首页 > 最新文献

Learning & memory最新文献

英文 中文
Sex and the facilitation of cued fear by prior contextual fear conditioning in rats. 大鼠的性别与先前情境恐惧条件反射对诱发恐惧的促进作用
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-09-26 Print Date: 2024-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.054010.124
Katherine Vazquez, Kehinde E Cole, Ryan G Parsons

Previous studies have shown that the formation of new memories can be influenced by prior experience. This includes work using Pavlovian fear conditioning in rodents that has shown that an initial fear conditioning experience can become associated with and facilitate the acquisition of new fear memories, especially when they occur close together in time. However, most of the prior studies used only males as subjects, resulting in questions about the generalizability of the findings from this work. Here we tested whether prior contextual fear conditioning would facilitate later learning of cued fear conditioning in both male and female rats, and if there were differences based on the interval between the two conditioning episodes. Our results showed that levels of cued fear were not influenced by prior contextual fear conditioning or by the interval between training; however, females showed lower levels of cued fear. Freezing behavior in the initial training context differed by sex, with females showing lower levels of contextual fear, and by the type of initial training, with rats given delayed shock showing higher levels of fear than rats given immediate shock during contextual fear conditioning. These results indicate that contextual fear conditioning does not prime subsequent cued fear conditioning and that female rats express lower levels of cued and contextual fear conditioning than males.

以往的研究表明,新记忆的形成会受到先前经验的影响。其中包括利用巴甫洛夫恐惧条件反射在啮齿类动物身上进行的研究,该研究表明,最初的恐惧条件反射经历会与新的恐惧记忆联系起来,并促进新恐惧记忆的获得,尤其是当它们发生的时间很接近时。然而,之前的大多数研究都只使用雄性动物作为研究对象,这就导致研究结果的普遍性受到质疑。在这里,我们测试了先前的情境恐惧条件反射是否会促进雄性和雌性大鼠后来对提示恐惧条件反射的学习,以及这两次条件反射之间的间隔是否存在差异。我们的结果表明,提示恐惧的水平不受先前情境恐惧条件反射或训练间隔的影响;但是,雌性大鼠的提示恐惧水平较低。初始训练情境中的冻结行为因性别和初始训练类型而异,雌性大鼠表现出较低的情境恐惧水平,在情境恐惧条件反射中,给予延迟电击的大鼠比给予立即电击的大鼠表现出更高的恐惧水平。这些结果表明,情境恐惧条件反射不会为随后的提示恐惧条件反射提供素材,而且雌性大鼠比雄性大鼠表现出更低的提示和情境恐惧条件反射水平。
{"title":"Sex and the facilitation of cued fear by prior contextual fear conditioning in rats.","authors":"Katherine Vazquez, Kehinde E Cole, Ryan G Parsons","doi":"10.1101/lm.054010.124","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.054010.124","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have shown that the formation of new memories can be influenced by prior experience. This includes work using Pavlovian fear conditioning in rodents that has shown that an initial fear conditioning experience can become associated with and facilitate the acquisition of new fear memories, especially when they occur close together in time. However, most of the prior studies used only males as subjects, resulting in questions about the generalizability of the findings from this work. Here we tested whether prior contextual fear conditioning would facilitate later learning of cued fear conditioning in both male and female rats, and if there were differences based on the interval between the two conditioning episodes. Our results showed that levels of cued fear were not influenced by prior contextual fear conditioning or by the interval between training; however, females showed lower levels of cued fear. Freezing behavior in the initial training context differed by sex, with females showing lower levels of contextual fear, and by the type of initial training, with rats given delayed shock showing higher levels of fear than rats given immediate shock during contextual fear conditioning. These results indicate that contextual fear conditioning does not prime subsequent cued fear conditioning and that female rats express lower levels of cued and contextual fear conditioning than males.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-09-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142349384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
High stakes slow responding, but do not help overcome Pavlovian biases in humans 高风险会减慢反应速度,但无助于克服人类的巴甫洛夫偏差
IF 2 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.054017.124
Johannes Algermissen, Hanneke E.M. den Ouden
“Pavlovian” or “motivational” biases are the phenomenon that the valence of prospective outcomes modulates action invigoration: the prospect of reward invigorates actions, while the prospect of punishment suppresses actions. Effects of the valence of prospective outcomes are well established, but it remains unclear how the magnitude of outcomes (“stake magnitude”) modulates these biases. In this preregistered study (N = 55), we manipulated stake magnitude (high vs. low) in an orthogonalized Motivational Go/NoGo Task. We tested whether higher stakes (a) strengthen biases or (b) elicit cognitive control recruitment, enhancing the suppression of biases in motivationally incongruent conditions. Confirmatory tests showed that high stakes slowed down responding, especially in motivationally incongruent conditions. However, high stakes did not affect whether a response was made or not, and did not change the magnitude of Pavlovian biases. Reinforcement-learning drift-diffusion models (RL-DDMs) fit to the data suggested that response slowing was best captured by stakes prolonging the non-decision time. There was no effect of the stakes on the response threshold (as in typical speed-accuracy trade-offs). In sum, these results suggest that high stakes slow down responses without affecting the expression of Pavlovian biases in behavior. We speculate that this slowing under high stakes might reflect heightened cognitive control, which is however ineffectively used, or reflect positive conditioned suppression, i.e., the interference between goal-directed and consummatory behaviors, a phenomenon previously observed in rodents that might also exist in humans. Pavlovian biases and slowing under high stakes may arise in parallel to each other.
"巴甫洛夫 "或 "动机 "偏差是指预期结果的价值调节行动激励的现象:奖励的前景激励行动,而惩罚的前景抑制行动。预期结果的价值影响已得到证实,但结果的大小("利害关系大小")如何调节这些偏差仍不清楚。在这项预先登记的研究中(N = 55),我们在一个正交化的动机去/不去任务中操纵了赌注大小(高与低)。我们测试了较高的赌注是否会(a)加强偏差或(b)引起认知控制招募,从而在动机不一致的条件下加强对偏差的抑制。确认测试表明,高赌注会减慢反应速度,尤其是在动机不一致的条件下。然而,高赌注并不会影响是否做出反应,也不会改变巴甫洛夫偏差的大小。与数据拟合的强化学习漂移-扩散模型(RL-DDM)表明,赌注延长了不做决定的时间最能反映反应减慢的情况。利害关系对反应阈值没有影响(如典型的速度-准确性权衡)。总之,这些结果表明,高风险会减慢反应速度,但不会影响行为中巴甫洛夫偏差的表达。我们推测,这种高风险下的反应减慢可能反映了认知控制的增强,然而这种控制并没有得到有效利用,或者反映了积极的条件抑制,即目标导向行为和消耗性行为之间的干扰,这种现象以前在啮齿类动物身上观察到过,可能也存在于人类身上。巴甫洛夫偏差和高风险下的迟钝可能同时出现。
{"title":"High stakes slow responding, but do not help overcome Pavlovian biases in humans","authors":"Johannes Algermissen, Hanneke E.M. den Ouden","doi":"10.1101/lm.054017.124","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.054017.124","url":null,"abstract":"“Pavlovian” or “motivational” biases are the phenomenon that the valence of prospective outcomes modulates action invigoration: the prospect of reward invigorates actions, while the prospect of punishment suppresses actions. Effects of the valence of prospective outcomes are well established, but it remains unclear how the magnitude of outcomes (“stake magnitude”) modulates these biases. In this preregistered study (<em>N</em> = 55), we manipulated stake magnitude (high vs. low) in an orthogonalized Motivational Go/NoGo Task. We tested whether higher stakes (a) strengthen biases or (b) elicit cognitive control recruitment, enhancing the suppression of biases in motivationally incongruent conditions. Confirmatory tests showed that high stakes slowed down responding, especially in motivationally incongruent conditions. However, high stakes did not affect whether a response was made or not, and did not change the magnitude of Pavlovian biases. Reinforcement-learning drift-diffusion models (RL-DDMs) fit to the data suggested that response slowing was best captured by stakes prolonging the non-decision time. There was no effect of the stakes on the response threshold (as in typical speed-accuracy trade-offs). In sum, these results suggest that high stakes slow down responses without affecting the expression of Pavlovian biases in behavior. We speculate that this slowing under high stakes might reflect heightened cognitive control, which is however ineffectively used, or reflect positive conditioned suppression, i.e., the interference between goal-directed and consummatory behaviors, a phenomenon previously observed in rodents that might also exist in humans. Pavlovian biases and slowing under high stakes may arise in parallel to each other.","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142251691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decoding Arc transcription: a live-cell study of stimulation patterns and transcriptional output 解码弧转录:刺激模式和转录输出的活细胞研究
IF 2 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.054024.124
Dong Wook Kim, Hyungseok C. Moon, Byung Hun Lee, Hye Yoon Park
Activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc) plays a crucial role in synaptic plasticity, a process integral to learning and memory. Arc transcription is induced within a few minutes of stimulation, making it a useful marker for neuronal activity. However, the specific neuronal activity patterns that initiate Arc transcription have remained elusive due to the inability to observe mRNA transcription in live cells in real time. Using a genetically encoded RNA indicator (GERI) mouse model that expresses endogenous Arc mRNA tagged with multiple GFPs, we investigated Arc transcriptional activity in response to various electrical field stimulation patterns. The GERI mouse model was generated by crossing the Arc-PBS knock-in mouse, engineered with binding sites in the 3′ untranslated region (UTR) of Arc mRNA, and the transgenic mouse expressing the cognate binding protein fused to GFP. In dissociated hippocampal neurons, we found that the pattern of stimulation significantly affects Arc transcription. Specifically, theta-burst stimulation consisting of high-frequency (100 Hz) bursts delivered at 10 Hz frequency induced the highest rate of Arc transcription. Concurrently, the amplitudes of nuclear calcium transients also reached their peak with 10 Hz burst stimulation, indicating a correlation between calcium concentration and transcription. However, our dual-color single-cell imaging revealed that there were no significant differences in calcium amplitudes between Arc-positive and Arc-negative neurons upon 10 Hz burst stimulation, suggesting the involvement of other factors in the induction of Arc transcription. Our live-cell RNA imaging provides a deeper insight into the complex regulation of transcription by activity patterns and calcium signaling pathways.
活动调节细胞骨架相关蛋白(Arc)在突触可塑性中扮演着至关重要的角色,而突触可塑性是学习和记忆过程中不可或缺的一个环节。Arc 在受到刺激后几分钟内就会被诱导转录,从而成为神经元活动的有用标记。然而,由于无法在活细胞中实时观察 mRNA 转录,启动 Arc 转录的特定神经元活动模式一直难以捉摸。我们利用基因编码 RNA 指示剂(GERI)小鼠模型(该模型表达标记有多个 GFP 的内源性 Arc mRNA),研究了 Arc 在各种电场刺激模式下的转录活动。GERI小鼠模型是由Arc-PBS基因敲入小鼠和表达融合了GFP的同源结合蛋白的转基因小鼠杂交产生的,Arc-PBS基因敲入小鼠在Arc mRNA的3′非翻译区(UTR)设计了结合位点。在离体的海马神经元中,我们发现刺激模式会显著影响 Arc 的转录。具体来说,由 10 赫兹频率的高频(100 赫兹)脉冲串组成的θ-脉冲串刺激诱导的 Arc 转录率最高。与此同时,核钙质瞬态的振幅也在 10 Hz 脉冲串刺激下达到峰值,这表明钙质浓度与转录之间存在相关性。然而,我们的双色单细胞成像显示,在 10 Hz 脉冲串刺激下,弧阳性神经元和弧阴性神经元的钙离子幅值没有显著差异,这表明弧转录的诱导还涉及其他因素。我们的活细胞 RNA 成像更深入地揭示了活动模式和钙信号通路对转录的复杂调控。
{"title":"Decoding Arc transcription: a live-cell study of stimulation patterns and transcriptional output","authors":"Dong Wook Kim, Hyungseok C. Moon, Byung Hun Lee, Hye Yoon Park","doi":"10.1101/lm.054024.124","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.054024.124","url":null,"abstract":"Activity-regulated cytoskeleton-associated protein (Arc) plays a crucial role in synaptic plasticity, a process integral to learning and memory. Arc transcription is induced within a few minutes of stimulation, making it a useful marker for neuronal activity. However, the specific neuronal activity patterns that initiate Arc transcription have remained elusive due to the inability to observe mRNA transcription in live cells in real time. Using a genetically encoded RNA indicator (GERI) mouse model that expresses endogenous Arc mRNA tagged with multiple GFPs, we investigated Arc transcriptional activity in response to various electrical field stimulation patterns. The GERI mouse model was generated by crossing the Arc-PBS knock-in mouse, engineered with binding sites in the 3′ untranslated region (UTR) of Arc mRNA, and the transgenic mouse expressing the cognate binding protein fused to GFP. In dissociated hippocampal neurons, we found that the pattern of stimulation significantly affects Arc transcription. Specifically, theta-burst stimulation consisting of high-frequency (100 Hz) bursts delivered at 10 Hz frequency induced the highest rate of Arc transcription. Concurrently, the amplitudes of nuclear calcium transients also reached their peak with 10 Hz burst stimulation, indicating a correlation between calcium concentration and transcription. However, our dual-color single-cell imaging revealed that there were no significant differences in calcium amplitudes between Arc-positive and Arc-negative neurons upon 10 Hz burst stimulation, suggesting the involvement of other factors in the induction of Arc transcription. Our live-cell RNA imaging provides a deeper insight into the complex regulation of transcription by activity patterns and calcium signaling pathways.","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142218466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Safety signals reinforce instrumental avoidance in humans 安全信号会强化人类的工具回避行为
IF 2 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053914.123
Courteney T.L. Fisher, Gonzalo P. Urcelay
Safety signals reinforce instrumental avoidance behavior in nonhuman animals. However, there are no conclusive demonstrations of this phenomenon in humans. Using human participants in an avoidance task, Experiments 1–3 and 5 were conducted online to assess the reinforcing properties of safety signals, and Experiment 4 was conducted in the laboratory. Participants were trained with CSs+ and CSs–, and they could avoid an aversive outcome during presentations of the CSs+ by pressing their space bar at a specific time. If successful, the aversive outcome was not presented but instead a safety signal was. Participants were then tested—whilst on extinction—with two new ambiguous test CSs. If participants made an avoidance response, one of the test CSs produced the trained safety signal and the other was a control. In Experiments 1 and 4, the control was followed by no signal. In Experiment 2, the control was followed by a signal that differed in one dimension (color) with the trained safety signal, and in Experiment 3, the control differed in two dimensions (shape and color) from the trained safety signal. Experiment 5 tested the reinforcing properties of the safety signal using a choice procedure and a new response during test. We observed that participants made more avoidance responses to the ambiguous test CSs when followed by the trained signal in Experiments 1, 3, 4, and 5 (but not in Experiment 2). Overall, these results suggest that trained safety signals can reinforce avoidance behavior in humans.
安全信号会强化非人类动物的工具性规避行为。然而,这种现象在人类身上还没有确凿的证据。为了评估安全信号的强化特性,实验 1-3 和实验 5 在网上进行,实验 4 在实验室进行。参与者接受了 CSs+ 和 CSs- 的训练,在 CSs+ 出现时,他们可以通过在特定时间按下空格键来避免厌恶结果。如果成功,则不会出现厌恶结果,而是会出现一个安全信号。然后,参与者在消退过程中接受两个新的模棱两可的 CS 测试。如果参与者做出回避反应,其中一个测试 CS 就会产生经过训练的安全信号,而另一个则是对照组。在实验 1 和 4 中,对照组之后没有信号。在实验 2 中,对照组之后的信号在一个维度(颜色)上与训练过的安全信号不同;在实验 3 中,对照组之后的信号在两个维度(形状和颜色)上与训练过的安全信号不同。实验 5 利用选择程序和测试期间的新反应测试了安全信号的强化特性。我们观察到,在实验 1、3、4 和 5 中(但在实验 2 中没有观察到),当被试在训练信号后对模棱两可的测试 CS 做出更多的回避反应。总之,这些结果表明,训练过的安全信号可以强化人类的回避行为。
{"title":"Safety signals reinforce instrumental avoidance in humans","authors":"Courteney T.L. Fisher, Gonzalo P. Urcelay","doi":"10.1101/lm.053914.123","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053914.123","url":null,"abstract":"Safety signals reinforce instrumental avoidance behavior in nonhuman animals. However, there are no conclusive demonstrations of this phenomenon in humans. Using human participants in an avoidance task, Experiments 1–3 and 5 were conducted online to assess the reinforcing properties of safety signals, and Experiment 4 was conducted in the laboratory. Participants were trained with CSs+ and CSs–, and they could avoid an aversive outcome during presentations of the CSs+ by pressing their space bar at a specific time. If successful, the aversive outcome was not presented but instead a safety signal was. Participants were then tested—whilst on extinction—with two new ambiguous test CSs. If participants made an avoidance response, one of the test CSs produced the trained safety signal and the other was a control. In Experiments 1 and 4, the control was followed by no signal. In Experiment 2, the control was followed by a signal that differed in one dimension (color) with the trained safety signal, and in Experiment 3, the control differed in two dimensions (shape and color) from the trained safety signal. Experiment 5 tested the reinforcing properties of the safety signal using a choice procedure and a new response during test. We observed that participants made more avoidance responses to the ambiguous test CSs when followed by the trained signal in Experiments 1, 3, 4, and 5 (but not in Experiment 2). Overall, these results suggest that trained safety signals can reinforce avoidance behavior in humans.","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142218465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connecting self-report and instrumental behavior during incubation of food craving in humans. 将人类食物渴求潜伏期的自我报告和工具行为联系起来。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-31 Print Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053869.123
Nicholas A Ruiz, Devlin Eckardt, Lisa A Briand, Mathieu Wimmer, Vishnu P Murty

Incubation of craving is a phenomenon describing the intensification of craving for a reward over extended periods of abstinence from reinforcement. Animal models use instrumental markers of craving to reward cues to examine incubation, while human paradigms rely on subjective self-reports. Here, we characterize an animal-inspired, novel human paradigm that showed strong positive relationships between self-reports and instrumental markers of craving for favored palatable foods. Further, we found consistent nonlinear relationships with time since last consumption and self-reports, and preliminary patterns between time and instrumental responses. These findings provide a novel approach to establishing an animal-inspired human model of incubation.

渴求潜伏是一种现象,描述的是在长时间缺乏强化的情况下,对奖赏的渴求会增强。动物模型使用渴求奖赏线索的工具性标记来研究潜伏期,而人类范例则依赖于主观的自我报告。在这里,我们描述了一个受动物启发的新型人类范例,该范例显示了自我报告与对所喜爱的美味食物的工具性渴求标记之间的密切正相关关系。此外,我们还发现了自上次消费后的时间与自我报告之间的一致非线性关系,以及时间与工具反应之间的初步模式。这些发现为建立受动物启发的人类潜伏模型提供了一种新方法。
{"title":"Connecting self-report and instrumental behavior during incubation of food craving in humans.","authors":"Nicholas A Ruiz, Devlin Eckardt, Lisa A Briand, Mathieu Wimmer, Vishnu P Murty","doi":"10.1101/lm.053869.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053869.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Incubation of craving is a phenomenon describing the intensification of craving for a reward over extended periods of abstinence from reinforcement. Animal models use instrumental markers of craving to reward cues to examine incubation, while human paradigms rely on subjective self-reports. Here, we characterize an animal-inspired, novel human paradigm that showed strong positive relationships between self-reports and instrumental markers of craving for favored palatable foods. Further, we found consistent nonlinear relationships with time since last consumption and self-reports, and preliminary patterns between time and instrumental responses. These findings provide a novel approach to establishing an animal-inspired human model of incubation.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11369634/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141860192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Network motifs exhibiting a differential response to spaced and massed inputs. 网络图案对间隔输入和大量输入表现出不同的反应。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-29 Print Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.054012.124
Ashley Sreejan, Priyanka Saxena, Chetan J Gadgil

One characteristic of long-term memory is the existence of an inverted U-shaped response to increasing intervals between training sessions, and consequently, an optimal spacing that maximizes memory formation. Current models of this spacing effect focus on specific molecular components and their interactions. Here, we computationally study the underlying network architecture, in particular, the potential of motif dynamics in qualitatively capturing the spacing effect in a manner that is independent of the animal model, biomolecular components, and the timescales involved. We define a common training and test protocol, and computationally identify network topologies that can qualitatively replicate the experimentally observed characteristics of the spacing effect. For 41 motifs derived from fundamental network architectures such as autoregulation, feedback, and feedforward motifs, we tested their capacity to manifest the spacing effect in terms of an inverted U-shaped response curve, using different combinations of stimulation protocols, response metrics, and kinetic parameters. Our findings indicate that positive feedback motifs where the stimulus enhances conversion reaction in the loop replicate the spacing effect across all response metrics, while feedforward motifs exhibit a metric-specific spacing effect. For some parameter combinations, linear cascades of activation and conversion reactions were found sufficient to qualitatively exhibit spacing effect characteristics.

长期记忆的一个特点是,训练间隔的增加会产生倒 "U "形反应,因此,最佳的间隔能最大限度地形成记忆。目前这种间隔效应的模型主要集中在特定的分子成分及其相互作用上。在这里,我们通过计算研究了底层网络结构,特别是动机动力学在定性捕捉间距效应方面的潜力,其方式与动物模型、生物分子成分和所涉及的时间尺度无关。我们定义了一个通用的训练和测试协议,并通过计算确定了可以定性复制实验观察到的间距效应特征的网络拓扑结构。对于从自动调节、反馈和前馈等基本网络架构中衍生出的 41 个图案,我们使用不同的刺激协议、响应指标和动力学参数组合,测试了它们在倒 U 型响应曲线方面体现间距效应的能力。我们的研究结果表明,在正反馈模式中,刺激会增强回路中的转换反应,从而在所有反应指标中复制间距效应,而前馈模式则表现出特定指标的间距效应。对于某些参数组合,我们发现激活和转换反应的线性级联足以定性地表现出间距效应特征。
{"title":"Network motifs exhibiting a differential response to spaced and massed inputs.","authors":"Ashley Sreejan, Priyanka Saxena, Chetan J Gadgil","doi":"10.1101/lm.054012.124","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.054012.124","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One characteristic of long-term memory is the existence of an inverted U-shaped response to increasing intervals between training sessions, and consequently, an optimal spacing that maximizes memory formation. Current models of this spacing effect focus on specific molecular components and their interactions. Here, we computationally study the underlying network architecture, in particular, the potential of motif dynamics in qualitatively capturing the spacing effect in a manner that is independent of the animal model, biomolecular components, and the timescales involved. We define a common training and test protocol, and computationally identify network topologies that can qualitatively replicate the experimentally observed characteristics of the spacing effect. For 41 motifs derived from fundamental network architectures such as autoregulation, feedback, and feedforward motifs, we tested their capacity to manifest the spacing effect in terms of an inverted U-shaped response curve, using different combinations of stimulation protocols, response metrics, and kinetic parameters. Our findings indicate that positive feedback motifs where the stimulus enhances conversion reaction in the loop replicate the spacing effect across all response metrics, while feedforward motifs exhibit a metric-specific spacing effect. For some parameter combinations, linear cascades of activation and conversion reactions were found sufficient to qualitatively exhibit spacing effect characteristics.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11369633/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141792852","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexible decision-making is related to strategy learning, vicarious trial and error, and medial prefrontal rhythms during spatial set-shifting. 灵活决策与策略学习、模仿性试验和错误以及空间集合转移过程中的内侧前额叶节律有关。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-22 Print Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053911.123
Jesse T Miles, Ginger L Mullins, Sheri J Y Mizumori

Flexible decision-making requires a balance between exploring features of an environment and exploiting prior knowledge. Behavioral flexibility is typically measured by how long it takes subjects to consistently make accurate choices after reward contingencies switch or task rules change. This measure, however, only allows for tracking flexibility across multiple trials, and does not assess the degree of flexibility. Plus, although increases in decision-making accuracy are strong indicators of learning, other decision-making behaviors have also been suggested as markers of flexibility, such as the on-the-fly decision reversals known as vicarious trial and error (VTE) or switches to a different, but incorrect, strategy. We sought to relate flexibility, learning, and neural activity by comparing choice history-derived evaluation of strategy use with changes in decision-making accuracy and VTE behavior while recording from the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in rats. Using a set-shifting task that required rats to repeatedly switch between spatial decision-making strategies, we show that a previously developed strategy likelihood estimation procedure could identify putative learning points based on decision history. We confirm the efficacy of learning point estimation by showing increases in decision-making accuracy aligned to the learning point. Additionally, we show increases in the rate of VTE behavior surrounding identified learning points. By calculating changes in strategy likelihoods across trials, we tracked flexibility on a trial-by-trial basis and show that flexibility scores also increased around learning points. Further, we demonstrate that VTE behaviors could be separated into indecisive and deliberative subtypes depending on whether they occurred during periods of high or low flexibility and whether they led to correct or incorrect choice outcomes. Field potential recordings from the mPFC during decisions exhibited increased beta band activity on trials with VTE compared to non-VTE trials, as well as increased gamma during periods when learned strategies could be exploited compared to prelearning, exploratory periods. This study demonstrates that increased behavioral flexibility and VTE rates are often aligned to task learning. These relationships can break down, however, suggesting that VTE is not always an indicator of deliberative decision-making. Additionally, we further implicate the mPFC in decision-making and learning by showing increased beta-based activity on VTE trials and increased gamma after learning.

灵活决策需要在探索环境特征和利用已有知识之间取得平衡。行为灵活性通常是通过受试者在奖励条件转换或任务规则改变后持续做出准确选择所需的时间来衡量的。然而,这种测量方法只能跟踪多次试验的灵活性,并不能评估灵活性的程度。此外,虽然决策准确性的提高是学习的有力指标,但其他决策行为也被认为是灵活性的标志,如被称为 "替代性试验和错误"(VTE)的即时决策逆转,或切换到不同但不正确的策略。我们试图将灵活性、学习和神经活动联系起来,方法是在记录大鼠内侧前额叶皮层(mPFC)的同时,比较由选择历史衍生的策略使用评估与决策准确性和VTE行为的变化。我们使用了一项要求大鼠在空间决策策略之间反复切换的集合转换任务,结果表明之前开发的策略可能性估计程序可以根据决策历史确定推定的学习点。我们证实了学习点估计的有效性,因为它显示了与学习点一致的决策准确性的提高。此外,我们还显示了在所识别的学习点周围 VTE 行为率的增加。通过计算各次试验中策略可能性的变化,我们对各次试验的灵活性进行了跟踪,结果表明学习点周围的灵活性得分也有所提高。此外,我们还证明,VTE 行为可根据其发生在灵活性高或低的时期,以及其导致的选择结果是正确还是错误,分为优柔寡断和深思熟虑两种亚型。在做决定时从 mPFC 进行的场电位记录显示,与非 VTE 试验相比,VTE 试验中的β波段活动增加了;与学习前的探索期相比,在学习策略可被利用的时期,γ波段活动增加了。这项研究表明,行为灵活性的提高和 VTE 率的增加往往与任务学习有关。然而,这些关系可能会破裂,这表明 VTE 并不总是慎重决策的指标。此外,我们通过显示 VTE 试验中增加的基于 beta 的活动和学习后增加的 gamma 活动,进一步揭示了 mPFC 在决策和学习中的作用。
{"title":"Flexible decision-making is related to strategy learning, vicarious trial and error, and medial prefrontal rhythms during spatial set-shifting.","authors":"Jesse T Miles, Ginger L Mullins, Sheri J Y Mizumori","doi":"10.1101/lm.053911.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053911.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Flexible decision-making requires a balance between exploring features of an environment and exploiting prior knowledge. Behavioral flexibility is typically measured by how long it takes subjects to consistently make accurate choices after reward contingencies switch or task rules change. This measure, however, only allows for tracking flexibility across multiple trials, and does not assess the degree of flexibility. Plus, although increases in decision-making accuracy are strong indicators of learning, other decision-making behaviors have also been suggested as markers of flexibility, such as the on-the-fly decision reversals known as vicarious trial and error (VTE) or switches to a different, but incorrect, strategy. We sought to relate flexibility, learning, and neural activity by comparing choice history-derived evaluation of strategy use with changes in decision-making accuracy and VTE behavior while recording from the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) in rats. Using a set-shifting task that required rats to repeatedly switch between spatial decision-making strategies, we show that a previously developed strategy likelihood estimation procedure could identify putative learning points based on decision history. We confirm the efficacy of learning point estimation by showing increases in decision-making accuracy aligned to the learning point. Additionally, we show increases in the rate of VTE behavior surrounding identified learning points. By calculating changes in strategy likelihoods across trials, we tracked flexibility on a trial-by-trial basis and show that flexibility scores also increased around learning points. Further, we demonstrate that VTE behaviors could be separated into indecisive and deliberative subtypes depending on whether they occurred during periods of high or low flexibility and whether they led to correct or incorrect choice outcomes. Field potential recordings from the mPFC during decisions exhibited increased beta band activity on trials with VTE compared to non-VTE trials, as well as increased gamma during periods when learned strategies could be exploited compared to prelearning, exploratory periods. This study demonstrates that increased behavioral flexibility and VTE rates are often aligned to task learning. These relationships can break down, however, suggesting that VTE is not always an indicator of deliberative decision-making. Additionally, we further implicate the mPFC in decision-making and learning by showing increased beta-based activity on VTE trials and increased gamma after learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11369635/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141748518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transient enhancement of stimulus-evoked activity in neocortex during sensory learning. 感觉学习过程中新皮层刺激诱发活动的短暂增强
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-02 Print Date: 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053870.123
Mo Zhu, Sandra J Kuhlman, Alison L Barth

Synaptic potentiation has been linked to learning in sensory cortex, but the connection between this potentiation and increased sensory-evoked neural activity is not clear. Here, we used longitudinal in vivo Ca2+ imaging in the barrel cortex of awake mice to test the hypothesis that increased excitatory synaptic strength during the learning of a whisker-dependent sensory-association task would be correlated with enhanced stimulus-evoked firing. To isolate stimulus-evoked responses from dynamic, task-related activity, imaging was performed outside of the training context. Although prior studies indicate that multiwhisker stimuli drive robust subthreshold activity, we observed sparse activation of L2/3 pyramidal (Pyr) neurons in both control and trained mice. Despite evidence for excitatory synaptic strengthening at thalamocortical and intracortical synapses in this brain area at the onset of learning-indeed, under our imaging conditions thalamocortical axons were robustly activated-we observed that L2/3 Pyr neurons in somatosensory (barrel) cortex displayed only modest increases in stimulus-evoked activity that were concentrated at the onset of training. Activity renormalized over longer training periods. In contrast, when stimuli and rewards were uncoupled in a pseudotraining paradigm, stimulus-evoked activity in L2/3 Pyr neurons was significantly suppressed. These findings indicate that sensory-association training but not sensory stimulation without coupled rewards may briefly enhance sensory-evoked activity, a phenomenon that might help link sensory input to behavioral outcomes at the onset of learning.

突触电位与感觉皮层的学习有关,但这种电位与感觉诱发的神经活动增加之间的联系尚不清楚。在这里,我们利用清醒小鼠桶状皮层的纵向活体 Ca2+ 成像来验证一个假设:在学习胡须依赖性感觉联想任务的过程中,兴奋性突触强度的增加会与刺激诱发的发射增强相关。为了将刺激诱发反应从动态的任务相关活动中分离出来,成像是在训练环境之外进行的。尽管之前的研究表明多鞭毛刺激会驱动强阈下活动,但我们在对照组和训练组小鼠中都观察到了 L2/3 锥体(Pyr)神经元的稀疏激活。尽管有证据表明在学习开始时这一脑区的丘皮质和皮质内突触的兴奋性突触加强--事实上,在我们的成像条件下,丘皮质轴突被强有力地激活--但我们观察到,体感(桶状)皮层中的 L2/3 Pyr 神经元只显示出刺激诱发活动的适度增加,而且集中在训练开始时。随着训练时间的延长,活动重新正常化。相反,在假训练范式中,当刺激和奖赏不耦合时,L2/3 Pyr 神经元的刺激诱发活动明显受到抑制。这些研究结果表明,感觉联想训练(而非无耦合奖励的感觉刺激)可能会短暂增强感觉诱发活动,这种现象可能有助于在学习开始时将感觉输入与行为结果联系起来。
{"title":"Transient enhancement of stimulus-evoked activity in neocortex during sensory learning.","authors":"Mo Zhu, Sandra J Kuhlman, Alison L Barth","doi":"10.1101/lm.053870.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053870.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Synaptic potentiation has been linked to learning in sensory cortex, but the connection between this potentiation and increased sensory-evoked neural activity is not clear. Here, we used longitudinal in vivo Ca<sup>2+</sup> imaging in the barrel cortex of awake mice to test the hypothesis that increased excitatory synaptic strength during the learning of a whisker-dependent sensory-association task would be correlated with enhanced stimulus-evoked firing. To isolate stimulus-evoked responses from dynamic, task-related activity, imaging was performed outside of the training context. Although prior studies indicate that multiwhisker stimuli drive robust subthreshold activity, we observed sparse activation of L2/3 pyramidal (Pyr) neurons in both control and trained mice. Despite evidence for excitatory synaptic strengthening at thalamocortical and intracortical synapses in this brain area at the onset of learning-indeed, under our imaging conditions thalamocortical axons were robustly activated-we observed that L2/3 Pyr neurons in somatosensory (barrel) cortex displayed only modest increases in stimulus-evoked activity that were concentrated at the onset of training. Activity renormalized over longer training periods. In contrast, when stimuli and rewards were uncoupled in a pseudotraining paradigm, stimulus-evoked activity in L2/3 Pyr neurons was significantly suppressed. These findings indicate that sensory-association training but not sensory stimulation without coupled rewards may briefly enhance sensory-evoked activity, a phenomenon that might help link sensory input to behavioral outcomes at the onset of learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11261211/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141492472","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Repeated stimulation of feeding mechanoafferents in Aplysia generates responses consistent with the release of food. 反复刺激水蚤的摄食机械感受器会产生与食物释放一致的反应。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Print Date: 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053880.123
Itay Hurwitz, Shlomit Tam, Jian Jing, Hillel J Chiel, Abraham J Susswein

How does repeated stimulation of mechanoafferents affect feeding motor neurons? Monosynaptic connections from a mechanoafferent population in the Aplysia buccal ganglia to five motor followers with different functions were examined during repeated stimulus trains. The mechanoafferents produced both fast and slow synaptic outputs, which could be excitatory or inhibitory. In contrast, other Aplysia mechanoafferents produce only fast excitation on their followers. In addition, patterns of synaptic connections were different to the different motor followers. Some followers received both fast excitation and fast inhibition, whereas others received exclusively fast excitation. All followers showed strong decreases in fast postsynaptic potential (PSP) amplitude within a stimulus train. Fast and slow synaptic connections were of net opposite signs in some followers but not in others. For one follower, synaptic contacts were not uniform from all subareas of the mechanoafferent cluster. Differences in properties of the buccal ganglia mechanoafferents and other Aplysia mechanoafferents may arise because the buccal ganglia neurons innervate the interior of the feeding apparatus, rather than an external surface, and connect to motor neurons for muscles with different motor functions. Fast connection patterns suggest that these synapses may be activated when food slips, biasing the musculature to release food. The largest slow inhibitory synaptic PSPs may contribute to a delay in the onset of the next behavior. Additional functions are also possible.

重复刺激机械传感如何影响摄食运动神经元?在重复刺激列车期间,研究人员考察了从臀足类颊神经节中的机械传入群到五个具有不同功能的运动跟随器的单突触连接。这些机械传入神经产生快速和慢速突触输出,可能是兴奋性的,也可能是抑制性的。相比之下,其他的臀足类机械传感只对其从动件产生快速兴奋。此外,不同运动追随器的突触连接模式也不同。一些追随器同时接受快速兴奋和快速抑制,而另一些则只接受快速兴奋。在一列刺激中,所有跟随者的快速突触后电位(PSP)振幅都出现了强烈的下降。在一些跟随者中,快速和慢速突触连接的净符号相反,而在另一些跟随者中则不然。对于一个跟随者,机械感受器簇所有亚区的突触联系并不一致。颊神经节机械感受器与其他蜻蜓动物机械感受器的特性不同,可能是因为颊神经节神经元支配摄食装置的内部而非外部,并与具有不同运动功能的肌肉的运动神经元相连。快速连接模式表明,当食物滑落时,这些突触可能会被激活,使肌肉组织释放食物。最大的慢速抑制性突触 PSP 可能有助于延迟下一个行为的发生。其他功能也是可能的。
{"title":"Repeated stimulation of feeding mechanoafferents in <i>Aplysia</i> generates responses consistent with the release of food.","authors":"Itay Hurwitz, Shlomit Tam, Jian Jing, Hillel J Chiel, Abraham J Susswein","doi":"10.1101/lm.053880.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053880.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How does repeated stimulation of mechanoafferents affect feeding motor neurons? Monosynaptic connections from a mechanoafferent population in the <i>Aplysia</i> buccal ganglia to five motor followers with different functions were examined during repeated stimulus trains. The mechanoafferents produced both fast and slow synaptic outputs, which could be excitatory or inhibitory. In contrast, other <i>Aplysia</i> mechanoafferents produce only fast excitation on their followers. In addition, patterns of synaptic connections were different to the different motor followers. Some followers received both fast excitation and fast inhibition, whereas others received exclusively fast excitation. All followers showed strong decreases in fast postsynaptic potential (PSP) amplitude within a stimulus train. Fast and slow synaptic connections were of net opposite signs in some followers but not in others. For one follower, synaptic contacts were not uniform from all subareas of the mechanoafferent cluster. Differences in properties of the buccal ganglia mechanoafferents and other <i>Aplysia</i> mechanoafferents may arise because the buccal ganglia neurons innervate the interior of the feeding apparatus, rather than an external surface, and connect to motor neurons for muscles with different motor functions. Fast connection patterns suggest that these synapses may be activated when food slips, biasing the musculature to release food. The largest slow inhibitory synaptic PSPs may contribute to a delay in the onset of the next behavior. Additional functions are also possible.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11261209/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141476911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multiple changes in connectivity between buccal ganglia mechanoafferents and motor neurons with different functions after learning that food is inedible in Aplysia. 学习到食物不可食用后,颊神经节机械传感与具有不同功能的运动神经元之间的连接发生了多重变化。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2024-07-01 Print Date: 2024-06-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053882.123
Itay Hurwitz, Shlomit Tam, Jian Jing, Hillel J Chiel, Jeffrey Gill, Abraham J Susswein

Changes caused by learning that a food is inedible in Aplysia were examined for fast and slow synaptic connections from the buccal ganglia S1 cluster of mechanoafferents to five followers, in response to repeated stimulus trains. Learning affected only fast connections. For these, unique patterns of change were present in each follower, indicating that learning differentially affects the different branches of the mechanoafferents to their followers. In some followers, there were increases in either excitatory or inhibitory connections, and in others, there were decreases. Changes in connectivity resulted from changes in the amplitude of excitation or inhibition, or as a result of the number of connections, or of both. Some followers also exhibited changes in either within or between stimulus train plasticity as a result of learning. In one follower, changes differed from the different areas of the S1 cluster. The patterns of changes in connectivity were consistent with the behavioral changes produced by learning, in that they would produce an increase in the bias to reject or to release food, and a decrease in the likelihood to respond to food.

研究了从颊神经节 S1 机械感受器群到五个追随器的快速和慢速突触连接对重复刺激序列的反应,以检验学习食物不可食用对plysia 所造成的变化。学习只影响快速连接。对于这些连接,每个从动者都有独特的变化模式,这表明学习对机械触动到其从动者的不同分支产生了不同的影响。在一些跟随者中,兴奋性或抑制性连接增加,而在另一些跟随者中,兴奋性或抑制性连接减少。连通性的变化源于兴奋或抑制幅度的变化,或源于连通性数量的变化,或源于两者的变化。一些追随者还表现出由于学习而导致的刺激序列内或刺激序列间可塑性的变化。在一个追随者中,S1 群组不同区域的变化各不相同。连通性的变化模式与学习产生的行为变化是一致的,即它们会增加拒绝或释放食物的偏好,并降低对食物做出反应的可能性。
{"title":"Multiple changes in connectivity between buccal ganglia mechanoafferents and motor neurons with different functions after learning that food is inedible in <i>Aplysia</i>.","authors":"Itay Hurwitz, Shlomit Tam, Jian Jing, Hillel J Chiel, Jeffrey Gill, Abraham J Susswein","doi":"10.1101/lm.053882.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053882.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Changes caused by learning that a food is inedible in <i>Aplysia</i> were examined for fast and slow synaptic connections from the buccal ganglia S1 cluster of mechanoafferents to five followers, in response to repeated stimulus trains. Learning affected only fast connections. For these, unique patterns of change were present in each follower, indicating that learning differentially affects the different branches of the mechanoafferents to their followers. In some followers, there were increases in either excitatory or inhibitory connections, and in others, there were decreases. Changes in connectivity resulted from changes in the amplitude of excitation or inhibition, or as a result of the number of connections, or of both. Some followers also exhibited changes in either within or between stimulus train plasticity as a result of learning. In one follower, changes differed from the different areas of the S1 cluster. The patterns of changes in connectivity were consistent with the behavioral changes produced by learning, in that they would produce an increase in the bias to reject or to release food, and a decrease in the likelihood to respond to food.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11261210/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141476910","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1