首页 > 最新文献

Glossa Psycholinguistics最新文献

英文 中文
The role of prosodic focus in the reanalysis of garden path sentences: Depth of semantic processing impedes the revision of an erroneous local analysis 韵律焦点在花园小径句再分析中的作用:语义加工深度阻碍了错误局部分析的修正
Pub Date : 2022-09-20 DOI: 10.5070/g601136
Jun Choe, Masaya Yoshida, Jennifer Cole
Research on post-repair representations of garden path sentences has found that readers systematically arrive at misinterpretations even after displaying evidence of reanalysis (Christianson et al., 2001; Ferreira et al., 2001). These comprehension errors have been attributed to the semantic interpretation associated with the incorrect parse persisting past disambiguation, but less is known about the mechanism driving this phenomenon (Sturt, 2007; Slattery et al., 2013). A speeded auditory comprehension experiment examined the depth of semantic processing as an independent influence on the strength of semantic persistence, drawing on known effects of pitch accent on the processing of focus-related semantic meaning (Fraundorf et al., 2010). Participants heard garden path sentences with early/late-closure ambiguity (e.g., While Anna dressed the baby stopped crying) with a sharply rising pitch accent on either the unambiguous adjunct subject or the ambiguously transitive adjunct verb, followed by a comprehension question that probed whether the incorrect late-closure analysis persisted. Since the pitch accent is often a strong cue for semantic focus when it occurs in prosodically marked phrase-medial positions, we reasoned that a deeper semantic processing would be facilitated for the late-closure analysis only when the verb receives a pitch accent. Findings indicate that a pitch accent on the verb significantly decreased accuracy without a corresponding increase on response time, suggesting that a deeper semantic processing of the erroneous parse can strengthen its resistance to revision without necessarily interfering with the process of structure-building.
对花园小径句子的修复后表征的研究发现,即使在展示了重新分析的证据后,读者也会系统地到达误解(Christianson et al., 2001;Ferreira et al., 2001)。这些理解错误归因于与错误解析持续过去消歧相关的语义解释,但对驱动这种现象的机制知之甚少(Sturt, 2007;Slattery等人,2013)。一项快速听觉理解实验利用音高重音对焦点相关语义加工的已知影响,检验了语义加工深度对语义持久性强度的独立影响(Fraundorf et al., 2010)。参与者听到的花园小径句子具有早/晚结束的模糊性(例如,当安娜给婴儿穿衣服时,婴儿停止了哭泣),在明确的主语或含糊的及物动词上,音调急剧上升,然后是一个理解问题,探究错误的晚结束分析是否持续存在。由于重音出现在韵律标记的短语-中间位置时,往往是语义聚焦的强烈线索,因此我们推断,只有当动词出现重音时,才有利于后期结束分析的更深层次的语义处理。研究结果表明,动词的音高重音显著降低了准确性,但没有相应的增加响应时间,这表明对错误解析进行更深层次的语义处理可以增强其对修改的抵抗力,而不一定会干扰结构构建过程。
{"title":"The role of prosodic focus in the reanalysis of garden path sentences: Depth of semantic processing impedes the revision of an erroneous local analysis","authors":"Jun Choe, Masaya Yoshida, Jennifer Cole","doi":"10.5070/g601136","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601136","url":null,"abstract":"Research on post-repair representations of garden path sentences has found that readers systematically arrive at misinterpretations even after displaying evidence of reanalysis (Christianson et al., 2001; Ferreira et al., 2001). These comprehension errors have been attributed to the semantic interpretation associated with the incorrect parse persisting past disambiguation, but less is known about the mechanism driving this phenomenon (Sturt, 2007; Slattery et al., 2013). A speeded auditory comprehension experiment examined the depth of semantic processing as an independent influence on the strength of semantic persistence, drawing on known effects of pitch accent on the processing of focus-related semantic meaning (Fraundorf et al., 2010). Participants heard garden path sentences with early/late-closure ambiguity (e.g., While Anna dressed the baby stopped crying) with a sharply rising pitch accent on either the unambiguous adjunct subject or the ambiguously transitive adjunct verb, followed by a comprehension question that probed whether the incorrect late-closure analysis persisted. Since the pitch accent is often a strong cue for semantic focus when it occurs in prosodically marked phrase-medial positions, we reasoned that a deeper semantic processing would be facilitated for the late-closure analysis only when the verb receives a pitch accent. Findings indicate that a pitch accent on the verb significantly decreased accuracy without a corresponding increase on response time, suggesting that a deeper semantic processing of the erroneous parse can strengthen its resistance to revision without necessarily interfering with the process of structure-building.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"259 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"132911948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prediction in the maze: Evidence for probabilistic pre-activation from the English a/an contrast 迷宫中的预测:来自英语a/an对比的概率预激活证据
Pub Date : 2022-09-19 DOI: 10.5070/g601153
E. M. Husband
The idea that comprehenders predict upcoming linguistic content has become core to many theories of language processing. Experimental studies exploiting morphosyntactic and phonotactic constraints on a word form preceding a high cloze target word have been key to underpinning predictive accounts of comprehension, but investigating these tight sequential contrasts with traditional behavioral methods is difficult. The maze task, with its more focal measure of incremental processing, may provide a cheap and easy methodology to study early cues to prediction. An experiment investigating the a/an contrast (DeLong, Urbach, & Kutas, 2005; Nieuwland, et al., 2018) using A-maze (Boyce, Futrell, & Levy, 2020) finds that unexpected articles, as well as nouns, elicit slower focal response times. Response times are also shown to be inversely related to noun cloze probabilities, with slower responders showing larger effects of expectation. This study demonstrates that the maze task can be sensitive to expectation and is a useful alternative methodology for investigating prediction in comprehension.
理解者预测即将到来的语言内容的观点已经成为许多语言处理理论的核心。利用高完形目标词前词形的形态语法和语音语法约束的实验研究是支持理解预测的关键,但用传统的行为方法研究这些紧密的顺序对比是困难的。迷宫任务,以其更集中的增量处理测量,可能提供一种廉价和简单的方法来研究预测的早期线索。研究a/ An对比的实验(DeLong, Urbach, & Kutas, 2005;Nieuwland等人,2018)使用A-maze (Boyce, Futrell, & Levy, 2020)发现,意外的文章和名词会引发较慢的焦点响应时间。反应时间也被证明与名词完形概率成反比,反应较慢的人表现出更大的期望影响。本研究表明,迷宫任务对预期是敏感的,是研究理解预测的一种有用的替代方法。
{"title":"Prediction in the maze: Evidence for probabilistic pre-activation from the English a/an contrast","authors":"E. M. Husband","doi":"10.5070/g601153","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601153","url":null,"abstract":"The idea that comprehenders predict upcoming linguistic content has become core to many theories of language processing. Experimental studies exploiting morphosyntactic and phonotactic constraints on a word form preceding a high cloze target word have been key to underpinning predictive accounts of comprehension, but investigating these tight sequential contrasts with traditional behavioral methods is difficult. The maze task, with its more focal measure of incremental processing, may provide a cheap and easy methodology to study early cues to prediction. An experiment investigating the a/an contrast (DeLong, Urbach, & Kutas, 2005; Nieuwland, et al., 2018) using A-maze (Boyce, Futrell, & Levy, 2020) finds that unexpected articles, as well as nouns, elicit slower focal response times. Response times are also shown to be inversely related to noun cloze probabilities, with slower responders showing larger effects of expectation. This study demonstrates that the maze task can be sensitive to expectation and is a useful alternative methodology for investigating prediction in comprehension.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"37 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114976168","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Complexity vs. salience of alternatives in implicature: A cross-linguistic investigation 复杂性与意义选择的显著性:一项跨语言研究
Pub Date : 2022-08-29 DOI: 10.5070/g601190
D. Dionne, E. Coppock
Scalar implicature depends on the activation of alternatives. For instance, in English, finger implicates 'not thumb', suggesting that thumb is an activated alternative. Is this because it is more specific (Quantity) and equally short (Manner)? Indeed, toe doesn't imply 'not big toe', perhaps because big toe is longer. As L. Horn points out, this Quantity/Manner explanation predicts that if English had the simplex Latin word pollex meaning 'thumb or big toe', then the asymmetry would disappear. But would it suffice for that word to exist in the language, or would the word also have to be sufficiently salient? We explore this question in four languages that are sometimes said to lack a single-word alternative for thumb: Spanish (which does have pulgar 'thumb or big toe' (< pollex), though it is a non-colloquial form), Russian, Persian, and Arabic. To gauge the salience of various ways of describing digits, we use a fill-in-the-blank production task. We then measure the availability of implicatures using a forced choice comprehension task. We find cross-linguistic differences in implicature, and moreover that implicature calculation tracks production probabilities more closely than structural complexity of the alternatives. A comparison between two Rational Speech Act models—one in which the speaker replicates our production data and a standard one in which the speaker chooses based on a standard cost/accuracy trade-off—shows that comprehension is more closely tied to production probability than to the complexity of alternatives. 
标量含义取决于备选项的激活。例如,在英语中,finger暗示着“不是大拇指”,这表明大拇指是一个激活的替代词。这是因为它更具体(数量)和同样简短(方式)吗?事实上,“脚趾”并不意味着“不是大脚趾”,也许是因为“大脚趾”更长。正如L. Horn所指出的那样,这种数量/方式的解释预示着,如果英语中有一个单一的拉丁单词pollex,意思是“拇指或大脚趾”,那么这种不对称性就会消失。但是,这个词在语言中存在就足够了吗,或者这个词也必须足够突出吗?我们在四种语言中探讨了这个问题,这四种语言有时被认为缺乏大拇指的单一单词替代:西班牙语(西班牙语中有pulgar“拇指或大脚趾”(< pollex),尽管它是非口语形式)、俄语、波斯语和阿拉伯语。为了衡量描述数字的各种方式的显著性,我们使用填空生产任务。然后,我们使用强制选择理解任务来测量隐含的可用性。我们发现含意的跨语言差异,而且含意计算比替代方案的结构复杂性更密切地跟踪生产概率。两个Rational Speech Act模型之间的比较——一个是讲话者复制我们的生产数据,另一个是讲话者根据标准成本/准确性权衡选择的标准模型——表明理解与生产概率的关系比选择的复杂性更紧密。
{"title":"Complexity vs. salience of alternatives in implicature: A cross-linguistic investigation","authors":"D. Dionne, E. Coppock","doi":"10.5070/g601190","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601190","url":null,"abstract":"Scalar implicature depends on the activation of alternatives. For instance, in English, finger implicates 'not thumb', suggesting that thumb is an activated alternative. Is this because it is more specific (Quantity) and equally short (Manner)? Indeed, toe doesn't imply 'not big toe', perhaps because big toe is longer. As L. Horn points out, this Quantity/Manner explanation predicts that if English had the simplex Latin word pollex meaning 'thumb or big toe', then the asymmetry would disappear. But would it suffice for that word to exist in the language, or would the word also have to be sufficiently salient? We explore this question in four languages that are sometimes said to lack a single-word alternative for thumb: Spanish (which does have pulgar 'thumb or big toe' (< pollex), though it is a non-colloquial form), Russian, Persian, and Arabic. To gauge the salience of various ways of describing digits, we use a fill-in-the-blank production task. We then measure the availability of implicatures using a forced choice comprehension task. We find cross-linguistic differences in implicature, and moreover that implicature calculation tracks production probabilities more closely than structural complexity of the alternatives. A comparison between two Rational Speech Act models—one in which the speaker replicates our production data and a standard one in which the speaker chooses based on a standard cost/accuracy trade-off—shows that comprehension is more closely tied to production probability than to the complexity of alternatives. ","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"3 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-08-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128435984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing infrared and webcam eye tracking in the Visual World Paradigm 比较红外和网络摄像头眼动追踪在视觉世界范式
Pub Date : 2022-08-15 DOI: 10.5070/g6011131
Myrte Vos, Serge Minor, G. Ramchand
Visual World eye tracking is a temporally fine-grained method of monitoring attention, making it a popular tool in the study of online sentence processing. Recently, while infrared eye tracking was mostly unavailable, various web-based experiment platforms have rapidly developed webcam eye tracking functionalities, which are now in urgent need of testing and evaluation. We replicated a recent Visual World study on the incremental processing of verb aspect in English using ‘out of the box’ webcam eye tracking software (jsPsych; de Leeuw, 2015) and crowdsourced participants, and fully replicated both the offline and online results of the original study. We furthermore discuss factors influencing the quality and interpretability of webcam eye tracking data, particularly with regards to temporal and spatial resolution; and conclude that remote webcam eye tracking can serve as an affordable and accessible alternative to lab-based infrared eye tracking, even for questions probing the time-course of language processing.
视觉世界眼动追踪是一种时间上细粒度的注意力监测方法,使其成为研究在线句子处理的流行工具。近年来,在红外眼动追踪基本无法实现的情况下,各种基于网络的实验平台迅速开发出了网络摄像头眼动追踪功能,迫切需要对其进行测试和评估。我们复制了Visual World最近的一项研究,使用“开箱即用”的网络摄像头眼动追踪软件(jsPsych;de Leeuw, 2015)和众包参与者,并充分复制了原始研究的离线和在线结果。我们进一步讨论了影响网络摄像头眼动追踪数据质量和可解释性的因素,特别是在时间和空间分辨率方面;并得出结论,远程网络摄像头眼动追踪可以作为一种负担得起的、可获得的替代实验室红外眼动追踪的方法,即使对于探索语言处理的时间过程的问题也是如此。
{"title":"Comparing infrared and webcam eye tracking in the Visual World Paradigm","authors":"Myrte Vos, Serge Minor, G. Ramchand","doi":"10.5070/g6011131","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g6011131","url":null,"abstract":"Visual World eye tracking is a temporally fine-grained method of monitoring attention, making it a popular tool in the study of online sentence processing. Recently, while infrared eye tracking was mostly unavailable, various web-based experiment platforms have rapidly developed webcam eye tracking functionalities, which are now in urgent need of testing and evaluation. We replicated a recent Visual World study on the incremental processing of verb aspect in English using ‘out of the box’ webcam eye tracking software (jsPsych; de Leeuw, 2015) and crowdsourced participants, and fully replicated both the offline and online results of the original study. We furthermore discuss factors influencing the quality and interpretability of webcam eye tracking data, particularly with regards to temporal and spatial resolution; and conclude that remote webcam eye tracking can serve as an affordable and accessible alternative to lab-based infrared eye tracking, even for questions probing the time-course of language processing.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-08-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130701398","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Why bother? What our eyes tell about psych verb (non) causative constructions 何苦呢?我们的眼睛告诉我们什么心理动词(非)使役结构
Pub Date : 2022-07-26 DOI: 10.5070/g601138
C. Gattei, Federico Alvarez, Luis París, A. Wainselboim, Y. Sevilla, Diego E. Shalom
We present an eyetracking study that investigates how linking is achieved during real-time comprehension of Spanish sentences with causative psych verbs and alternative case marking. This group of verbs lead to verbs’ argument structures that require direct or inverse syntax-to-semantics linking according to the type of case marking assigned to their object. The study aimed at disentangling whether processing inverse linking was more costly than direct linking, and exploring how incremental argument interpretation takes place when lexemes that accept several case markings are used. Results showed that during incremental comprehension, inverse linking is more difficult than direct linking, irrespective of word order. As for argument interpretation, the current study partially replicated the results of previous studies conducted in this language using different verb types. Findings are discussed under the light of different psycholinguistic models addressing case marking processing and incremental linking.
我们提出了一项眼球追踪研究,调查了在实时理解带有使役心理动词和替代格标记的西班牙语句子时如何实现连接。这组动词导致动词的参数结构,这些参数结构需要根据分配给其对象的大小写标记类型进行直接或反向的语法到语义链接。该研究旨在解开处理反向链接是否比直接链接更昂贵的问题,并探索当使用接受多个大小写标记的词汇时,增量参数解释是如何发生的。结果表明,在增量理解过程中,无论词序如何,反连读都比直接连读困难。在论证解释方面,本研究部分重复了以往使用不同动词类型在该语言中进行的研究的结果。研究结果在不同的心理语言学模型下讨论了案例标记处理和增量连接。
{"title":"Why bother? What our eyes tell about psych verb (non) causative constructions","authors":"C. Gattei, Federico Alvarez, Luis París, A. Wainselboim, Y. Sevilla, Diego E. Shalom","doi":"10.5070/g601138","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601138","url":null,"abstract":"We present an eyetracking study that investigates how linking is achieved during real-time comprehension of Spanish sentences with causative psych verbs and alternative case marking. This group of verbs lead to verbs’ argument structures that require direct or inverse syntax-to-semantics linking according to the type of case marking assigned to their object. The study aimed at disentangling whether processing inverse linking was more costly than direct linking, and exploring how incremental argument interpretation takes place when lexemes that accept several case markings are used. Results showed that during incremental comprehension, inverse linking is more difficult than direct linking, irrespective of word order. As for argument interpretation, the current study partially replicated the results of previous studies conducted in this language using different verb types. Findings are discussed under the light of different psycholinguistic models addressing case marking processing and incremental linking.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"25 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114808964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
(How) Visual properties affect the perception and description of transitive events 视觉属性如何影响及物事件的感知和描述
Pub Date : 2022-07-06 DOI: 10.5070/g601193
Judith Schlenter, M. Penke
In a non-verbal aesthetic judgement task and a pre-registered production task, we tested how the orientation of the patient relative to the agent in a visual scene affects the perception and description of the depicted transitive event. Previous research has shown that a visual property like the position of the patient relative to the agent can affect speakers’ verbalization of events. Here, we investigated whether orientation constitutes another factor besides position that affects scene description. While speakers of German displayed an overall preference for scenes in which agent and patient faced each other, these scenes needed more time for sentence planning than the same scenes that showed the patient looking in the same direction and thus away from the agent. Moreover, we elicited more patient-initial sentences for face-to-face scenes than for same-direction scenes. The increase in patient-initial sentences was comparable to the increase in patient-initial sentences for scenes with left-positioned patients as compared to right-positioned patients. Based on our findings, we argue that manipulations of both position and orientation can change the prominence of the patient. The more prominent the patient (facing the agent, being placed to the left of the agent), the more likely speakers are to choose the patient as the sentence-initial subject. Hence, subtle changes of visual properties may affect not only how speakers perceive an event but also how they describe an event. Our findings are of relevance for a range of tasks that use visual materials.
在非语言审美判断任务和预注册生产任务中,我们测试了患者相对于视觉场景中的代理的方向如何影响所描述的及物事件的感知和描述。先前的研究表明,像病人相对于说话者的位置这样的视觉属性会影响说话者对事件的言语化。在这里,我们研究了除了位置之外,方向是否构成影响场景描述的另一个因素。虽然说德语的人总体上更喜欢施者和病人面对面的场景,但这些场景比病人朝同一个方向看、因此远离施者的场景需要更多的时间来规划句子。此外,我们在面对面的场景中比在相同方向的场景中引出了更多耐心的初始句子。与右位患者相比,左位患者场景中患者首句的增加与右位患者场景中患者首句的增加相当。根据我们的研究结果,我们认为位置和方向的操作可以改变患者的突出。病人的位置越突出(面对施动者,被置于施动者的左侧),说话者就越有可能选择病人作为句子的起始主语。因此,视觉特性的细微变化不仅会影响说话者感知事件的方式,还会影响他们描述事件的方式。我们的发现与使用视觉材料的一系列任务相关。
{"title":"(How) Visual properties affect the perception and description of transitive events","authors":"Judith Schlenter, M. Penke","doi":"10.5070/g601193","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601193","url":null,"abstract":"In a non-verbal aesthetic judgement task and a pre-registered production task, we tested how the orientation of the patient relative to the agent in a visual scene affects the perception and description of the depicted transitive event. Previous research has shown that a visual property like the position of the patient relative to the agent can affect speakers’ verbalization of events. Here, we investigated whether orientation constitutes another factor besides position that affects scene description. While speakers of German displayed an overall preference for scenes in which agent and patient faced each other, these scenes needed more time for sentence planning than the same scenes that showed the patient looking in the same direction and thus away from the agent. Moreover, we elicited more patient-initial sentences for face-to-face scenes than for same-direction scenes. The increase in patient-initial sentences was comparable to the increase in patient-initial sentences for scenes with left-positioned patients as compared to right-positioned patients. Based on our findings, we argue that manipulations of both position and orientation can change the prominence of the patient. The more prominent the patient (facing the agent, being placed to the left of the agent), the more likely speakers are to choose the patient as the sentence-initial subject. Hence, subtle changes of visual properties may affect not only how speakers perceive an event but also how they describe an event. Our findings are of relevance for a range of tasks that use visual materials.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"63 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-07-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114229454","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Agreement attraction error and timing profiles in continuous speech 连续讲话中的协议吸引错误和时间特征
Pub Date : 2022-05-25 DOI: 10.5070/g601157
Margaret Kandel, Cassidy Wyatt, C. Phillips
Studies of agreement attraction in language production have shown that speakers systematically produce verb agreement errors in the presence of a local noun whose features differ from that of the agreement controller. However, in attraction experiments, these errors only ever occur in a subset of trials. In the present study, we applied a naturalistic scene-description paradigm to investigate how attraction affects the distribution of errors and the time-course of correctly inflected verbs. We conducted our experiment both in the lab and in an unsupervised web-based setting. The results were strikingly similar across the experimental settings for both the error and timing analyses, demonstrating that it is possible to conduct production experiments via the internet with a high level of similarity to those done in the lab. The experiments replicated the basic number attraction effect, though they elicited comparable interference from both singular and plural local nouns, challenging common assumptions about a strong plural markedness effect in attraction. We observed slowdowns before correct verbs that paralleled the distribution of agreement errors, suggesting that the process resulting in attraction can be active even when no error is produced. Our results are easily captured by a model of agreement attraction in which errors arise at the point of computing agreement, rather than reflecting earlier errors made during initial encoding of the subject number.
对语言生成过程中一致性吸引的研究表明,当局部名词出现时,说话者会系统性地产生动词一致性错误,而局部名词的特征与一致性控制者不同。然而,在吸引力实验中,这些错误只会发生在一小部分实验中。在本研究中,我们采用自然情景描述范式来研究吸引力如何影响错误分布和正确屈折动词的时间过程。我们在实验室和无人监督的网络环境中进行了实验。在误差和时间分析的实验设置中,结果惊人地相似,这表明通过互联网进行生产实验与在实验室中进行的实验高度相似是可能的。这些实验复制了基本的数字吸引效应,尽管它们引起了来自单数和复数本地名词的类似干扰,挑战了关于复数吸引效应的普遍假设。我们观察到,在正确动词出现之前,语速减慢与一致性错误的分布是平行的,这表明,即使没有错误产生,导致吸引力的过程也可能是活跃的。我们的结果很容易被协议吸引力模型捕获,在该模型中,错误出现在计算协议的点上,而不是反映在受试者编号初始编码期间产生的早期错误。
{"title":"Agreement attraction error and timing profiles in continuous speech","authors":"Margaret Kandel, Cassidy Wyatt, C. Phillips","doi":"10.5070/g601157","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601157","url":null,"abstract":"Studies of agreement attraction in language production have shown that speakers systematically produce verb agreement errors in the presence of a local noun whose features differ from that of the agreement controller. However, in attraction experiments, these errors only ever occur in a subset of trials. In the present study, we applied a naturalistic scene-description paradigm to investigate how attraction affects the distribution of errors and the time-course of correctly inflected verbs. We conducted our experiment both in the lab and in an unsupervised web-based setting. The results were strikingly similar across the experimental settings for both the error and timing analyses, demonstrating that it is possible to conduct production experiments via the internet with a high level of similarity to those done in the lab. The experiments replicated the basic number attraction effect, though they elicited comparable interference from both singular and plural local nouns, challenging common assumptions about a strong plural markedness effect in attraction. We observed slowdowns before correct verbs that paralleled the distribution of agreement errors, suggesting that the process resulting in attraction can be active even when no error is produced. Our results are easily captured by a model of agreement attraction in which errors arise at the point of computing agreement, rather than reflecting earlier errors made during initial encoding of the subject number.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"18 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"131623927","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The processing of ambiguous pronominal reference is sensitive to depth of processing 歧义代词指称的加工对加工深度很敏感
Pub Date : 2022-05-03 DOI: 10.5070/g601166
Ava Creemers, A. Meyer
Previous studies on the processing of ambiguous pronominal reference have led to contradictory results: some suggested that ambiguity may hinder processing (Stewart, Holler, & Kidd, 2007), while others showed an ambiguity advantage (Grant, Sloggett, & Dillon, 2020) similar to what has been reported for structural ambiguities. This study provides a conceptual replication of Stewart et al. (2007, Experiment 1), to examine whether the discrepancy in earlier results is caused by the processing depth that participants engage in (cf. Swets, Desmet, Clifton, & Ferreira, 2008). We present the results from a word-by-word self-paced reading experiment with Dutch sentences that contained a personal pronoun in an embedded clause that was either ambiguous or disambiguated through gender features. Depth of processing of the embedded clause was manipulated through offline comprehension questions. The results showed that the difference in reading times for ambiguous versus unambiguous sentences depends on the processing depth: a significant ambiguity penalty was found under deep processing but not under shallow processing. No significant ambiguity advantage was found, regardless of processing depth. This replicates the results in Stewart et al. (2007) using a different methodology and a larger sample size for appropriate statistical power. These findings provide further evidence that ambiguous pronominal reference resolution is a flexible process, such that the way in which ambiguous sentences are processed depends on the depth of processing of the relevant information. Theoretical and methodological implications of these findings are discussed.
先前关于歧义代词参考的加工研究得出了相互矛盾的结果:一些研究认为歧义可能会阻碍加工(Stewart, Holler, & Kidd, 2007),而另一些研究则显示出与结构性歧义相似的歧义优势(Grant, Sloggett, & Dillon, 2020)。本研究提供了Stewart等人(2007,实验1)的概念复制,以检验早期结果的差异是否由参与者参与的加工深度引起(参见Swets, Desmet, Clifton, & Ferreira, 2008)。我们展示了一项逐字自定节奏阅读实验的结果,该实验使用荷兰语句子,这些句子在嵌入的从句中包含一个人称代词,这些句子要么模棱两可,要么通过性别特征消除了歧义。通过离线理解题来操纵嵌入子句的处理深度。结果表明,歧义句与非歧义句的阅读时间差异与加工深度有关:深度加工对歧义句的阅读时间有显著的影响,而浅加工对歧义句的阅读时间没有影响。无论加工深度如何,都没有发现明显的歧义优势。这重复了Stewart等人(2007)的结果,使用了不同的方法和更大的样本量以获得适当的统计能力。这些发现进一步证明了歧义代词指代消解是一个灵活的过程,歧义句子的处理方式取决于对相关信息的处理深度。讨论了这些发现的理论和方法意义。
{"title":"The processing of ambiguous pronominal reference is sensitive to depth of processing","authors":"Ava Creemers, A. Meyer","doi":"10.5070/g601166","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601166","url":null,"abstract":"Previous studies on the processing of ambiguous pronominal reference have led to contradictory results: some suggested that ambiguity may hinder processing (Stewart, Holler, & Kidd, 2007), while others showed an ambiguity advantage (Grant, Sloggett, & Dillon, 2020) similar to what has been reported for structural ambiguities. This study provides a conceptual replication of Stewart et al. (2007, Experiment 1), to examine whether the discrepancy in earlier results is caused by the processing depth that participants engage in (cf. Swets, Desmet, Clifton, & Ferreira, 2008). We present the results from a word-by-word self-paced reading experiment with Dutch sentences that contained a personal pronoun in an embedded clause that was either ambiguous or disambiguated through gender features. Depth of processing of the embedded clause was manipulated through offline comprehension questions. The results showed that the difference in reading times for ambiguous versus unambiguous sentences depends on the processing depth: a significant ambiguity penalty was found under deep processing but not under shallow processing. No significant ambiguity advantage was found, regardless of processing depth. This replicates the results in Stewart et al. (2007) using a different methodology and a larger sample size for appropriate statistical power. These findings provide further evidence that ambiguous pronominal reference resolution is a flexible process, such that the way in which ambiguous sentences are processed depends on the depth of processing of the relevant information. Theoretical and methodological implications of these findings are discussed.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"37 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-05-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"115236585","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Lexicalization in the developing parser 正在开发的解析器中的词汇化
Pub Date : 2022-03-30 DOI: 10.5070/g601148
Aaron Steven White, J. Lidz
We use children’s noun learning as a probe into the nature of their syntactic prediction mechanism and the statistical knowledge on which that prediction mechanism is based. We focus on verb-based predictions, considering two possibilities: children’s syntactic predictions might rely on distributional knowledge about specific verbs—i.e. they might be lexicalized — or they might rely on distributional knowledge that is general to all verbs. In an intermodal preferential looking experiment, we establish that, by as early as 19 months of age, verb-based predictions are lexicalized: children encode the syntactic distributions of particular verbs and use those distributions to make predictions, but they do not assume that these can be used for verbs in general. knowledge from specific lexical Our data suggests that syntactic knowledge begins with abstract categories and that lexically specific distributional information informs the development of parsing strategies, but not the knowledge itself. That knowledge is revealed when we take away children’s ability to rely on lexically specific knowledge, as in the current study.
本文以儿童名词学习为研究对象,探讨儿童句法预测机制的本质及其所依据的统计知识。我们专注于基于动词的预测,考虑了两种可能性:儿童的句法预测可能依赖于对特定动词的分布性知识。它们可能是词汇化的——或者它们可能依赖于所有动词通用的分布知识。在一项多式联运优先观察实验中,我们发现,早在19个月大的时候,基于动词的预测就已经被词汇化了:孩子们对特定动词的句法分布进行编码,并使用这些分布进行预测,但他们并不认为这些可以用于一般的动词。我们的数据表明,句法知识始于抽象类别,词汇特定的分布信息会影响解析策略的发展,而不是知识本身。当我们不考虑孩子依赖词汇特定知识的能力时,这种知识就会显现出来,就像目前的研究一样。
{"title":"Lexicalization in the developing parser","authors":"Aaron Steven White, J. Lidz","doi":"10.5070/g601148","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5070/g601148","url":null,"abstract":"We use children’s noun learning as a probe into the nature of their syntactic prediction mechanism and the statistical knowledge on which that prediction mechanism is based. We focus on verb-based predictions, considering two possibilities: children’s syntactic predictions might rely on distributional knowledge about specific verbs—i.e. they might be lexicalized — or they might rely on distributional knowledge that is general to all verbs. In an intermodal preferential looking experiment, we establish that, by as early as 19 months of age, verb-based predictions are lexicalized: children encode the syntactic distributions of particular verbs and use those distributions to make predictions, but they do not assume that these can be used for verbs in general. knowledge from specific lexical Our data suggests that syntactic knowledge begins with abstract categories and that lexically specific distributional information informs the development of parsing strategies, but not the knowledge itself. That knowledge is revealed when we take away children’s ability to rely on lexically specific knowledge, as in the current study.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"23 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-03-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114987200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Assessing the replication landscape in experimental linguistics 评估实验语言学中的复制景观
Pub Date : 2022-02-18 DOI: 10.31234/osf.io/fzngs
Kristina Kobrock, Timo B. Roettger
Replications are an integral part of cumulative experimental science. Yet many scientific disciplines do not replicate enough because novel confirmatory findings are valued over direct replications. To provide a systematic assessment of the replication landscape in experimental linguistics, the present study estimated replication rates for over 50.000 articles across 98 journals. We used automatic string matching using the Web of Science combined with in-depth manual inspections of 210 papers. The median rate of mentioning the search string "replicat*" was as low as 1.6%. Subsequent manual analyses revealed that only eight of these were direct replications, i.e. studies that arrive at the same scientific conclusions as an initial study by using exactly the same methodology. Moreover, only 1 in 1600 experimental linguistic studies reports a direct replication performed by independent researchers. We conclude that, similar to neighboring disciplines, experimental linguistics does not replicate enough.
重复实验是累积实验科学的一个组成部分。然而,许多科学学科没有足够的重复性,因为新的证实性发现比直接重复性更有价值。为了对实验语言学的复制景观进行系统评估,本研究估计了98种期刊上超过50,000篇文章的复制率。我们使用了自动字符串匹配,结合了对210篇论文的深入人工检查。提及搜索字符串“replicat*”的中位数率低至1.6%。随后的人工分析显示,其中只有8项是直接重复的,即通过使用完全相同的方法得出与最初研究相同的科学结论的研究。此外,1600项实验语言学研究中只有1项报告了由独立研究人员进行的直接复制。我们得出的结论是,与邻近学科类似,实验语言学的重复性不够。
{"title":"Assessing the replication landscape in experimental linguistics","authors":"Kristina Kobrock, Timo B. Roettger","doi":"10.31234/osf.io/fzngs","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/fzngs","url":null,"abstract":"Replications are an integral part of cumulative experimental science. Yet many scientific disciplines do not replicate enough because novel confirmatory findings are valued over direct replications. To provide a systematic assessment of the replication landscape in experimental linguistics, the present study estimated replication rates for over 50.000 articles across 98 journals. We used automatic string matching using the Web of Science combined with in-depth manual inspections of 210 papers. The median rate of mentioning the search string \"replicat*\" was as low as 1.6%. Subsequent manual analyses revealed that only eight of these were direct replications, i.e. studies that arrive at the same scientific conclusions as an initial study by using exactly the same methodology. Moreover, only 1 in 1600 experimental linguistic studies reports a direct replication performed by independent researchers. We conclude that, similar to neighboring disciplines, experimental linguistics does not replicate enough.","PeriodicalId":164622,"journal":{"name":"Glossa Psycholinguistics","volume":"26 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2022-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"128939139","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Glossa Psycholinguistics
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1