首页 > 最新文献

Learning & memory最新文献

英文 中文
Parsing memory and nonmemory contributions to age-related declines in mnemonic discrimination performance: a hierarchical Bayesian diffusion decision modeling approach. 解析记忆和非记忆对记忆辨别性能与年龄相关的下降的贡献:一种分层贝叶斯扩散决策建模方法。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-03 Print Date: 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053838.123
Caroline Chwiesko, John Janecek, Stephanie Doering, Martina Hollearn, Liv McMillan, Joachim Vandekerckhove, Michael D Lee, Roger Ratcliff, Michael A Yassa

The mnemonic discrimination task (MDT) is a widely used cognitive assessment tool. Performance in this task is believed to indicate an age-related deficit in episodic memory stemming from a decreased ability to pattern-separate among similar experiences. However, cognitive processes other than memory ability might impact task performance. In this study, we investigated whether nonmnemonic decision-making processes contribute to the age-related deficit in the MDT. We applied a hierarchical Bayesian version of the Ratcliff diffusion model to the MDT performance of 26 younger and 31 cognitively normal older adults. It allowed us to decompose decision behavior in the MDT into different underlying cognitive processes, represented by specific model parameters. Model parameters were compared between groups, and differences were evaluated using the Bayes factor. Our results suggest that the age-related decline in MDT performance indicates a predominantly mnemonic deficit rather than differences in nonmnemonic decision-making processes. In addition, this mnemonic deficit might also involve a slowing in processes related to encoding and retrieval strategies, which are relevant for successful memory as well. These findings help to better understand what cognitive processes contribute to the age-related decline in MDT performance and may help to improve the diagnostic value of this popular task.

助记符辨别任务(MDT)是一种广泛使用的认知评估工具。这项任务的表现被认为表明,与年龄相关的情景记忆缺陷源于相似经历中模式分离能力的下降。然而,记忆能力以外的认知过程可能会影响任务表现。在这项研究中,我们调查了非单调决策过程是否会导致MDT中与年龄相关的缺陷。我们将Ratcliff扩散模型的分层贝叶斯版本应用于26名年轻人和31名认知正常的老年人的MDT表现。它使我们能够将MDT中的决策行为分解为不同的底层认知过程,由特定的模型参数表示。比较各组之间的模型参数,并使用贝叶斯因子评估差异。我们的研究结果表明,与年龄相关的MDT表现下降表明,在非记忆决策过程中,记忆缺陷占主导地位,而不是差异。此外,这种助记符缺陷还可能涉及与编码和检索策略相关的过程的减慢,这也与成功的记忆有关。这些发现有助于更好地了解是什么认知过程导致了与年龄相关的MDT表现下降,并可能有助于提高这项流行任务的诊断价值。
{"title":"Parsing memory and nonmemory contributions to age-related declines in mnemonic discrimination performance: a hierarchical Bayesian diffusion decision modeling approach.","authors":"Caroline Chwiesko, John Janecek, Stephanie Doering, Martina Hollearn, Liv McMillan, Joachim Vandekerckhove, Michael D Lee, Roger Ratcliff, Michael A Yassa","doi":"10.1101/lm.053838.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053838.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The mnemonic discrimination task (MDT) is a widely used cognitive assessment tool. Performance in this task is believed to indicate an age-related deficit in episodic memory stemming from a decreased ability to pattern-separate among similar experiences. However, cognitive processes other than memory ability might impact task performance. In this study, we investigated whether nonmnemonic decision-making processes contribute to the age-related deficit in the MDT. We applied a hierarchical Bayesian version of the Ratcliff diffusion model to the MDT performance of 26 younger and 31 cognitively normal older adults. It allowed us to decompose decision behavior in the MDT into different underlying cognitive processes, represented by specific model parameters. Model parameters were compared between groups, and differences were evaluated using the Bayes factor. Our results suggest that the age-related decline in MDT performance indicates a predominantly mnemonic deficit rather than differences in nonmnemonic decision-making processes. In addition, this mnemonic deficit might also involve a slowing in processes related to encoding and retrieval strategies, which are relevant for successful memory as well. These findings help to better understand what cognitive processes contribute to the age-related decline in MDT performance and may help to improve the diagnostic value of this popular task.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631138/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71483155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cue predictiveness and uncertainty determine cue representation during visual statistical learning. 线索的预测性和不确定性决定了视觉统计学习中的线索表示。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-11-03 Print Date: 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053777.123
Puyuan Zhang, Hui Chen, Shelley Xiuli Tong

This study investigated how humans process probabilistic-associated information when encountering varying levels of uncertainty during implicit visual statistical learning. A novel probabilistic cueing validation paradigm was developed to probe the representation of cues with high (75% probability), medium (50%), low (25%), or zero levels of predictiveness in response to preceding targets that appeared with high (75%), medium (50%), or low (25%) transitional probabilities (TPs). Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated a significant negative association between cue probe identification accuracy and cue predictiveness when these cues appeared after high-TP but not medium-TP or low-TP targets, establishing exploration-like cue processing triggered by lower-uncertainty rather than high-uncertainty inputs. Experiment 3 ruled out the confounding factor of probe repetition and extended this finding by demonstrating (1) enhanced representation of low-predictive and zero-predictive but not high-predictive cues across blocks after high-TP targets and (2) enhanced representation of high-predictive but not low-predictive and zero-predictive cues across blocks after low-TP targets for learners who exhibited above-chance awareness of cue-target transition. These results suggest that during implicit statistical learning, input characteristics alter cue-processing mechanisms, such that exploration-like and exploitation-like mechanisms are triggered by lower-uncertainty and higher-uncertainty cue-target sequences, respectively.

这项研究调查了人类在内隐视觉统计学习过程中遇到不同程度的不确定性时如何处理概率相关信息。开发了一种新的概率线索验证范式,以探索具有高(75%概率)、中(50%)、低(25%)或零水平预测性的线索的表示,以响应具有高(75%)、中等(50%)或低(25%)过渡概率(TP)的先前目标。实验1和2表明,当这些线索出现在高TP而不是中TP或低TP目标之后时,线索-探针识别准确性和线索预测性之间存在显著的负相关,建立了由较低不确定性而不是高不确定性输入触发的探索式线索处理。实验3排除了探针重复的混杂因素,并通过证明(1)在高TP目标后跨块增强了低预测性和零预测性但不高预测性线索的表示,以及表现出提示-目标转换的机会意识。这些结果表明,在内隐统计学习过程中,输入特征改变了线索处理机制,使得类似探索和类似开发的机制分别由较低不确定性和较高不确定性的线索-目标序列触发。
{"title":"Cue predictiveness and uncertainty determine cue representation during visual statistical learning.","authors":"Puyuan Zhang, Hui Chen, Shelley Xiuli Tong","doi":"10.1101/lm.053777.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053777.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study investigated how humans process probabilistic-associated information when encountering varying levels of uncertainty during implicit visual statistical learning. A novel probabilistic cueing validation paradigm was developed to probe the representation of cues with high (75% probability), medium (50%), low (25%), or zero levels of predictiveness in response to preceding targets that appeared with high (75%), medium (50%), or low (25%) transitional probabilities (TPs). Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated a significant negative association between cue probe identification accuracy and cue predictiveness when these cues appeared after high-TP but not medium-TP or low-TP targets, establishing exploration-like cue processing triggered by lower-uncertainty rather than high-uncertainty inputs. Experiment 3 ruled out the confounding factor of probe repetition and extended this finding by demonstrating (1) enhanced representation of low-predictive and zero-predictive but not high-predictive cues across blocks after high-TP targets and (2) enhanced representation of high-predictive but not low-predictive and zero-predictive cues across blocks after low-TP targets for learners who exhibited above-chance awareness of cue-target transition. These results suggest that during implicit statistical learning, input characteristics alter cue-processing mechanisms, such that exploration-like and exploitation-like mechanisms are triggered by lower-uncertainty and higher-uncertainty cue-target sequences, respectively.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631146/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71483154","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An in vitro analog of learning that food is inedible in Aplysia: decreased responses to a transmitter signaling food after pairing with transmitters signaling failed swallowing. 一种在体外类似于学习Aplysia中食物不可食用的方法:在与发出吞咽失败信号的发射器配对后,对发出食物信号的发射器的反应减少。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-18 Print Date: 2023-11-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053867.123
Miryam Levy, Jian Jing, Abraham J Susswein

An in vitro analog of learning that a food is inedible provided insight into mechanisms underlying the learning. Aplysia learn to stop responding to a food when they attempt but fail to swallow it. Pairing a cholinergic agonist with an NO donor or histamine in the Aplysia cerebral ganglion produced significant decreases in fictive feeding in response to the cholinergic agonist alone. Acetylcholine (ACh) is the transmitter of chemoreceptors sensing food touching the lips. Nitric oxide (NO) and histamine (HA) signal failed attempts to swallow food. Reduced responses to the cholinergic agonist after pairing with NO or HA indicate that learning partially arises via a decreased response to ACh in the cerebral ganglion.

一种体外模拟学习食物不可食用的方法提供了对学习机制的深入了解。当试图吞咽食物但未能吞咽时,Aplysia学会停止对食物的反应。将胆碱能激动剂与NO供体或Aplysia脑神经节中的组胺配对,可显著减少对胆碱能激动药的模拟进食。乙酰胆碱(ACh)是感知食物触碰嘴唇的化学受体的递质。一氧化氮(NO)和组胺(HA)信号在吞咽食物时失败。与NO或HA配对后对胆碱能激动剂的反应减少表明,学习部分是通过脑神经节中对ACh的反应减少而产生的。
{"title":"An in vitro analog of learning that food is inedible in <i>Aplysia</i>: decreased responses to a transmitter signaling food after pairing with transmitters signaling failed swallowing.","authors":"Miryam Levy, Jian Jing, Abraham J Susswein","doi":"10.1101/lm.053867.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053867.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An in vitro analog of learning that a food is inedible provided insight into mechanisms underlying the learning. <i>Aplysia</i> learn to stop responding to a food when they attempt but fail to swallow it. Pairing a cholinergic agonist with an NO donor or histamine in the <i>Aplysia</i> cerebral ganglion produced significant decreases in fictive feeding in response to the cholinergic agonist alone. Acetylcholine (ACh) is the transmitter of chemoreceptors sensing food touching the lips. Nitric oxide (NO) and histamine (HA) signal failed attempts to swallow food. Reduced responses to the cholinergic agonist after pairing with NO or HA indicate that learning partially arises via a decreased response to ACh in the cerebral ganglion.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10631127/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49679140","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A novel approach to the assessment of higher-order rule learning in male mice. 一种评估雄性小鼠高阶规则学习的新方法。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-10-06 Print Date: 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053771.123
Renee Y Chasse, Peter A Perrino, Ruth M McLeod, Gerry T M Altmann, R Holly Fitch

Historically, the development of valid and reliable methods for assessing higher-order cognitive abilities (e.g., rule learning and transfer) has been difficult in rodent models. To date, limited evidence supports the existence of higher cognitive abilities such as rule generation and complex decision-making in mice, rats, and rabbits. To this end, we sought to develop a task that would require mice to learn and transfer a rule. We trained mice to visually discriminate a series of images (image set, six total) of increasing complexity following three stages: (1) learn a visual target, (2) learn a rule (ignore any new images around the target), and finally (3) apply this rule in abstract form to a comparable but new image set. To evaluate learning for each stage, we measured (1) days (and performance by day) to discriminate the original target at criterion, (2) days (and performance by day) to get back to criterion when images in the set were altered by the introduction of distractors (rule learning), and (3) overall days (and performance by day) to criterion when experienced versus naïve cohorts of mice were tested on the same image set (rule transfer). Twenty-seven wild-type male C57 mice were tested using Bussey-Saksida touchscreen operant conditioning boxes (Lafayette Instruments). Two comparable black-white image sets were delivered sequentially (counterbalanced for order) to two identical cohorts of mice. Results showed that all mice were able to effectively learn their initial target image and could recall it >80 d later. We also found that mice were able to quickly learn and apply a "rule" : Ignore new distractors and continue to identify their visual target embedded in more complex images. The presence of rule learning was supported because performance criterion thresholds were regained much faster than initial learning when distractors were introduced. On the other hand, mice appeared unable to transfer this rule to a new set of stimuli. This is supported because visual discrimination curves for a new image set were no better than an initial (naïve) learning by a matched cohort of mice. Overall results have important implications for phenotyping research and particularly for the modeling of complex disorders in mice.

从历史上看,在啮齿类动物模型中,开发有效可靠的方法来评估高阶认知能力(如规则学习和迁移)一直很困难。到目前为止,有限的证据支持小鼠、大鼠和兔子存在更高的认知能力,如规则生成和复杂决策。为此,我们试图开发一项任务,要求小鼠学习和传递规则。我们训练小鼠在视觉上区分一系列复杂度不断增加的图像(图像集,共六个),分为三个阶段:(1)学习视觉目标,(2)学习规则(忽略目标周围的任何新图像),最后(3)将该规则以抽象形式应用于可比较但新的图像集。为了评估每个阶段的学习情况,我们测量了(1)天(和每天的表现)来区分标准下的原始目标,(2)天(以及每天的表现,以及(3)当在同一图像集上测试经验丰富的小鼠与天真的小鼠队列时,根据标准的总天数(以及每天的表现)(规则转移)。使用Bussey Saksida触摸屏操作条件调节盒(Lafayette Instruments)对27只野生型雄性C57小鼠进行测试。将两个可比较的黑白图像集依次(按顺序平衡)传递给两组相同的小鼠。结果表明,所有小鼠都能有效地学习其初始目标图像,并能在>80d后回忆起。我们还发现,小鼠能够快速学习并应用一条“规则”:忽略新的干扰物,继续识别嵌入更复杂图像中的视觉目标。规则学习的存在得到了支持,因为当引入干扰物时,性能标准阈值的恢复速度比初始学习快得多。另一方面,小鼠似乎无法将这一规则转移到一组新的刺激中。这是支持的,因为新图像集的视觉辨别曲线并不比匹配的小鼠队列的初始(天真)学习好。总体结果对表型研究,特别是对小鼠复杂疾病的建模具有重要意义。
{"title":"A novel approach to the assessment of higher-order rule learning in male mice.","authors":"Renee Y Chasse,&nbsp;Peter A Perrino,&nbsp;Ruth M McLeod,&nbsp;Gerry T M Altmann,&nbsp;R Holly Fitch","doi":"10.1101/lm.053771.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053771.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Historically, the development of valid and reliable methods for assessing higher-order cognitive abilities (e.g., rule learning and transfer) has been difficult in rodent models. To date, limited evidence supports the existence of higher cognitive abilities such as rule generation and complex decision-making in mice, rats, and rabbits. To this end, we sought to develop a task that would require mice to learn and transfer a rule. We trained mice to visually discriminate a series of images (image set, six total) of increasing complexity following three stages: (1) learn a visual target, (2) learn a rule (ignore any new images around the target), and finally (3) apply this rule in abstract form to a comparable but new image set. To evaluate learning for each stage, we measured (1) days (and performance by day) to discriminate the original target at criterion, (2) days (and performance by day) to get back to criterion when images in the set were altered by the introduction of distractors (rule learning), and (3) overall days (and performance by day) to criterion when experienced versus naïve cohorts of mice were tested on the same image set (rule transfer). Twenty-seven wild-type male C57 mice were tested using Bussey-Saksida touchscreen operant conditioning boxes (Lafayette Instruments). Two comparable black-white image sets were delivered sequentially (counterbalanced for order) to two identical cohorts of mice. Results showed that all mice were able to effectively learn their initial target image and could recall it >80 d later. We also found that mice were able to quickly learn and apply a \"rule\" : Ignore new distractors and continue to identify their visual target embedded in more complex images. The presence of rule learning was supported because performance criterion thresholds were regained much faster than initial learning when distractors were introduced. On the other hand, mice appeared unable to transfer this rule to a new set of stimuli. This is supported because visual discrimination curves for a new image set were no better than an initial (naïve) learning by a matched cohort of mice. Overall results have important implications for phenotyping research and particularly for the modeling of complex disorders in mice.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10561631/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41135457","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
On the participation of adenosinergic receptors in the reconsolidation of spatial long-term memory in male rats. 腺能受体参与雄性大鼠空间长期记忆的再巩固。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-06 Print Date: 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053785.123
Anne Karine Bosetto Fiebrantz, Luana Felski Leite, Eduarda Dal Pisol Schwab, Juliana Sartori Bonini, Weber Cláudio da Silva

To date, there is insufficient evidence to explain the role of adenosinergic receptors in the reconsolidation of long-term spatial memory. In this work, the role of the adenosinergic receptor family (A1, A2A, A2B, and A3) in this process has been elucidated. It was demonstrated that when infused bilaterally into the hippocampal CA1 region immediately after an early nonreinforced test session performed 24 h posttraining in the Morris water maze task, adenosine can cause anterograde amnesia for recent and late long-term spatial memory. This effect on spatial memory reconsolidation was blocked by A1 or A3 receptor antagonists and mimicked by A1 plus A3 receptor agonists, showing that this effect occurs through A1 and A3 receptors simultaneously. The A3 receptor alone participates only in the reconsolidation of late long-term spatial memory. When the memory to be reconsolidated was delayed (reactivation 5 d posttraining), the amnesic effect of adenosine became transient and did not occur in a test performed 5 d after the reactivation of the mnemonic trace. Finally, it has been shown that the amnesic effect of adenosine on spatial memory reconsolidation depends on the occurrence of protein degradation and that the amnesic effect of inhibition of protein synthesis on spatial memory reconsolidation is dependent on the activation of A3 receptors.

到目前为止,没有足够的证据来解释腺苷能受体在长期空间记忆的再巩固中的作用。在这项工作中,腺苷能受体家族(A1、A2A、A2B和A3)在这一过程中的作用已经阐明。研究表明,在Morris水迷宫任务中训练后24小时进行的早期非强化测试后,立即将腺苷双侧注入海马CA1区,腺苷可导致近期和晚期长期空间记忆的顺行性健忘症。这种对空间记忆再巩固的作用被A1或A3受体拮抗剂阻断,并被A1加A3受体激动剂模拟,表明这种作用同时通过A1和A3受体发生。A3受体单独参与晚期长期空间记忆的再巩固。当要重新巩固的记忆被延迟时(训练后5天重新激活),腺苷的遗忘效应变得短暂,并且在记忆痕迹重新激活后5天进行的测试中没有发生。最后,研究表明,腺苷对空间记忆再巩固的遗忘作用取决于蛋白质降解的发生,而抑制蛋白质合成对空间记忆重新巩固的遗忘效应取决于A3受体的激活。
{"title":"On the participation of adenosinergic receptors in the reconsolidation of spatial long-term memory in male rats.","authors":"Anne Karine Bosetto Fiebrantz, Luana Felski Leite, Eduarda Dal Pisol Schwab, Juliana Sartori Bonini, Weber Cláudio da Silva","doi":"10.1101/lm.053785.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053785.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>To date, there is insufficient evidence to explain the role of adenosinergic receptors in the reconsolidation of long-term spatial memory. In this work, the role of the adenosinergic receptor family (A1, A2A, A2B, and A3) in this process has been elucidated. It was demonstrated that when infused bilaterally into the hippocampal CA1 region immediately after an early nonreinforced test session performed 24 h posttraining in the Morris water maze task, adenosine can cause anterograde amnesia for recent and late long-term spatial memory. This effect on spatial memory reconsolidation was blocked by A1 or A3 receptor antagonists and mimicked by A1 plus A3 receptor agonists, showing that this effect occurs through A1 and A3 receptors simultaneously. The A3 receptor alone participates only in the reconsolidation of late long-term spatial memory. When the memory to be reconsolidated was delayed (reactivation 5 d posttraining), the amnesic effect of adenosine became transient and did not occur in a test performed 5 d after the reactivation of the mnemonic trace. Finally, it has been shown that the amnesic effect of adenosine on spatial memory reconsolidation depends on the occurrence of protein degradation and that the amnesic effect of inhibition of protein synthesis on spatial memory reconsolidation is dependent on the activation of A3 receptors.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10561635/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41142255","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Presence of a remote fear memory engram in the central amygdala. 杏仁核中央存在一个遥远的恐惧记忆。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-10-06 Print Date: 2023-10-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053833.123
Robert J Hammack, Victoria E Fischer, Mary Ann Andrade, Glenn M Toney

Fear memory formation and recall are highly regulated processes, with the central amygdala (CeA) contributing to fear memory-related behaviors. We recently reported that a remote fear memory engram is resident in the anterior basolateral amygdala (aBLA). However, the extent to which downstream neurons in the CeA participate in this engram is unknown. We tested the hypothesis that CeA neurons activated during fear memory formation are reactivated during remote memory retrieval such that a CeA engram participates in remote fear memory recall and its associated behavior. Using contextual fear conditioning in TRAP2;Ai14 mice, we identified, by persistent Cre-dependent tdTomato expression (i.e., "TRAPing"), CeA neurons that were c-fos-activated during memory formation. Twenty-one days later, we quantified neurons activated during remote memory recall using Fos immunohistochemistry. Dual labeling was used to identify the subpopulation of CeA neurons that was both activated during memory formation and reactivated during recall. Compared with their context-conditioned (no shock) controls, fear-conditioned (electric shock) mice (n = 5/group) exhibited more robust fear memory-related behavior (freezing) as well as larger populations of activated (tdTomato+) and reactivated (dual-labeled) CeA neurons. Most neurons in both groups were mainly located in the capsular CeA subdivision (CeAC). Notably, however, only the size of the TRAPed population distributed throughout the CeA was significantly correlated with time spent freezing during remote fear memory recall. Our findings indicate that fear memory formation robustly activates CeA neurons and that a subset located mainly in the CeAC may contribute to both remote fear memory storage/retrieval and the resulting fear-like behavior.

恐惧记忆的形成和回忆是高度调节的过程,中央杏仁核(CeA)对恐惧记忆相关行为有贡献。我们最近报道了一个遥远的恐惧记忆印记存在于前基底外侧杏仁核(aBLA)中。然而,CeA的下游神经元在多大程度上参与了这一植入尚不清楚。我们检验了这样一种假设,即在恐惧记忆形成过程中激活的CeA神经元在远程记忆检索过程中被重新激活,因此CeA印迹参与了远程恐惧记忆回忆及其相关行为。在TRAP2中使用上下文恐惧条件反射;我们通过持续的Cre依赖性tdTomato表达(即“TRAPing”)鉴定了Ai14小鼠,即在记忆形成过程中被c-fos激活的CeA神经元。21天后,我们使用Fos免疫组织化学对远程记忆回忆过程中激活的神经元进行了量化。双重标记用于识别在记忆形成过程中被激活和在回忆过程中被重新激活的CeA神经元亚群。与上下文条件(无电击)对照组相比,恐惧条件(电击)小鼠(n=5/组)表现出更强大的恐惧记忆相关行为(冷冻),以及更多的激活(tdTomato+)和重新激活(双重标记)CeA神经元。两组的大部分神经元主要分布在囊膜CeA区(CeAC)。然而,值得注意的是,只有分布在整个CeA中的TRAPed群体的大小与在远程恐惧记忆回忆过程中冻结的时间显著相关。我们的研究结果表明,恐惧记忆的形成有力地激活了CeA神经元,并且主要位于CeAC中的一个子集可能有助于远程恐惧记忆的存储/检索以及由此产生的类似恐惧的行为。
{"title":"Presence of a remote fear memory engram in the central amygdala.","authors":"Robert J Hammack, Victoria E Fischer, Mary Ann Andrade, Glenn M Toney","doi":"10.1101/lm.053833.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053833.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Fear memory formation and recall are highly regulated processes, with the central amygdala (CeA) contributing to fear memory-related behaviors. We recently reported that a remote fear memory engram is resident in the anterior basolateral amygdala (aBLA). However, the extent to which downstream neurons in the CeA participate in this engram is unknown. We tested the hypothesis that CeA neurons activated during fear memory formation are reactivated during remote memory retrieval such that a CeA engram participates in remote fear memory recall and its associated behavior. Using contextual fear conditioning in TRAP2;Ai14 mice, we identified, by persistent Cre-dependent tdTomato expression (i.e., \"TRAPing\"), CeA neurons that were <i>c-fos</i>-activated during memory formation. Twenty-one days later, we quantified neurons activated during remote memory recall using Fos immunohistochemistry. Dual labeling was used to identify the subpopulation of CeA neurons that was both activated during memory formation and reactivated during recall. Compared with their context-conditioned (no shock) controls, fear-conditioned (electric shock) mice (<i>n</i> = 5/group) exhibited more robust fear memory-related behavior (freezing) as well as larger populations of activated (tdTomato<sup>+</sup>) and reactivated (dual-labeled) CeA neurons. Most neurons in both groups were mainly located in the capsular CeA subdivision (CeAC). Notably, however, only the size of the TRAPed population distributed throughout the CeA was significantly correlated with time spent freezing during remote fear memory recall. Our findings indicate that fear memory formation robustly activates CeA neurons and that a subset located mainly in the CeAC may contribute to both remote fear memory storage/retrieval and the resulting fear-like behavior.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-10-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10561632/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41131789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional memory consolidation during sleep is associated with slow oscillation-spindle coupling strength in young and older adults. 睡眠中的情绪记忆巩固与年轻人和老年人的慢振荡纺锤体耦合强度有关。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-28 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053685.122
Katrina Rodheim, Kyle Kainec, Eunsol Noh, Bethany Jones, Rebecca M C Spencer

Emotional memories are processed during sleep; however, the specific mechanisms are unclear. Understanding such mechanisms may provide critical insight into preventing and treating mood disorders. Consolidation of neutral memories is associated with the coupling of NREM sleep slow oscillations (SOs) and sleep spindles (SPs). Whether SO-SP coupling is likewise involved in emotional memory processing is unknown. Furthermore, there is an age-related emotional valence bias such that sleep consolidates and preserves reactivity to negative but not positive emotional memories in young adults and positive but not negative emotional memories in older adults. If SO-SP coupling contributes to the effect of sleep on emotional memory, then it may selectively support negative memory in young adults and positive memory in older adults. To address these questions, we examined whether emotional memory recognition and overnight change in emotional reactivity were associated with the strength of SO-SP coupling in young (n = 22) and older (n = 32) adults. In younger adults, coupling strength predicted negative but not positive emotional memory performance after sleep. In contrast, coupling strength predicted positive but not negative emotional memory performance after sleep in older adults. Coupling strength was not associated with emotional reactivity in either age group. Our findings suggest that SO-SP coupling may play a mechanistic role in sleep-dependent consolidation of emotional memories.

情绪记忆是在睡眠中处理的;然而,具体机制尚不清楚。了解这些机制可以为预防和治疗情绪障碍提供重要的见解。中性记忆的巩固与NREM睡眠慢振荡(SO)和睡眠纺锤波(SP)的耦合有关。SO-SP耦合是否同样参与情绪记忆处理尚不清楚。此外,还有一种与年龄相关的情绪效价偏差,即睡眠巩固并保持年轻人对消极但非积极情绪记忆的反应,老年人对积极但非消极情绪记忆的响应。如果SO-SP耦合有助于睡眠对情绪记忆的影响,那么它可能选择性地支持年轻人的负记忆和老年人的正记忆。为了解决这些问题,我们研究了年轻人(n=22)和老年人(n=32)的情绪记忆识别和情绪反应的夜间变化是否与SO-SP偶联强度有关。在年轻人中,耦合强度预测了睡眠后消极但不积极的情绪记忆表现。相反,耦合强度预测了老年人睡眠后的积极而非消极的情绪记忆表现。在两个年龄组中,耦合强度均与情绪反应无关。我们的研究结果表明,SO-SP偶联可能在依赖睡眠的情绪记忆巩固中发挥机制作用。
{"title":"Emotional memory consolidation during sleep is associated with slow oscillation-spindle coupling strength in young and older adults.","authors":"Katrina Rodheim, Kyle Kainec, Eunsol Noh, Bethany Jones, Rebecca M C Spencer","doi":"10.1101/lm.053685.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053685.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Emotional memories are processed during sleep; however, the specific mechanisms are unclear. Understanding such mechanisms may provide critical insight into preventing and treating mood disorders. Consolidation of neutral memories is associated with the coupling of NREM sleep slow oscillations (SOs) and sleep spindles (SPs). Whether SO-SP coupling is likewise involved in emotional memory processing is unknown. Furthermore, there is an age-related emotional valence bias such that sleep consolidates and preserves reactivity to negative but not positive emotional memories in young adults and positive but not negative emotional memories in older adults. If SO-SP coupling contributes to the effect of sleep on emotional memory, then it may selectively support negative memory in young adults and positive memory in older adults. To address these questions, we examined whether emotional memory recognition and overnight change in emotional reactivity were associated with the strength of SO-SP coupling in young (<i>n</i> = 22) and older (<i>n</i> = 32) adults. In younger adults, coupling strength predicted negative but not positive emotional memory performance after sleep. In contrast, coupling strength predicted positive but not negative emotional memory performance after sleep in older adults. Coupling strength was not associated with emotional reactivity in either age group. Our findings suggest that SO-SP coupling may play a mechanistic role in sleep-dependent consolidation of emotional memories.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547370/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41134006","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Better late than never: sleep still supports memory consolidation after prolonged periods of wakefulness. 晚睡总比不睡好:在长时间清醒后,睡眠仍然有助于巩固记忆。
IF 2 4区 医学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-09-28 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053660.122
Marit Petzka, Ondrej Zika, Bernhard P Staresina, Scott A Cairney

While the benefits of sleep for associative memory are well established, it is unclear whether single-item memories profit from overnight consolidation to the same extent. We addressed this question in a preregistered, online study and also investigated how the temporal proximity between learning and sleep influences overnight retention. Sleep relative to wakefulness improved retention of item and associative memories to similar extents irrespective of whether sleep occurred soon after learning or following a prolonged waking interval. Our findings highlight the far-reaching influences of sleep on memory that can arise even after substantial periods of wakefulness.

虽然睡眠对联想记忆的好处已经得到了很好的证实,但尚不清楚单项记忆是否在同样程度上从一夜巩固中受益。我们在一项预先注册的在线研究中解决了这个问题,还调查了学习和睡眠之间的时间接近度如何影响夜间记忆力。睡眠相对于清醒在相似程度上改善了项目和联想记忆的保留,无论睡眠是在学习后不久还是在长时间清醒后发生。我们的研究结果强调了睡眠对记忆的深远影响,即使在长时间清醒后也会产生这种影响。
{"title":"Better late than never: sleep still supports memory consolidation after prolonged periods of wakefulness.","authors":"Marit Petzka,&nbsp;Ondrej Zika,&nbsp;Bernhard P Staresina,&nbsp;Scott A Cairney","doi":"10.1101/lm.053660.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053660.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While the benefits of sleep for associative memory are well established, it is unclear whether single-item memories profit from overnight consolidation to the same extent. We addressed this question in a preregistered, online study and also investigated how the temporal proximity between learning and sleep influences overnight retention. Sleep relative to wakefulness improved retention of item and associative memories to similar extents irrespective of whether sleep occurred soon after learning or following a prolonged waking interval. Our findings highlight the far-reaching influences of sleep on memory that can arise even after substantial periods of wakefulness.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547377/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41132581","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hippocampal memory reactivation during sleep is correlated with specific cortical states of the retrosplenial and prefrontal cortices. 睡眠中海马记忆的再激活与脾后和前额叶皮层的特定皮层状态相关。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-27 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053834.123
Pedro A Feliciano-Ramos, Maria Galazo, Hector Penagos, Matthew Wilson

Episodic memories are thought to be stabilized through the coordination of cortico-hippocampal activity during sleep. However, the timing and mechanism of this coordination remain unknown. To investigate this, we studied the relationship between hippocampal reactivation and slow-wave sleep up and down states of the retrosplenial cortex (RTC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC). We found that hippocampal reactivations are strongly correlated with specific cortical states. Reactivation occurred during sustained cortical Up states or during the transition from up to down state. Interestingly, the most prevalent interaction with memory reactivation in the hippocampus occurred during sustained up states of the PFC and RTC, while hippocampal reactivation and cortical up-to-down state transition in the RTC showed the strongest coordination. Reactivation usually occurred within 150-200 msec of a cortical Up state onset, indicating that a buildup of excitation during cortical Up state activity influences the probability of memory reactivation in CA1. Conversely, CA1 reactivation occurred 30-50 msec before the onset of a cortical down state, suggesting that memory reactivation affects down state initiation in the RTC and PFC, but the effect in the RTC was more robust. Our findings provide evidence that supports and highlights the complexity of bidirectional communication between cortical regions and the hippocampus during sleep.

情景记忆被认为是通过睡眠中皮质-海马活动的协调来稳定的。然而,这种协调的时机和机制仍然未知。为了研究这一点,我们研究了海马再激活与脾后皮层(RTC)和前额叶皮层(PFC)慢波睡眠上下状态之间的关系。我们发现海马的再激活与特定的皮层状态密切相关。再激活发生在持续的皮层向上状态或从向上状态到向下状态的过渡过程中。有趣的是,海马中与记忆再激活最普遍的相互作用发生在PFC和RTC的持续上升状态期间,而RTC中的海马再激活和皮层从上到下的状态转换显示出最强的协调性。再激活通常发生在皮层Up状态开始后的150-200毫秒内,表明皮层Up状态活动期间兴奋的积累影响CA1记忆再激活的概率。相反,CA1再激活发生在皮层下降状态开始前30-50毫秒,这表明记忆再激活影响RTC和PFC的下降状态开始,但RTC的影响更为强烈。我们的发现提供了证据,支持并强调了睡眠期间皮层区域和海马体之间双向交流的复杂性。
{"title":"Hippocampal memory reactivation during sleep is correlated with specific cortical states of the retrosplenial and prefrontal cortices.","authors":"Pedro A Feliciano-Ramos, Maria Galazo, Hector Penagos, Matthew Wilson","doi":"10.1101/lm.053834.123","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053834.123","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Episodic memories are thought to be stabilized through the coordination of cortico-hippocampal activity during sleep. However, the timing and mechanism of this coordination remain unknown. To investigate this, we studied the relationship between hippocampal reactivation and slow-wave sleep up and down states of the retrosplenial cortex (RTC) and prefrontal cortex (PFC). We found that hippocampal reactivations are strongly correlated with specific cortical states. Reactivation occurred during sustained cortical Up states or during the transition from up to down state. Interestingly, the most prevalent interaction with memory reactivation in the hippocampus occurred during sustained up states of the PFC and RTC, while hippocampal reactivation and cortical up-to-down state transition in the RTC showed the strongest coordination. Reactivation usually occurred within 150-200 msec of a cortical Up state onset, indicating that a buildup of excitation during cortical Up state activity influences the probability of memory reactivation in CA1. Conversely, CA1 reactivation occurred 30-50 msec before the onset of a cortical down state, suggesting that memory reactivation affects down state initiation in the RTC and PFC, but the effect in the RTC was more robust. Our findings provide evidence that supports and highlights the complexity of bidirectional communication between cortical regions and the hippocampus during sleep.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547389/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41125698","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Targeted memory reactivation during slow-wave sleep vs. sleep stage N2: no significant differences in a vocabulary task. 慢波睡眠与睡眠阶段N2的定向记忆再激活:词汇任务没有显著差异。
IF 1.8 4区 医学 Q4 NEUROSCIENCES Pub Date : 2023-09-19 Print Date: 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1101/lm.053683.122
Anna Wick, Björn Rasch

Sleep supports memory consolidation, and slow-wave sleep (SWS) in particular is assumed to benefit the consolidation of verbal learning material. Re-exposure to previously learned words during SWS with a technique known as targeted memory reactivation (TMR) consistently benefits memory. However, TMR has also been successfully applied during sleep stage N2, though a direct comparison between words selectively reactivated during SWS versus N2 is still missing. Here, we directly compared the effects of N2 TMR and SWS TMR on memory performance in a vocabulary learning task in a within-subject design. Thirty-four healthy young participants (21 in the main sample and 13 in an additional sample) learned 120 Dutch-German word pairs before sleep. Participants in the main sample slept for ∼8 h during the night, while participants in the additional sample slept ∼3 h. We reactivated the Dutch words selectively during N2 and SWS in one single night. Forty words were not cued. Participants in the main sample recalled the German translations of the Dutch words after sleep in the morning, while those in the additional sample did so at 2:00 a.m. As expected, we observed no differences in recall performance between words reactivated during N2 and SWS. However, we failed to find an overall memory benefit of reactivated over nonreactivated words. Detailed time-frequency analyses showed that words played during N2 elicited stronger characteristic oscillatory responses in several frequency bands, including spindle and theta frequencies, compared with SWS. These oscillatory responses did not vary with the memory strengths of individual words. Our results question the robustness and replicability of the TMR benefit on memory using our Dutch vocabulary learning task. We discuss potential boundary conditions for vocabulary reactivation paradigms and, most importantly, see the need for further replication studies, ideally including multiple laboratories and larger sample sizes.

睡眠支持记忆巩固,尤其是慢波睡眠(SWS)被认为有利于巩固语言学习材料。在SWS过程中,通过一种称为目标记忆再激活(TMR)的技术,再次接触以前学习过的单词始终有利于记忆。然而,TMR也已在睡眠阶段N2期间成功应用,尽管在SWS期间选择性地重新激活的字与N2之间的直接比较仍然缺失。在这里,我们直接比较了在主题内设计的词汇学习任务中,N2 TMR和SWS TMR对记忆性能的影响。34名健康的年轻参与者(21名在主样本中,13名在附加样本中)在睡觉前学习了120个荷兰语-德语单词对。主样本中的参与者在夜间睡了~8小时,而附加样本中的与会者睡了~3小时。我们在一个晚上的N2和SWS期间选择性地重新激活了荷兰语单词。没有提示40个单词。主样本的参与者在早上睡觉后回忆荷兰语单词的德语翻译,而附加样本的参与者则在凌晨2:00回忆。不出所料,我们观察到N2和SWS期间重新激活的单词之间的回忆表现没有差异。然而,我们没有发现重新激活的单词比未激活的单词有更大的记忆力优势。详细的时频分析表明,与SWS相比,N2期间播放的单词在几个频带(包括纺锤和θ频率)上引发了更强的特征振荡响应。这些振荡反应不随单个单词的记忆强度而变化。使用我们的荷兰语词汇学习任务,我们的结果对TMR在记忆方面的益处的稳健性和可复制性提出了质疑。我们讨论了词汇再激活范式的潜在边界条件,最重要的是,我们看到了进一步复制研究的必要性,理想情况下包括多个实验室和更大的样本量。
{"title":"Targeted memory reactivation during slow-wave sleep vs. sleep stage N2: no significant differences in a vocabulary task.","authors":"Anna Wick, Björn Rasch","doi":"10.1101/lm.053683.122","DOIUrl":"10.1101/lm.053683.122","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sleep supports memory consolidation, and slow-wave sleep (SWS) in particular is assumed to benefit the consolidation of verbal learning material. Re-exposure to previously learned words during SWS with a technique known as targeted memory reactivation (TMR) consistently benefits memory. However, TMR has also been successfully applied during sleep stage N2, though a direct comparison between words selectively reactivated during SWS versus N2 is still missing. Here, we directly compared the effects of N2 TMR and SWS TMR on memory performance in a vocabulary learning task in a within-subject design. Thirty-four healthy young participants (21 in the main sample and 13 in an additional sample) learned 120 Dutch-German word pairs before sleep. Participants in the main sample slept for ∼8 h during the night, while participants in the additional sample slept ∼3 h. We reactivated the Dutch words selectively during N2 and SWS in one single night. Forty words were not cued. Participants in the main sample recalled the German translations of the Dutch words after sleep in the morning, while those in the additional sample did so at 2:00 a.m. As expected, we observed no differences in recall performance between words reactivated during N2 and SWS. However, we failed to find an overall memory benefit of reactivated over nonreactivated words. Detailed time-frequency analyses showed that words played during N2 elicited stronger characteristic oscillatory responses in several frequency bands, including spindle and theta frequencies, compared with SWS. These oscillatory responses did not vary with the memory strengths of individual words. Our results question the robustness and replicability of the TMR benefit on memory using our Dutch vocabulary learning task. We discuss potential boundary conditions for vocabulary reactivation paradigms and, most importantly, see the need for further replication studies, ideally including multiple laboratories and larger sample sizes.</p>","PeriodicalId":18003,"journal":{"name":"Learning & memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.8,"publicationDate":"2023-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10547374/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41135237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"医学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Learning & memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1