首页 > 最新文献

Learning & memory最新文献

英文 中文
Elevated corticosterone after fear learning impairs remote auditory memory retrieval and alters brain network connectivity. 恐惧学习后皮质酮升高会损害远程听觉记忆检索并改变大脑网络连接。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-07-24 Print Date: 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053836.123
Niek Brosens, Sylvie L Lesuis, Ilse Bassie, Lara Reyes, Priya Gajadien, Paul J Lucassen, Harm J Krugers

Glucocorticoids are potent memory modulators that can modify behavior in an adaptive or maladaptive manner. Elevated glucocorticoid levels after learning promote memory consolidation at recent time points, but their effects on remote time points are not well established. Here we set out to assess whether corticosterone (CORT) given after learning modifies remote fear memory. To that end, mice were exposed to a mild auditory fear conditioning paradigm followed by a single 2 mg/kg CORT injection, and after 28 d, auditory memory was assessed. Neuronal activation was investigated using immunohistochemistry for the immediate early gene c-Fos, and coactivation of brain regions was determined using a correlation matrix analysis. CORT-treated mice displayed significantly less remote auditory memory retrieval. While the net activity of studied brain regions was similar compared with the control condition, CORT-induced remote memory impairment was associated with altered correlated activity between brain regions. Specifically, connectivity of the lateral amygdala with the basal amygdala and the dorsal dentate gyrus was significantly reduced in CORT-treated mice, suggesting disrupted network connectivity that may underlie diminished remote memory retrieval. Elucidating the pathways underlying these effects could help provide mechanistic insight into the effects of stress on memory and possibly provide therapeutic targets for psychopathology.

糖皮质激素是一种强大的记忆调节剂,可以以适应或不适应的方式改变行为。学习后糖皮质激素水平的升高促进了最近时间点的记忆巩固,但它们对远程时间点的影响尚不明确。在这里,我们开始评估学习后给予皮质酮(CORT)是否会改变远程恐惧记忆。为此,将小鼠暴露于轻度听觉恐惧条件反射模式,然后单次注射2mg/kg CORT,28天后,评估听觉记忆。使用免疫组织化学方法研究即刻早期基因c-Fos的神经元激活,并使用相关矩阵分析确定大脑区域的共激活。CORT处理的小鼠表现出明显较少的远程听觉记忆检索。虽然与对照条件相比,所研究的大脑区域的净活动相似,但CORT诱导的远程记忆损伤与大脑区域之间的相关活动改变有关。具体而言,在CORT治疗的小鼠中,外侧杏仁核与基底杏仁核和齿状回的连接显著降低,这表明网络连接中断,这可能是远程记忆检索减少的原因。阐明这些影响的潜在途径有助于从机制上深入了解压力对记忆的影响,并可能为精神病理学提供治疗靶点。
{"title":"Elevated corticosterone after fear learning impairs remote auditory memory retrieval and alters brain network connectivity.","authors":"Niek Brosens, Sylvie L Lesuis, Ilse Bassie, Lara Reyes, Priya Gajadien, Paul J Lucassen, Harm J Krugers","doi":"10.1101/lm.053836.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053836.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Glucocorticoids are potent memory modulators that can modify behavior in an adaptive or maladaptive manner. Elevated glucocorticoid levels after learning promote memory consolidation at recent time points, but their effects on remote time points are not well established. Here we set out to assess whether corticosterone (CORT) given after learning modifies remote fear memory. To that end, mice were exposed to a mild auditory fear conditioning paradigm followed by a single 2 mg/kg CORT injection, and after 28 d, auditory memory was assessed. Neuronal activation was investigated using immunohistochemistry for the immediate early gene <i>c</i>-<i>Fos</i>, and coactivation of brain regions was determined using a correlation matrix analysis. CORT-treated mice displayed significantly less remote auditory memory retrieval. While the net activity of studied brain regions was similar compared with the control condition, CORT-induced remote memory impairment was associated with altered correlated activity between brain regions. Specifically, connectivity of the lateral amygdala with the basal amygdala and the dorsal dentate gyrus was significantly reduced in CORT-treated mice, suggesting disrupted network connectivity that may underlie diminished remote memory retrieval. Elucidating the pathways underlying these effects could help provide mechanistic insight into the effects of stress on memory and possibly provide therapeutic targets for psychopathology.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10519398/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10244687","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distribution, cellular localization, and colocalization of several peptide neurotransmitters in the central nervous system of Aplysia. 几种多肽神经递质在水蚤中枢神经系统中的分布、细胞定位和共定位。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-13 Print Date: 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053758.123
Robert D Hawkins, Lennart Brodin, Elvar Theodorsson, Ákos Végvári, Eric R Kandel, Tomas Hokfelt

Neuropeptides are widely used as neurotransmitters in vertebrates and invertebrates. In vertebrates, a detailed understanding of their functions as transmitters has been hampered by the complexity of the nervous system. The marine mollusk Aplysia, with a simpler nervous system and many large, identified neurons, presents several advantages for addressing this question and has been used to examine the roles of tens of peptides in behavior. To screen for other peptides that might also play roles in behavior, we observed immunoreactivity in individual neurons in the central nervous system of adult Aplysia with antisera raised against the Aplysia peptide FMRFamide and two mammalian peptides that are also found in Aplysia, cholecystokinin (CCK) and neuropeptide Y (NPY), as well as serotonin (5HT). In addition, we observed staining of individual neurons with antisera raised against mammalian somatostatin (SOM) and peptide histidine isoleucine (PHI). However, genomic analysis has shown that these two peptides are not expressed in the Aplysia nervous system, and we have therefore labeled the unknown peptides stained by these two antibodies as XSOM and XPHI There was an area at the anterior end of the cerebral ganglion that had staining by antisera raised against many different transmitters, suggesting that this may be a modulatory region of the nervous system. There was also staining for XSOM and, in some cases, FMRFamide in the bag cell cluster of the abdominal ganglion. In addition, these and other studies have revealed a fairly high degree of colocalization of different neuropeptides in individual neurons, suggesting that the peptides do not just act independently but can also interact in different combinations to produce complex functions. The simple nervous system of Aplysia is advantageous for further testing these ideas.

神经肽被脊椎动物和无脊椎动物广泛用作神经递质。在脊椎动物中,神经系统的复杂性阻碍了对神经肽作为递质的功能的详细了解。海洋软体动物plysia 的神经系统较为简单,而且有许多大型的、可识别的神经元,这为解决这一问题提供了多项优势,并已被用于研究数十种肽在行为中的作用。为了筛选可能在行为中也起作用的其他肽,我们用针对水蚤肽FMRFamide和两种哺乳动物肽--胆囊收缩素(CCK)和神经肽Y(NPY)以及5-羟色胺(5HT)的抗血清在成年水蚤中枢神经系统的单个神经元中观察免疫反应。此外,我们还观察到个别神经元被针对哺乳动物体生长抑素(SOM)和组氨酸异亮氨酸肽(PHI)的抗血清染色。然而,基因组分析表明,这两种肽并不表达于板蓝根神经系统,因此我们将这两种抗体染色的未知肽标记为XSOM和XPHI。在大脑神经节的前端,有一个区域被针对多种不同递质的抗血清染色,表明这可能是神经系统的一个调节区域。腹神经节的袋细胞簇也有 XSOM 染色,在某些情况下还有 FMRFamide 染色。此外,这些研究和其他研究还发现,不同的神经肽在单个神经元中的共定位程度相当高,这表明这些肽不仅能独立发挥作用,还能以不同的组合相互作用,产生复杂的功能。简单的水蚤神经系统有利于进一步检验这些观点。
{"title":"Distribution, cellular localization, and colocalization of several peptide neurotransmitters in the central nervous system of <i>Aplysia</i>.","authors":"Robert D Hawkins, Lennart Brodin, Elvar Theodorsson, Ákos Végvári, Eric R Kandel, Tomas Hokfelt","doi":"10.1101/lm.053758.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053758.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Neuropeptides are widely used as neurotransmitters in vertebrates and invertebrates. In vertebrates, a detailed understanding of their functions as transmitters has been hampered by the complexity of the nervous system. The marine mollusk <i>Aplysia</i>, with a simpler nervous system and many large, identified neurons, presents several advantages for addressing this question and has been used to examine the roles of tens of peptides in behavior. To screen for other peptides that might also play roles in behavior, we observed immunoreactivity in individual neurons in the central nervous system of adult <i>Aplysia</i> with antisera raised against the <i>Aplysia</i> peptide FMRFamide and two mammalian peptides that are also found in <i>Aplysia</i>, cholecystokinin (CCK) and neuropeptide Y (NPY), as well as serotonin (5HT). In addition, we observed staining of individual neurons with antisera raised against mammalian somatostatin (SOM) and peptide histidine isoleucine (PHI). However, genomic analysis has shown that these two peptides are not expressed in the <i>Aplysia</i> nervous system, and we have therefore labeled the unknown peptides stained by these two antibodies as X<sub>SOM</sub> and X<sub>PHI</sub> There was an area at the anterior end of the cerebral ganglion that had staining by antisera raised against many different transmitters, suggesting that this may be a modulatory region of the nervous system. There was also staining for X<sub>SOM</sub> and, in some cases, FMRFamide in the bag cell cluster of the abdominal ganglion. In addition, these and other studies have revealed a fairly high degree of colocalization of different neuropeptides in individual neurons, suggesting that the peptides do not just act independently but can also interact in different combinations to produce complex functions. The simple nervous system of <i>Aplysia</i> is advantageous for further testing these ideas.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10353257/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9836040","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pattern separation of fear extinction memory. 恐惧消退记忆的模式分离
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-11 Print Date: 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053760.123
Patrick A F Laing, Joseph E Dunsmoor

While fear generalizes widely, extinction is stimulus-specific. Using a hybrid conditioning/episodic memory paradigm, subjects encoded nonrepeating category exemplars during fear conditioning and extinction. Twenty-four hours later, a surprise memory test included old, similar, and novel category exemplars. Results showed strong dissociation between pattern completion (generalization) and pattern separation (discrimination) in episodic memory for items encoded during fear conditioning versus extinction, respectively. These data suggest that directly threat-conditioned stimuli are better recognized at the expense of mnemonic precision, whereas discrimination is enhanced for extinguished stimuli. Overly precise extinction memory may be a contributing factor to fear relapse.

恐惧的泛化范围很广,而消退则是针对特定刺激的。受试者在恐惧条件反射和消退过程中,使用混合条件反射/记忆范式对非重复类别范例进行编码。二十四小时后,突击记忆测试包括旧的、相似的和新的类别范例。结果显示,对于在恐惧条件反射和消退过程中编码的项目,外显记忆中的模式完成(泛化)和模式分离(辨别)之间存在很强的分离。这些数据表明,以牺牲记忆的精确性为代价,直接受威胁条件反射的刺激物能被更好地识别,而对消退刺激物的辨别能力则会增强。过于精确的消退记忆可能是导致恐惧复发的一个因素。
{"title":"Pattern separation of fear extinction memory.","authors":"Patrick A F Laing, Joseph E Dunsmoor","doi":"10.1101/lm.053760.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053760.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While fear generalizes widely, extinction is stimulus-specific. Using a hybrid conditioning/episodic memory paradigm, subjects encoded nonrepeating category exemplars during fear conditioning and extinction. Twenty-four hours later, a surprise memory test included old, similar, and novel category exemplars. Results showed strong dissociation between pattern completion (generalization) and pattern separation (discrimination) in episodic memory for items encoded during fear conditioning versus extinction, respectively. These data suggest that directly threat-conditioned stimuli are better recognized at the expense of mnemonic precision, whereas discrimination is enhanced for extinguished stimuli. Overly precise extinction memory may be a contributing factor to fear relapse.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10353259/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9826242","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence that anterograde learning interference depends on the stage of learning of the interferer: blocked versus interleaved training. 前向学习干扰取决于干扰者学习阶段的证据:阻塞与交错训练。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-07 Print Date: 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053710.122
Ruijing Ning, Beverly A Wright

Training on one task (task A) can disrupt learning on a subsequently trained task (task B), illustrating anterograde learning interference. We asked whether the induction of anterograde learning interference depends on the learning stage that task A has reached when the training on task B begins. To do so, we drew on previous observations in perceptual learning in which completing all training on one task before beginning training on another task (blocked training) yielded markedly different learning outcomes than alternating training between the same two tasks for the same total number of trials (interleaved training). Those blocked versus interleaved contrasts suggest that there is a transition between two differentially vulnerable learning stages that is related to the number of consecutive training trials on each task, with interleaved training presumably tapping acquisition, and blocked training tapping consolidation. Here, we used the blocked versus interleaved paradigm in auditory perceptual learning in a case in which blocked training generated anterograde-but not its converse, retrograde-learning interference (A→B, not B←A). We report that anterograde learning interference of training on task A (interaural time difference discrimination) on learning on task B (interaural level difference discrimination) occurred with blocked training and diminished with interleaved training, with faster rates of interleaving leading to less interference. This pattern held for across-day, within-session, and offline learning. Thus, anterograde learning interference only occurred when the number of consecutive training trials on task A surpassed some critical value, consistent with other recent evidence that anterograde learning interference only arises when learning on task A has entered the consolidation stage.

对一项任务(任务 A)的训练会干扰对随后训练的任务(任务 B)的学习,这就是逆向学习干扰。我们想知道,逆向学习干扰的诱发是否取决于任务 A 在任务 B 开始训练时所达到的学习阶段。为此,我们借鉴了之前在知觉学习中的观察结果,即在开始另一项任务的训练之前完成一项任务的所有训练(阻断训练)与在相同的试验总数下交替进行同两项任务的训练(交错训练)所产生的学习结果明显不同。阻断训练与交错训练的对比表明,在两个不同的易受伤害的学习阶段之间存在着一个过渡阶段,这与每个任务的连续训练次数有关,交错训练可能是为了习得,而阻断训练则是为了巩固。在这里,我们在听觉知觉学习中使用了阻断与交错范式,在这种情况下,阻断训练会产生逆向学习干扰(A→B,而不是B←A),而逆向学习干扰不会产生。我们报告说,任务A(耳间时差辨别)的训练对任务B(耳间电平差辨别)的学习的逆向学习干扰在阻塞训练中出现,在交错训练中减弱,交错速度越快干扰越小。这种模式在跨日、会话和离线学习中都是如此。因此,只有当任务 A 的连续训练次数超过某个临界值时,才会出现逆向学习干扰,这与最近的其他证据一致,即只有当任务 A 的学习进入巩固阶段时,才会出现逆向学习干扰。
{"title":"Evidence that anterograde learning interference depends on the stage of learning of the interferer: blocked versus interleaved training.","authors":"Ruijing Ning, Beverly A Wright","doi":"10.1101/lm.053710.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053710.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Training on one task (task A) can disrupt learning on a subsequently trained task (task B), illustrating anterograde learning interference. We asked whether the induction of anterograde learning interference depends on the learning stage that task A has reached when the training on task B begins. To do so, we drew on previous observations in perceptual learning in which completing all training on one task before beginning training on another task (blocked training) yielded markedly different learning outcomes than alternating training between the same two tasks for the same total number of trials (interleaved training). Those blocked versus interleaved contrasts suggest that there is a transition between two differentially vulnerable learning stages that is related to the number of consecutive training trials on each task, with interleaved training presumably tapping acquisition, and blocked training tapping consolidation. Here, we used the blocked versus interleaved paradigm in auditory perceptual learning in a case in which blocked training generated anterograde-but not its converse, retrograde-learning interference (A→B, not B←A). We report that anterograde learning interference of training on task A (interaural time difference discrimination) on learning on task B (interaural level difference discrimination) occurred with blocked training and diminished with interleaved training, with faster rates of interleaving leading to less interference. This pattern held for across-day, within-session, and offline learning. Thus, anterograde learning interference only occurred when the number of consecutive training trials on task A surpassed some critical value, consistent with other recent evidence that anterograde learning interference only arises when learning on task A has entered the consolidation stage.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-07-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10353258/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10193700","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Self-reported encoding quality promotes lure rejections and false alarms. 自我报告的编码质量会导致诱饵被拒绝和误报。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-05-04 Print Date: 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053751.123
Christopher N Wahlheim, Sydney M Garlitch, Rawan M Mohamed, Blaire J Weidler

The hippocampus supports distinctive encoding, enabling discrimination of perceptions from similar memories. Here, an experimental and individual differences approach examined the role of encoding quality in the classification of similar lures. An object recognition task included thought probes during study and similar lures at test. On-task study reports were associated with lure discrimination in within-subject and between-subjects analyses. Within-subject on-task reports were also associated with false classifications of lures as studied objects. These results are compatible with the view that quality encoding supports memory-based rejection of lures but also engenders false alarms when perceptions and memories are inaccurately compared.

海马支持独特编码,从而能够从相似记忆中分辨出感知。本文采用实验和个体差异的方法研究了编码质量在相似诱饵分类中的作用。物体识别任务包括学习时的思维探针和测试时的相似诱饵。在主体内和主体间分析中,任务学习报告与诱饵辨别相关。主体内的任务报告也与将诱饵错误分类为研究对象有关。这些结果与以下观点相吻合:质量编码支持基于记忆的对诱饵的排斥,但当感知与记忆不准确比较时,也会产生错误警报。
{"title":"Self-reported encoding quality promotes lure rejections and false alarms.","authors":"Christopher N Wahlheim, Sydney M Garlitch, Rawan M Mohamed, Blaire J Weidler","doi":"10.1101/lm.053751.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053751.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The hippocampus supports distinctive encoding, enabling discrimination of perceptions from similar memories. Here, an experimental and individual differences approach examined the role of encoding quality in the classification of similar lures. An object recognition task included thought probes during study and similar lures at test. On-task study reports were associated with lure discrimination in within-subject and between-subjects analyses. Within-subject on-task reports were also associated with false classifications of lures as studied objects. These results are compatible with the view that quality encoding supports memory-based rejection of lures but also engenders false alarms when perceptions and memories are inaccurately compared.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10165994/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9555096","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Corrigendum: Depotentiation depends on IP3 receptor activation sustained by synaptic inputs after LTP induction. 勘误:减弱依赖于LTP诱导后突触输入维持的IP3受体激活。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-05-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053796.123
Satoshi Fujii, Yoshihiko Yamazaki, Jun-Ichi Goto, Hiroki Fujiwara, Katsuhiko Mikoshiba
{"title":"Corrigendum: Depotentiation depends on IP<sub>3</sub> receptor activation sustained by synaptic inputs after LTP induction.","authors":"Satoshi Fujii,&nbsp;Yoshihiko Yamazaki,&nbsp;Jun-Ichi Goto,&nbsp;Hiroki Fujiwara,&nbsp;Katsuhiko Mikoshiba","doi":"10.1101/lm.053796.123","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053796.123","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10353256/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9891055","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Standardized extract of Ginkgo biloba treatment and novelty on the weak encoding of spatial recognition memory in rats. 银杏叶标准化提取物治疗和新奇感对大鼠空间识别记忆弱编码的影响
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-18 Print Date: 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053755.123
Carla Vitor de Andrade, Andressa Gabriela Soliani, Suzete Maria Cerutti

Long-term memory (LTM) formation is dependent on neurochemical changes that guarantee that a recently formed memory (short-term memory [STM]) remains in the specific neural circuitry via the consolidation process. The persistence of recognition memory has been evidenced by using behavioral tagging in young adult rats, but it has not been effective on aging. Here, we investigated the effects of treatment with a standardized extract of Ginkgo biloba (EGb) associated with novelty on the consolidation of object location memory (OLM) and its persistence after weak training of spatial object preference in young adult and aged rats. The object location task used in this study included two habituation sessions, training sessions associated or not associated with EGb treatment and contextual novelty, and short-term or long-term retention testing sessions. Altogether, our data showed that treatment with EGb associated with novelty close to the time of encoding resulted in STM that lasted for 1 h and persisted for 24 h for both young adult and aged rats. In aged rats, the cooperative mechanisms induced robust long-term OLM. Our findings support and extend our knowledge about recognition memory in aged rats and the modulating effects of EGb treatment and contextual novelty on the persistence of memory.

长期记忆(LTM)的形成依赖于神经化学变化,这种变化保证了最近形成的记忆(短期记忆 [STM])通过巩固过程留在特定的神经回路中。识别记忆的持久性已通过对年轻成年大鼠的行为标记得到证明,但对衰老却没有效果。在此,我们研究了与新奇感相关的银杏叶标准化提取物(EGb)治疗对年轻成年大鼠和老年大鼠的物体位置记忆(OLM)巩固及其在空间物体偏好弱训练后的持续性的影响。本研究中使用的物体定位任务包括两次习惯性训练、与 EGb 治疗和情境新奇性相关或不相关的训练以及短期或长期保持测试。总之,我们的数据表明,在接近编码时间时使用与新奇事物相关的EGb会导致年轻成年大鼠和老年大鼠的STM持续1小时并持续24小时。在老年大鼠中,合作机制诱导了强有力的长期 OLM。我们的研究结果支持并扩展了我们对老年大鼠识别记忆以及 EGb 处理和情境新奇性对记忆持续性的调节作用的认识。
{"title":"Standardized extract of <i>Ginkgo biloba</i> treatment and novelty on the weak encoding of spatial recognition memory in rats.","authors":"Carla Vitor de Andrade, Andressa Gabriela Soliani, Suzete Maria Cerutti","doi":"10.1101/lm.053755.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053755.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Long-term memory (LTM) formation is dependent on neurochemical changes that guarantee that a recently formed memory (short-term memory [STM]) remains in the specific neural circuitry via the consolidation process. The persistence of recognition memory has been evidenced by using behavioral tagging in young adult rats, but it has not been effective on aging. Here, we investigated the effects of treatment with a standardized extract of <i>Ginkgo biloba</i> (EGb) associated with novelty on the consolidation of object location memory (OLM) and its persistence after weak training of spatial object preference in young adult and aged rats. The object location task used in this study included two habituation sessions, training sessions associated or not associated with EGb treatment and contextual novelty, and short-term or long-term retention testing sessions. Altogether, our data showed that treatment with EGb associated with novelty close to the time of encoding resulted in STM that lasted for 1 h and persisted for 24 h for both young adult and aged rats. In aged rats, the cooperative mechanisms induced robust long-term OLM. Our findings support and extend our knowledge about recognition memory in aged rats and the modulating effects of EGb treatment and contextual novelty on the persistence of memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10165992/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9494805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Specific behaviors during auditory fear conditioning and postsynaptic expression of AMPA receptors in the basolateral amygdala predict interindividual differences in fear generalization in male rats. 听觉恐惧条件反射中的特定行为和杏仁核基底外侧AMPA受体的突触后表达预测雄性大鼠恐惧泛化的个体间差异。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053612.122
Bruno José Moraes, Oliver Hardt

Auditory fear conditioning in rats is a widely used method to study learning, memory, and emotional responding. Despite procedural standardizations and optimizations, there is substantial interindividual variability in fear expression during test, notably in terms of fear expressed toward the testing context alone. To better understand which factors could explain this variation between subjects, we here explored whether behavior during training and expression of AMPA receptors (AMPARs) after long-term memory formation in the amygdala could predict freezing during test. We studied outbred male rats and found strong variation in fear generalization to a different context. Hierarchical clustering of these data identified two distinct groups of subjects that independently correlated with a specific pattern of behaviors expressed during initial training (i.e., rearing and freezing). The extent of fear generalization correlated positively with postsynaptic expression of GluA1-containing AMPA receptors in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala. Our data thus identify candidate behavioral and molecular predictors of fear generalization that may inform our understanding of some anxiety-related disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), that are characterized by overgeneralized fear.

大鼠的听觉恐惧条件反射是一种广泛应用于研究学习、记忆和情绪反应的方法。尽管程序标准化和优化,但在测试过程中,恐惧表达存在实质性的个体间差异,特别是在单独针对测试环境表达的恐惧方面。为了更好地理解哪些因素可以解释受试者之间的这种差异,我们在这里探讨了训练期间的行为和杏仁核长期记忆形成后AMPA受体(ampar)的表达是否可以预测测试期间的冻结。我们研究了近亲繁殖的雄性大鼠,发现在不同的环境下,恐惧泛化的差异很大。这些数据的分层聚类确定了两组不同的受试者,它们独立地与初始训练期间表达的特定行为模式相关(即饲养和冻结)。恐惧泛化程度与杏仁核基底外侧含有glua1的AMPA受体突触后表达呈正相关。因此,我们的数据确定了恐惧泛化的候选行为和分子预测因子,这可能会告诉我们对一些焦虑相关疾病的理解,例如以过度泛化恐惧为特征的创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)。
{"title":"Specific behaviors during auditory fear conditioning and postsynaptic expression of AMPA receptors in the basolateral amygdala predict interindividual differences in fear generalization in male rats.","authors":"Bruno José Moraes,&nbsp;Oliver Hardt","doi":"10.1101/lm.053612.122","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1101/lm.053612.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Auditory fear conditioning in rats is a widely used method to study learning, memory, and emotional responding. Despite procedural standardizations and optimizations, there is substantial interindividual variability in fear expression during test, notably in terms of fear expressed toward the testing context alone. To better understand which factors could explain this variation between subjects, we here explored whether behavior during training and expression of AMPA receptors (AMPARs) after long-term memory formation in the amygdala could predict freezing during test. We studied outbred male rats and found strong variation in fear generalization to a different context. Hierarchical clustering of these data identified two distinct groups of subjects that independently correlated with a specific pattern of behaviors expressed during initial training (i.e., rearing and freezing). The extent of fear generalization correlated positively with postsynaptic expression of GluA1-containing AMPA receptors in the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala. Our data thus identify candidate behavioral and molecular predictors of fear generalization that may inform our understanding of some anxiety-related disorders, such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), that are characterized by overgeneralized fear.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/87/2a/LM053612Mor.PMC10165993.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9503484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Differential effects of emotional valence on mnemonic performance with greater hippocampal maturity. 海马成熟度越高,情绪情感对记忆力表现的影响越不同
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-03-15 Print Date: 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053628.122
Adam Kimbler, Dana L McMakin, Nicholas J Tustison, Aaron T Mattfeld

The hippocampal formation (HF) facilitates declarative memory, with subfields providing unique contributions to memory performance. Maturational differences across subfields facilitate a shift toward increased memory specificity, with peripuberty sitting at the inflection point. Peripuberty is also a sensitive period in the development of anxiety disorders. We believe HF development during puberty is critical to negative overgeneralization, a common feature of anxiety disorders. To investigate this claim, we examined the relationship between mnemonic generalization and a cross-sectional pubertal maturity index (PMI) derived from partial least squares correlation (PLSC) analyses of subfield volumes and structural connectivity from T1-weighted and diffusion-weighted scans, respectively. Participants aged 9-14 yr, from clinical and community sources, performed a recognition task with emotionally valent (positive, negative, and neutral) images. HF volumetric PMI was positively associated with generalization for negative images. Hippocampal-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity PMI evidenced a behavioral relationship similar to that of the HF volumetric approach. These findings reflect a novel developmentally related balance between generalization behavior supported by the hippocampus and its connections with other regions, with maturational differences in this balance potentially contributing to negative overgeneralization during peripuberty.

海马体形成(HF)促进了陈述性记忆,其子领域为记忆表现做出了独特的贡献。各子领域之间的成熟差异促进了记忆特异性的增强,而青春期正处于拐点。青春期也是焦虑症发展的敏感期。我们认为,青春期的高频发展对消极过度概括至关重要,而消极过度概括是焦虑症的常见特征。为了探究这一观点,我们研究了记忆泛化与横断面青春期成熟指数(PMI)之间的关系,该指数分别来自对T1加权扫描和扩散加权扫描的子场体积和结构连接性的偏最小二乘法相关性分析。来自临床和社区的9-14岁的参与者进行了一项识别任务,识别的图像为情绪性图像(正面、负面和中性)。高频容积PMI与负面图像的泛化呈正相关。海马-内侧前额叶皮层连接PMI与高频容积法的行为关系相似。这些发现反映出海马支持的泛化行为与海马与其他区域的连接之间存在一种与发育相关的新平衡,这种平衡的成熟差异可能会导致围青春期的消极过度泛化。
{"title":"Differential effects of emotional valence on mnemonic performance with greater hippocampal maturity.","authors":"Adam Kimbler, Dana L McMakin, Nicholas J Tustison, Aaron T Mattfeld","doi":"10.1101/lm.053628.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053628.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The hippocampal formation (HF) facilitates declarative memory, with subfields providing unique contributions to memory performance. Maturational differences across subfields facilitate a shift toward increased memory specificity, with peripuberty sitting at the inflection point. Peripuberty is also a sensitive period in the development of anxiety disorders. We believe HF development during puberty is critical to negative overgeneralization, a common feature of anxiety disorders. To investigate this claim, we examined the relationship between mnemonic generalization and a cross-sectional pubertal maturity index (PMI) derived from partial least squares correlation (PLSC) analyses of subfield volumes and structural connectivity from T1-weighted and diffusion-weighted scans, respectively. Participants aged 9-14 yr, from clinical and community sources, performed a recognition task with emotionally valent (positive, negative, and neutral) images. HF volumetric PMI was positively associated with generalization for negative images. Hippocampal-medial prefrontal cortex connectivity PMI evidenced a behavioral relationship similar to that of the HF volumetric approach. These findings reflect a novel developmentally related balance between generalization behavior supported by the hippocampus and its connections with other regions, with maturational differences in this balance potentially contributing to negative overgeneralization during peripuberty.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10027236/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9149369","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sleep disruption by memory cues selectively weakens reactivated memories. 记忆线索对睡眠的干扰会选择性地削弱重新激活的记忆。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-03-15 Print Date: 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053615.122
Nathan W Whitmore, Ken A Paller

A widely accepted view in memory research is that recently stored information can be reactivated during sleep, leading to memory strengthening. Two recent studies have shown that this effect can be reversed in participants with highly disrupted sleep. To test whether weakening of reactivated memories can result directly from sleep disruption, in this experiment we varied the intensity of memory reactivation cues such that some produced sleep arousals. Prior to sleep, participants (local community members) learned the locations of 75 objects, each accompanied by a sound naturally associated with that object. Location recall was tested before and after sleep, and a subset of the sounds was presented during sleep to provoke reactivation of the corresponding locations. Reactivation with sleep arousal weakened memories, unlike the improvement typically found after reactivation without sleep arousal. We conclude that reactivated memories can be selectively weakened during sleep, and that memory reactivation may strengthen or weaken memories depending on additional factors such as concurrent sleep disruption.

记忆研究中一个广为接受的观点是,最近存储的信息可以在睡眠中被重新激活,从而增强记忆。最近的两项研究表明,这种效果在睡眠高度紊乱的参与者身上会发生逆转。为了测试睡眠中断是否会直接导致重新激活记忆的减弱,在本实验中,我们改变了记忆重新激活线索的强度,使一些线索产生睡眠唤醒。在入睡前,参与者(当地社区成员)学习了 75 个物体的位置,每个物体都伴有一种与之自然相关的声音。在睡眠前和睡眠后对位置记忆进行测试,并在睡眠期间播放声音子集,以激发相应位置的再激活。在睡眠唤醒的情况下重新激活记忆会削弱记忆,而在没有睡眠唤醒的情况下重新激活记忆通常会有所改善。我们的结论是,重新激活的记忆会在睡眠中被选择性地削弱,记忆的重新激活可能会增强或削弱记忆,这取决于其他因素,如同时存在的睡眠中断。
{"title":"Sleep disruption by memory cues selectively weakens reactivated memories.","authors":"Nathan W Whitmore, Ken A Paller","doi":"10.1101/lm.053615.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053615.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A widely accepted view in memory research is that recently stored information can be reactivated during sleep, leading to memory strengthening. Two recent studies have shown that this effect can be reversed in participants with highly disrupted sleep. To test whether weakening of reactivated memories can result directly from sleep disruption, in this experiment we varied the intensity of memory reactivation cues such that some produced sleep arousals. Prior to sleep, participants (local community members) learned the locations of 75 objects, each accompanied by a sound naturally associated with that object. Location recall was tested before and after sleep, and a subset of the sounds was presented during sleep to provoke reactivation of the corresponding locations. Reactivation with sleep arousal weakened memories, unlike the improvement typically found after reactivation without sleep arousal. We conclude that reactivated memories can be selectively weakened during sleep, and that memory reactivation may strengthen or weaken memories depending on additional factors such as concurrent sleep disruption.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10027237/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9149370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1