigenous, N = 641) at three time-points, 10–12 months apart. Analyses supported an a priori multidimensional model of 6 higher-order domains of well-being, represented by 15 first-order factors. This structure was invariant across Indigenous and non-Indigenous, male and female, and primary and secondary schooling levels. Correlates provided support for convergent and discriminant validity. There was a downward trend in well-being over time, which calls for attention to multidimensional domains of students’ well-being to promote healthy development throughout school life and beyond. The results support a multidimensional model of student well-being appropriate for primary and secondary schooling and both Indigenous and non-Indigenous students.

提高儿童和青少年的幸福感受到全世界家庭和社区的广泛重视。然而,对于学校教育背景下的幸福感结构和衡量标准,尤其是对于构成世界上教育最弱势人群的土著学生而言,还没有达成普遍共识。我们以最新研究为基础,理论化了多维学生幸福感模型和多维学生幸福感(MSW)工具。我们在相隔 10-12 个月的三个时间点,对 1,405 名澳大利亚学生(土著,764 人;非土著,641 人)的匹配样本进行了调查,研究了其结构、测量方法以及与幸福感相关因素的关系。分析支持一个由 6 个高阶幸福感领域组成的先验多维模型,该模型由 15 个一阶因子代表。这一结构在土著和非土著、男性和女性、小学和中学教育水平之间是不变的。相关因素为收敛性和区分性有效性提供了支持。随着时间的推移,幸福感呈下降趋势,这就要求关注学生幸福感的多维领域,以促进学生在整个学校生活及以后的健康发展。研究结果支持适合中小学教育以及土著和非土著学生的学生幸福感多维模型。
{"title":"The Multidimensional Student Well-being (MSW) instrument: Conceptualisation, measurement, and differences between Indigenous and non-Indigenous primary and secondary students","authors":"Rhonda G. Craven ,&nbsp;Herbert W. Marsh ,&nbsp;Alexander S. Yeung ,&nbsp;Diego Vasconcellos ,&nbsp;Anthony Dillon ,&nbsp;Richard M. Ryan ,&nbsp;Janet Mooney ,&nbsp;Alicia Franklin ,&nbsp;Lily Barclay ,&nbsp;Annalies van Westenbrugge","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102274","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102274","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Enabling children’s and youth’s well-being is widely valued by families and communities worldwide. However, there is no general agreement about the structure and measurement of well-being in schooling contexts, nor in particular for Indigenous students who comprise some of the most educationally disadvantaged populations in the world. We theorised a multidimensional student well-being model and the Multidimensional Student Well-being (MSW) instrument, grounded on recent research. We investigated its structure, measurement, and relation to correlates of well-being for a matched sample of 1,405 Australian students (Indigenous, N = 764; non-Indigenous, N = 641) at three time-points, 10–12 months apart. Analyses supported an a priori multidimensional model of 6 higher-order domains of well-being, represented by 15 first-order factors. This structure was invariant across Indigenous and non-Indigenous, male and female, and primary and secondary schooling levels. Correlates provided support for convergent and discriminant validity. There was a downward trend in well-being over time, which calls for attention to multidimensional domains of students’ well-being to promote healthy development throughout school life and beyond. The results support a multidimensional model of student well-being appropriate for primary and secondary schooling and both Indigenous and non-Indigenous students.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0361476X24000195/pdfft?md5=88d81a8c95a2fffc092dda5dd079365f&pid=1-s2.0-S0361476X24000195-main.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140549414","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Test anxiety fluctuations during low-stakes secondary school assessments: The role of the needs for autonomy and competence over and above the number of tests 中学低风险评估中的考试焦虑波动:自主性和能力需求在考试次数之外的作用
IF 10.3 1区 心理学 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102273
Stefanie De Jonge , Evelien Opdecam , Leen Haerens

Test anxiety poses a fundamental educational challenge as it is associated with lower academic performance and well-being. Grounded in the Self-Determination Theory, this study will focus on test anxiety fluctuations in relation to low-stakes assessments and investigates whether fluctuations in students’ experiences of autonomy and competence satisfaction and frustration relate to their test anxiety. For this purpose, 253 secondary school students completed a survey at three different times throughout the second semester. Students' feelings of autonomy and competence in the classroom were administered as well as their test anxiety. Each student completed the same two test anxiety scales at each measurement occasion, with one scale consistently administered to all students and the other two scales randomly assigned between classes. Multilevel analyses revealed that students showed higher test anxiety in weeks in which their need for competence was more frustrated and when they had to take more low-stakes tests. This association was robust across the three test anxiety instruments and after considering important test anxiety covariates (e.g., gender and prior achievement). These findings imply that competence frustration is an important underlying mechanism of test anxiety that should be taken into account when designing anxiety-reducing interventions.

考试焦虑与学业成绩和幸福感的降低有关,因此对教育构成了根本性的挑战。本研究以 "自我决定理论"(Self-Determination Theory)为基础,重点关注与低分数评估相关的考试焦虑波动,并调查学生在自主性和能力方面的满意度和挫折感的波动是否与他们的考试焦虑有关。为此,253 名中学生在整个第二学期的三个不同时间完成了一项调查。调查内容包括学生在课堂上的自主感和能力感,以及他们的考试焦虑。每个学生在每次测量时都填写了相同的两个考试焦虑量表,其中一个量表对所有学生统一使用,另外两个量表则在班级之间随机分配。多层次分析表明,当学生对能力的需求更受挫、需要参加更多低分数测试时,他们的考试焦虑就会更高。这种关联在三种考试焦虑工具中都是稳健的,而且在考虑了重要的考试焦虑协变量(如性别和以前的成绩)之后也是稳健的。这些研究结果表明,能力受挫是考试焦虑的一个重要潜在机制,在设计减轻焦虑的干预措施时应加以考虑。
{"title":"Test anxiety fluctuations during low-stakes secondary school assessments: The role of the needs for autonomy and competence over and above the number of tests","authors":"Stefanie De Jonge ,&nbsp;Evelien Opdecam ,&nbsp;Leen Haerens","doi":"10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102273","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2024.102273","url":null,"abstract":"<div><p>Test anxiety poses a fundamental educational challenge as it is associated with lower academic performance and well-being. Grounded in the Self-Determination Theory, this study will focus on test anxiety fluctuations in relation to low-stakes assessments and investigates whether fluctuations in students’ experiences of autonomy and competence satisfaction and frustration relate to their test anxiety. For this purpose, 253 secondary school students completed a survey at three different times throughout the second semester. Students' feelings of autonomy and competence in the classroom were administered as well as their test anxiety. Each student completed the same two test anxiety scales at each measurement occasion, with one scale consistently administered to all students and the other two scales randomly assigned between classes. Multilevel analyses revealed that students showed higher test anxiety in weeks in which their need for competence was more frustrated and when they had to take more low-stakes tests. This association was robust across the three test anxiety instruments and after considering important test anxiety covariates (e.g., gender and prior achievement). These findings imply that competence frustration is an important underlying mechanism of test anxiety that should be taken into account when designing anxiety-reducing interventions.</p></div>","PeriodicalId":10635,"journal":{"name":"Contemporary Educational Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":10.3,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140327937","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Contemporary Educational Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1