首页 > 最新文献

Memory最新文献

英文 中文
Flashbulb and first-hand memories for campus closings due to COVID-19: consistency and change. 因 COVID-19 而关闭校园的闪光灯和第一手记忆:一致性和变化。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-18 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2404499
Yan Xuan, Mackenna Greenberg, Sharda Umanath, Jennifer H Coane

Flashbulb memories (FBM) refer to the vivid and detailed retrieval of the reception context of a highly salient event. We examined FBMs and personal memories for one college's sudden transition to remote learning due to COVID-19. We explored whether the announcement of the campus' closure resulted in FBMs, how respondents felt about the decision, and the impacts of the decision. Employing a two-wave longitudinal survey conducted in March and May 2020, participants responded to questions regarding learning about the campus' closure and a control memory (an event from the same week chosen by participants). Participant reports suggested they did form FBMs, and FBMs were more consistent over time than control memories. Confidence did not differ across memory types. Additionally, we observed an initial strong positive response to the decision to close the campus - a sentiment that intensified over time. Lastly, participants' emotional responses transitioned from negative feelings in the first wave of testing to more neutral feelings in the second. This work offers a unique exploration of FBMs within the broader context of a global health crisis that intruded into daily life, effectively merging the typically public and distant nature of flashbulb events with first-hand, personal experience.

闪光记忆(FBM)指的是对高度突出事件的接收背景进行生动而详细的检索。我们研究了一所大学因 COVID-19 而突然转型为远程学习的闪光记忆和个人记忆。我们探讨了校园关闭的消息是否导致了 FBMs、受访者对该决定的感受以及该决定的影响。在 2020 年 3 月和 5 月进行的两波纵向调查中,参与者回答了有关得知校园关闭消息和对照记忆(参与者选择的同一周事件)的问题。参与者的报告表明,他们确实形成了FBM,而且随着时间的推移,FBM比对照记忆更加一致。不同记忆类型的信心没有差异。此外,我们还观察到,最初参与者对关闭校园的决定有强烈的积极反应,但随着时间的推移,这种情绪逐渐增强。最后,参与者的情绪反应从第一波测试的消极情绪过渡到第二波测试的中性情绪。这项研究在全球健康危机侵入日常生活的大背景下,对 "家庭信任管理 "进行了独特的探索,有效地将闪光灯事件典型的公开性和遥远性与第一手的个人体验相结合。
{"title":"Flashbulb and first-hand memories for campus closings due to COVID-19: consistency and change.","authors":"Yan Xuan, Mackenna Greenberg, Sharda Umanath, Jennifer H Coane","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2404499","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2404499","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Flashbulb memories (FBM) refer to the vivid and detailed retrieval of the reception context of a highly salient event. We examined FBMs and personal memories for one college's sudden transition to remote learning due to COVID-19. We explored whether the announcement of the campus' closure resulted in FBMs, how respondents felt about the decision, and the impacts of the decision. Employing a two-wave longitudinal survey conducted in March and May 2020, participants responded to questions regarding learning about the campus' closure and a control memory (an event from the same week chosen by participants). Participant reports suggested they did form FBMs, and FBMs were more consistent over time than control memories. Confidence did not differ across memory types. Additionally, we observed an initial strong positive response to the decision to close the campus - a sentiment that intensified over time. Lastly, participants' emotional responses transitioned from negative feelings in the first wave of testing to more neutral feelings in the second. This work offers a unique exploration of FBMs within the broader context of a global health crisis that intruded into daily life, effectively merging the typically public and distant nature of flashbulb events with first-hand, personal experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
You don't understand me! But, I do! Awareness of cross-generational differences in collective remembering of national historic events. 你不理解我!但是,我懂!对国家历史事件集体记忆中跨代差异的认识。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2403714
Claire Hou, Sharda Umanath, Amy Corning, Magdalena Abel

Collective memories refer to a group's shared representation of the past, which are slow to change over time. In this study, representative samples of American and German Younger Adults (YAs) and Older Adults (OAs) rated the emotional valence of 12 national historic events. Critically, both age groups were also asked to take on the perspective of the other: OAs imagined how YAs feel, whereas YAs imagined how OAs feel about the same events today. The results replicated previous findings that OAs and YAs hold differing opinions on numerous events. Both age groups successfully recognised these different collective perceptions between generations to some extent. Yet, OAs were more accurate in the perspective-taking task, with YAs consistently underestimating the intensity of OAs' emotional valence. Self-reported perspective-taking strategies suggest that OAs relied more on stereotypes and considered education, while knowledge from specific people was universally used to rate the other age group's perspective.

集体记忆指的是一个群体对过去的共同表述,这种表述随着时间的推移变化缓慢。在这项研究中,具有代表性的美国和德国青少年(YAs)和老年人(OAs)样本对 12 个国家历史事件的情感价值进行了评分。重要的是,这两个年龄组的人还被要求站在对方的角度考虑问题:老年成人想象青年成人的感受,而青年成人则想象老年成人今天对相同事件的感受。结果重复了以前的研究结果,即老年大学和青年大学对许多事件持有不同的观点。两个年龄组都在一定程度上成功地认识到了代际之间不同的集体看法。然而,在透视任务中,老年观众更准确,而青年观众则一直低估老年观众情绪情感的强度。自我报告的角度选择策略表明,老年助听器更多地依赖于刻板印象和教育考虑,而来自特定人群的知识则被普遍用于评价另一年龄组的角度。
{"title":"You don't understand me! But, I do! Awareness of cross-generational differences in collective remembering of national historic events.","authors":"Claire Hou, Sharda Umanath, Amy Corning, Magdalena Abel","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2403714","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2403714","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Collective memories refer to a group's shared representation of the past, which are slow to change over time. In this study, representative samples of American and German Younger Adults (YAs) and Older Adults (OAs) rated the emotional valence of 12 national historic events. Critically, both age groups were also asked to take on the perspective of the other: OAs imagined how YAs feel, whereas YAs imagined how OAs feel about the same events today. The results replicated previous findings that OAs and YAs hold differing opinions on numerous events. Both age groups successfully recognised these different collective perceptions between generations to some extent. Yet, OAs were more accurate in the perspective-taking task, with YAs consistently underestimating the intensity of OAs' emotional valence. Self-reported perspective-taking strategies suggest that OAs relied more on stereotypes and considered education, while knowledge from specific people was universally used to rate the other age group's perspective.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291193","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Negative life events predict depressive trends: the moderating effect of overgeneral autobiographical memory and early parenting behaviour. 负面生活事件预测抑郁趋势:过度自传体记忆和早期养育行为的调节作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2404261
Xinyu Zhang, Yuanxia Zheng, Guoxiong Liu

The Emerging Adulthood is a complex and chaotic period and depression is one of the main psychological health problems during this period. Overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) is prevalent among patients with clinical depression. However, the prediction of OGM in groups with non-clinical depression and its influencing mechanisms remain inconclusive. Studies have shown that OGM and early parenting behaviour are vulnerable factors of depression in emerging adulthood, which may be triggered by negative life events. Our longitudinal study included 241 participants (Mage = 21.88 years). At baseline, participants completed measures of current negative life events, depression, early parenting behaviour and an autobiographical memory test. Thereafter, they were tracked for depression every 35 days. We used the latent class growth model to differentiate levels and trends of depression among non-clinical participants. The analysis showed that the effect of negative life events on depression was moderated by OGM and early parenting behaviour. However, this moderating effect was found only in the low-risk depression group. Our findings indicate that early parenting behaviour might account for the different mechanisms of OGM production in non-clinical groups. Moreover, it underlines the importance of OGM and early parenting behaviour as potential predictors of future depression in non-clinical groups.

青春期是一个复杂而混乱的时期,抑郁症是这一时期的主要心理健康问题之一。过度自传体记忆(OGM)在临床抑郁症患者中十分普遍。然而,对非临床抑郁症群体中 OGM 的预测及其影响机制仍无定论。研究表明,OGM 和早期养育行为是成年期抑郁症的易感因素,可能由负面生活事件引发。我们的纵向研究包括 241 名参与者(年龄 = 21.88 岁)。在基线期,参与者完成了对当前负面生活事件、抑郁、早期养育行为和自传体记忆测试的测量。此后,每 35 天对他们进行一次抑郁跟踪。我们使用潜类增长模型来区分非临床参与者的抑郁水平和趋势。分析结果表明,负面生活事件对抑郁的影响受到 OGM 和早期养育行为的调节。然而,这种调节作用仅在低风险抑郁组中发现。我们的研究结果表明,早期养育行为可能是非临床群体产生 OGM 的不同机制的原因。此外,它还强调了 OGM 和早期养育行为作为非临床群体未来抑郁的潜在预测因素的重要性。
{"title":"Negative life events predict depressive trends: the moderating effect of overgeneral autobiographical memory and early parenting behaviour.","authors":"Xinyu Zhang, Yuanxia Zheng, Guoxiong Liu","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2404261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2404261","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The Emerging Adulthood is a complex and chaotic period and depression is one of the main psychological health problems during this period. Overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) is prevalent among patients with clinical depression. However, the prediction of OGM in groups with non-clinical depression and its influencing mechanisms remain inconclusive. Studies have shown that OGM and early parenting behaviour are vulnerable factors of depression in emerging adulthood, which may be triggered by negative life events. Our longitudinal study included 241 participants (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 21.88 years). At baseline, participants completed measures of current negative life events, depression, early parenting behaviour and an autobiographical memory test. Thereafter, they were tracked for depression every 35 days. We used the latent class growth model to differentiate levels and trends of depression among non-clinical participants. The analysis showed that the effect of negative life events on depression was moderated by OGM and early parenting behaviour. However, this moderating effect was found only in the low-risk depression group. Our findings indicate that early parenting behaviour might account for the different mechanisms of OGM production in non-clinical groups. Moreover, it underlines the importance of OGM and early parenting behaviour as potential predictors of future depression in non-clinical groups.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291192","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inducing positive involuntary mental imagery in daily life using personalized photograph stimuli. 在日常生活中利用个性化照片刺激诱发积极的非自愿心理意象。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2402920
Mahdi Bagheri, Marcella L Woud, Jolina Simon, Lilah Abdalla, Mats Dombrowe, Cem Woinek, Jürgen Margraf, Simon E Blackwell

Most people experience positive involuntary mental imagery (IMI) frequently in daily life; however, evidence for the importance and effects of positive IMI is largely indirect. The current study adapted a paradigm to experimentally induce positive IMI in participants' daily lives. This could in turn provide a means to directly test positive IMI's effects. In a within-subjects design, participants (N = 41) generated positive mental images (imagery condition) and sentences (verbal condition) from photo cues, half of which participants provided from their own living environment. Participants then recorded involuntary memories of the previously generated images or sentences in a seven-day diary, before returning to the lab and completing some measures including an involuntary memory task. In the diary, participants reported more involuntary memories from the imagery condition than from the verbal condition, and more involuntary memories from their own photos compared to the other photos. A more mixed pattern of findings was found across other tasks in the lab. The study indicates that the paradigm can be used as a means to induce positive IMI and that using photos as the basis for generating positive imagery increases the amount of IMI in daily life. Theoretical and potential clinical implications are discussed.

大多数人在日常生活中都会经常经历积极的非自主心理意象(IMI);然而,有关积极心理意象的重要性和效果的证据大多是间接的。本研究调整了一种范式,在参与者的日常生活中实验性地诱发积极的心理意象。这反过来又为直接测试积极 IMI 的效果提供了一种方法。在主体内设计中,参与者(N = 41)根据照片线索生成积极的心理图像(意象条件)和句子(语言条件),其中一半是参与者从自己的生活环境中提供的。然后,参与者在七天的日记中记录下对之前生成的图像或句子的非自主记忆,之后再返回实验室完成包括非自主记忆任务在内的一些测量。在日记中,参与者对图像条件的非自主记忆多于对语言条件的非自主记忆,对自己照片的非自主记忆多于对其他照片的非自主记忆。在实验室进行的其他任务中,发现的结果则较为混杂。该研究表明,该范式可用作诱导积极意象记忆的一种手段,而且以照片为基础产生积极意象会增加日常生活中的意象记忆数量。研究还讨论了理论和潜在的临床意义。
{"title":"Inducing positive involuntary mental imagery in daily life using personalized photograph stimuli.","authors":"Mahdi Bagheri, Marcella L Woud, Jolina Simon, Lilah Abdalla, Mats Dombrowe, Cem Woinek, Jürgen Margraf, Simon E Blackwell","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2402920","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2402920","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Most people experience positive involuntary mental imagery (IMI) frequently in daily life; however, evidence for the importance and effects of positive IMI is largely indirect. The current study adapted a paradigm to experimentally induce positive IMI in participants' daily lives. This could in turn provide a means to directly test positive IMI's effects. In a within-subjects design, participants (<i>N </i>= 41) generated positive mental images (imagery condition) and sentences (verbal condition) from photo cues, half of which participants provided from their own living environment. Participants then recorded involuntary memories of the previously generated images or sentences in a seven-day diary, before returning to the lab and completing some measures including an involuntary memory task. In the diary, participants reported more involuntary memories from the imagery condition than from the verbal condition, and more involuntary memories from their own photos compared to the other photos. A more mixed pattern of findings was found across other tasks in the lab. The study indicates that the paradigm can be used as a means to induce positive IMI and that using photos as the basis for generating positive imagery increases the amount of IMI in daily life. Theoretical and potential clinical implications are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291191","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Effectiveness of production and drawing as encoding techniques on recall using mixed- and pure-list designs. 使用混合列表和纯列表设计,将制作和绘图作为编码技术对回忆的有效性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-17 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2399116
Sophia H N Tran, Myra A Fernandes

We compared the benefit of production and drawing on recall of concrete and abstract words, using mixed- and pure-list designs. We varied stimulus and list types to examine whether the memory benefit from these strategies was sustained across these manipulations. For all experiments, the memory retrieval task was free recall. In Experiment 1, participants studied concrete and abstract words sequentially, with prompts to either silently-read, read aloud, write, or draw each target (intermixed). Reading aloud, writing, and drawing improved recall compared to silent reading, with drawing leading to the largest boost. Performance, however, was at floor in all but the drawing condition. In Experiment 2, the number of targets was reduced, and each strategy (between-subjects) was compared to silent-reading. We eliminated floor effects and replicated results from Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, we manipulated strategy in a pure-list-design. The drawing benefit was maintained while that from production was eliminated. In all experiments, recall was higher for concrete than abstract words that were drawn; no such effect was found for words produced. Results suggest that drawing facilitates memory by enhancing semantic elaboration, whereas the production benefit is largely perceptually based. Importantly, the memory benefit conferred by drawing at encoding, unlike production, cannot be explained by a distinctiveness account as it was relatively unaffected by study design.

我们采用混合列表和纯列表设计,比较了制作和绘制对具体单词和抽象单词记忆的益处。我们改变了刺激和列表类型,以考察这些策略带来的记忆益处是否在这些操作中持续存在。在所有实验中,记忆检索任务均为自由回忆。在实验 1 中,受试者按顺序学习具体和抽象单词,并根据提示默读、朗读、书写或绘制每个目标(混合进行)。与默读相比,朗读、书写和绘画都能提高回忆能力,其中绘画的提高幅度最大。然而,除绘画外,其他条件下的成绩都处于最低水平。在实验 2 中,我们减少了目标的数量,并将每种策略(被试间)与默读进行了比较。我们消除了底线效应,并复制了实验 1 的结果。在实验 3 中,我们在纯列表设计中对策略进行了操作。我们保留了画图的益处,同时消除了制作的益处。在所有实验中,绘制的具体单词比抽象单词的记忆率更高;而制作的单词则没有这种效应。实验结果表明,绘画通过加强语义的阐述来促进记忆,而制作的益处则主要基于感知。重要的是,与制作不同,绘图在编码时所带来的记忆益处不能用独特性来解释,因为它相对不受研究设计的影响。
{"title":"Effectiveness of production and drawing as encoding techniques on recall using mixed- and pure-list designs.","authors":"Sophia H N Tran, Myra A Fernandes","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2399116","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2399116","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We compared the benefit of production and drawing on recall of concrete and abstract words, using mixed- and pure-list designs. We varied stimulus and list types to examine whether the memory benefit from these strategies was sustained across these manipulations. For all experiments, the memory retrieval task was free recall. In Experiment 1, participants studied concrete and abstract words sequentially, with prompts to either silently-read, read aloud, write, or draw each target (intermixed). Reading aloud, writing, and drawing improved recall compared to silent reading, with drawing leading to the largest boost. Performance, however, was at floor in all but the drawing condition. In Experiment 2, the number of targets was reduced, and each strategy (between-subjects) was compared to silent-reading. We eliminated floor effects and replicated results from Experiment 1. In Experiment 3, we manipulated strategy in a pure-list-design. The drawing benefit was maintained while that from production was eliminated. In all experiments, recall was higher for concrete than abstract words that were drawn; no such effect was found for words produced. Results suggest that drawing facilitates memory by enhancing semantic elaboration, whereas the production benefit is largely perceptually based. Importantly, the memory benefit conferred by drawing at encoding, unlike production, cannot be explained by a distinctiveness account as it was relatively unaffected by study design.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291188","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender differences and the association between the phenomenological characteristics of autobiographical memories and psychopathic traits in a university student sample. 大学生样本中的性别差异以及自传体记忆的现象学特征与精神变态特质之间的关联。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-16 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2402417
Dolores Fernández-Pérez, Arturo Gómez-Guillén, Irene Talavera, Marta Nieto, Jorge J Ricarte, Laura Ros

The study aimed to analyse the relationship between the dimensions of the triarchic model of psychopathy (meanness, boldness and disinhibition) and the phenomenological characteristics of Autobiographical Memory (AM) in a sample of university students, examining potential gender differences. Participants (N = 260; 55.4% women; aged 18-25) performed an AM task, followed by the Autobiographical Memory Characteristics Questionnaire and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure. We found gender differences, with men scoring higher than women in meanness and disinhibition as well as in precision, accessibility, sharing and narrative coherence of AM. Correlations showed that boldness was negatively related to the valence and emotional intensity of the memory. Meanness was positively associated with precision, sensory details, accessibility, sharing, narrative coherence, distancing and preoccupation with emotions and negatively with recollection. Disinhibition was positively related to precision, sensory details and accessibility and negatively to intensity, distancing and preoccupation with emotions. Our results suggest that psychopathic traits could predict certain characteristics of AM, highlighting the predictive value of meanness, especially regarding memory quality characteristics (e.g., precision), as well as disinhibition, concerning the emotional content (e.g., preoccupation with emotions). Our results contribute to understanding psychopathy through an autobiographical perspective, showing how psychopathic traits may shape how people remember personal events.

本研究旨在分析心理变态的三元模型(卑鄙、大胆和不自制)与大学生自传体记忆(AM)现象学特征之间的关系,并研究潜在的性别差异。参与者(N = 260;55.4% 为女性;年龄在 18-25 岁之间)在完成自传体记忆任务后,进行了自传体记忆特征问卷调查和三元心理变态测量。我们发现了性别差异,男性在自传体记忆的刻薄性和抑制性以及精确性、可及性、分享性和叙述连贯性方面的得分都高于女性。相关性表明,大胆与记忆的价值和情感强度呈负相关。吝啬与记忆的精确性、感官细节、可及性、共享性、叙述连贯性、距离感和对情绪的关注呈正相关,而与回忆呈负相关。抑制与精确度、感官细节和可及性呈正相关,而与强度、距离感和对情绪的专注呈负相关。我们的研究结果表明,精神变态特质可以预测上午记忆的某些特征,突出了刻薄的预测价值,特别是在记忆质量特征(如精确性)方面,以及抑制的预测价值,在情感内容(如对情感的专注)方面。我们的研究结果有助于从自传的角度来理解心理变态,说明心理变态的特质如何影响人们对个人事件的记忆。
{"title":"Gender differences and the association between the phenomenological characteristics of autobiographical memories and psychopathic traits in a university student sample.","authors":"Dolores Fernández-Pérez, Arturo Gómez-Guillén, Irene Talavera, Marta Nieto, Jorge J Ricarte, Laura Ros","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2402417","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2402417","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The study aimed to analyse the relationship between the dimensions of the triarchic model of psychopathy (meanness, boldness and disinhibition) and the phenomenological characteristics of Autobiographical Memory (AM) in a sample of university students, examining potential gender differences. Participants (<i>N</i> = 260; 55.4% women; aged 18-25) performed an AM task, followed by the Autobiographical Memory Characteristics Questionnaire and the Triarchic Psychopathy Measure. We found gender differences, with men scoring higher than women in meanness and disinhibition as well as in precision, accessibility, sharing and narrative coherence of AM. Correlations showed that boldness was negatively related to the valence and emotional intensity of the memory. Meanness was positively associated with precision, sensory details, accessibility, sharing, narrative coherence, distancing and preoccupation with emotions and negatively with recollection. Disinhibition was positively related to precision, sensory details and accessibility and negatively to intensity, distancing and preoccupation with emotions. Our results suggest that psychopathic traits could predict certain characteristics of AM, highlighting the predictive value of meanness, especially regarding memory quality characteristics (e.g., precision), as well as disinhibition, concerning the emotional content (e.g., preoccupation with emotions). Our results contribute to understanding psychopathy through an autobiographical perspective, showing how psychopathic traits may shape how people remember personal events.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age differences in memory for names and occupations associated with faces: the effects of assigned and self-perceived social importance. 与人脸相关的姓名和职业记忆的年龄差异:指定和自我感觉的社会重要性的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2399110
Lixia Yang, Julia Scaringi, Lingqian Li

It has been documented that older adults' memory deficits can be reduced for information depicted as personally and socially important (e.g., Hargis & Castel, 2017 [Younger and older adults' associative memory for social information: The role of information importance. Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 325-330]). The current study aimed to further assess the effects of both arbitrarily assigned and self-perceived importance in younger and older adults' memory for names (low in schematic support) and occupations (high in schematic support) associated with faces. Participants studied the same 16 face-name-occupation triplets (with neutral facial expressions) across four blocks, each including a free recall of names and occupations. At the end, they completed a cued recall of names and occupations. The faces were arbitrarily cued as socially important (i.e., with an orange frame) or unimportant (e.g., without a frame). The perceived social importance was assessed by rating all the triplets based on a 10-point Likert Scale (1 = least and 10 = most important) at the end. The results showed that age-related memory deficits were reduced or even eliminated for occupations (high in schematic support) relative to names (low in schematic support), especially in the free recall of faces self-perceived as important. In other words, the combination of schematic support and self-perceived importance can effectively mitigate older adults' memory deficit.

有资料表明,对于被描述为对个人和社会重要的信息,老年人的记忆缺陷会减少(例如,Hargis & Castel, 2017 [年轻人和老年人对社会信息的联想记忆:信息重要性的作用。Psychology and Aging, 32(4), 325-330])。本研究旨在进一步评估任意指定的重要性和自我感知的重要性对年轻人和老年人记忆与人脸相关的姓名(图式支持度低)和职业(图式支持度高)的影响。受试者在四个区块中学习了相同的 16 个面孔-姓名-职业三联体(带有中性面部表情),每个区块包括姓名和职业的自由回忆。最后,他们完成了对姓名和职业的提示回忆。这些面孔被任意提示为社会重要面孔(即带有橙色边框)或社会不重要面孔(如没有边框)。最后根据 10 分制李克特量表(1 = 最不重要,10 = 最重要)对所有三胞胎的社会重要性进行评分。结果表明,与年龄有关的记忆缺陷在职业(图式支持度高)方面比在姓名(图式支持度低)方面有所减少甚至消除,尤其是在自由回忆自我认为重要的面孔时。换句话说,图式支持和自我认为的重要性相结合,可以有效缓解老年人的记忆缺陷。
{"title":"Age differences in memory for names and occupations associated with faces: the effects of assigned and self-perceived social importance.","authors":"Lixia Yang, Julia Scaringi, Lingqian Li","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2399110","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2399110","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It has been documented that older adults' memory deficits can be reduced for information depicted as personally and socially important (e.g., Hargis & Castel, 2017 [Younger and older adults' associative memory for social information: The role of information importance. <i>Psychology and Aging</i>, <i>32</i>(4), 325-330]). The current study aimed to further assess the effects of both arbitrarily assigned and self-perceived importance in younger and older adults' memory for names (low in schematic support) and occupations (high in schematic support) associated with faces. Participants studied the same 16 face-name-occupation triplets (with neutral facial expressions) across four blocks, each including a free recall of names and occupations. At the end, they completed a cued recall of names and occupations. The faces were arbitrarily cued as socially important (i.e., with an orange frame) or unimportant (e.g., without a frame). The perceived social importance was assessed by rating all the triplets based on a 10-point Likert Scale (1 = least and 10 = most important) at the end. The results showed that age-related memory deficits were reduced or even eliminated for occupations (high in schematic support) relative to names (low in schematic support), especially in the free recall of faces self-perceived as important. In other words, the combination of schematic support and self-perceived importance can effectively mitigate older adults' memory deficit.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291187","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A novel study: fragmented and holistic forgetting. 一项新颖的研究:片段遗忘和整体遗忘。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-09 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2401020
Dani Parra, Gabriel A Radvansky

According to recent theoretical work, certain event memories are more likely to be remembered or forgotten in their entirety. This prior work focused on collections of concepts, such as person-location-object triples. To explore this idea with complex materials, we created triples of people, locations, objects, or activities from events in real-world novels. People who had read one of the included novels were provided with one element from these triples (the cue) and asked to identify which of six alternatives best went with it. The results revealed that memory for the narrative events remained stable across many years. Moreover, people recalled events in a more holistic manner than would be expected by chance. This was more likely the more causally important an event was. This pattern of performance also remained stable over time. Our results are consistent with the idea that event models involve integrating separate elements into a single coherent representation, and this is likely to stay integrated over long periods of time. However, the degree to which this is so appears to be related to how well-integrated the information is within a larger set of events.

根据最近的理论研究,某些事件记忆更有可能被完整地记住或遗忘。之前的工作主要集中在概念的集合上,如人-地点-物体三元组。为了用复杂的材料来探讨这一观点,我们根据现实世界小说中的事件创建了人物、地点、物体或活动的三元组。我们向读过其中一本小说的人提供了这些三元组中的一个元素(线索),并要求他们从六个备选方案中找出与之最匹配的方案。结果显示,人们对叙事事件的记忆在多年后仍然保持稳定。此外,人们对事件的记忆比预期的更全面。事件的因果关系越重要,这种可能性就越大。这种表现模式也会随着时间的推移而保持稳定。我们的研究结果与以下观点是一致的,即事件模型涉及将不同的元素整合到一个单一的连贯表征中,而这很可能会在很长一段时间内保持整合。然而,这种整合的程度似乎与信息在更大的事件集合中的整合程度有关。
{"title":"A novel study: fragmented and holistic forgetting.","authors":"Dani Parra, Gabriel A Radvansky","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2401020","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2401020","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>According to recent theoretical work, certain event memories are more likely to be remembered or forgotten in their entirety. This prior work focused on collections of concepts, such as person-location-object triples. To explore this idea with complex materials, we created triples of people, locations, objects, or activities from events in real-world novels. People who had read one of the included novels were provided with one element from these triples (the cue) and asked to identify which of six alternatives best went with it. The results revealed that memory for the narrative events remained stable across many years. Moreover, people recalled events in a more holistic manner than would be expected by chance. This was more likely the more causally important an event was. This pattern of performance also remained stable over time. Our results are consistent with the idea that event models involve integrating separate elements into a single coherent representation, and this is likely to stay integrated over long periods of time. However, the degree to which this is so appears to be related to how well-integrated the information is within a larger set of events.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142291186","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toward mastering foreign-language translations: transfer between productive and receptive learning. 掌握外语翻译:生产性学习和接受性学习之间的迁移。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-02 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2397043
Emma Bernardi, Kalif E Vaughn, John Dunlosky, Katherine A Rawson

Learners can study foreign language-English vocabulary (e.g., denken - to think) both receptively and productively. Receptive learning involves being cued with a foreign language word (e.g., denken) and trying to translate it (i.e., to think). Productive learning involves being cued with an English word (e.g., to think) and trying to produce the translation. When students use retrieval practice to learn foreign-language translations in one direction (e.g., receptively) until they correctly recall the translation, do they demonstrate transfer in the other direction (i.e., productively)? Across three experiments, we answered this question by manipulating the order of learning schedule (reception first followed by production or vice versa). For a given schedule, participants continued to practice retrieving translations (with feedback) using the dropout method until they correctly recalled each translation three times; they then proceeded to practice the pairs in the opposite direction until they correctly recalled each translation three times. Across all experiments, transfer was partial (learning in one direction did not entirely eliminate the need to practice in the other), but transfer did occur regardless of which schedule students used first during practice.

学习者可以通过接受和生产两种方式学习外语-英语词汇(如:denken - to think)。接受式学习是以外语单词(如:denken)为线索,尝试翻译该单词(即:思考)。生产性学习则是以一个英语单词(例如,to think)为提示,并尝试进行翻译。当学生使用检索练习从一个方向(如接受性学习)学习外语译文,直到他们正确地回忆起译文时,他们是否会表现出另一个方向(即生产性学习)的迁移?在三项实验中,我们通过调整学习计划的顺序(先接受后生产或反之)来回答这个问题。在给定的学习计划中,被试继续使用 "放弃 "法练习检索译文(带反馈),直到他们正确地回忆起每个译文三次为止;然后,他们继续进行相反方向的对译练习,直到他们正确地回忆起每个译文三次为止。在所有实验中,迁移都是部分的(在一个方向上的学习并不能完全消除在另一个方向上的练习需要),但迁移确实发生了,无论学生在练习时首先使用哪个时间表。
{"title":"Toward mastering foreign-language translations: transfer between productive and receptive learning.","authors":"Emma Bernardi, Kalif E Vaughn, John Dunlosky, Katherine A Rawson","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2397043","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2397043","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learners can study foreign language-English vocabulary (e.g., <i>denken</i> - to think) both receptively and productively. Receptive learning involves being cued with a foreign language word (e.g., <i>denken</i>) and trying to translate it (i.e., to think). Productive learning involves being cued with an English word (e.g., to think) and trying to produce the translation. When students use retrieval practice to learn foreign-language translations in one direction (e.g., receptively) until they correctly recall the translation, do they demonstrate transfer in the other direction (i.e., productively)? Across three experiments, we answered this question by manipulating the order of learning schedule (reception first followed by production or vice versa). For a given schedule, participants continued to practice retrieving translations (with feedback) using the dropout method until they correctly recalled each translation three times; they then proceeded to practice the pairs in the opposite direction until they correctly recalled each translation three times. Across all experiments, transfer was partial (learning in one direction did not entirely eliminate the need to practice in the other), but transfer did occur regardless of which schedule students used first during practice.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142120221","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Accuracy and completeness of autobiographical memory: evidence from a wearable camera study. 自传体记忆的准确性和完整性:来自可穿戴相机研究的证据。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2377193
Jason R Finley, William F Brewer

A small wearable camera, SenseCam, passively captured pictures from everyday experience that were later used to evaluate the accuracy and completeness of autobiographical memory. Nine undergraduates wore SenseCams that took pictures every 10 s for two days. After one week and one month, participants first recalled their experiences from specific time periods (timeslices), then reviewed the corresponding pictures to make corrections and report information omitted from initial recall. Results demonstrated the utility of wearable cameras as research tools, and illustrated several characteristics of everyday memory. Recall contents reflected the structure of undergraduate lives. Three different types of omissions were reported: neglected, reminded, and forgotten. Pictures stimulated memory, even for non-visual information (e.g., feelings, thoughts), increasing recall by 23%. The mean completeness of initial recall was 79% (upper bound), with at least 21% forgetting. Accuracy was self-scored by participants (M = 89%), and the mean error rate (11%) provided evidence against strong reconstructive and copy theories of memory. The characteristics of errors shed light on the cognitive processes underlying them. Ratings of recall (confidence, reliving, knowledge, and frequency) supported the episodic/semantic distinction, the dual-process theory of repetition, and reconstructive imagery. Metamemory measures showed a positive correlation between confidence and accuracy.

小型可穿戴相机 SenseCam 可被动捕捉日常经历中的照片,随后用于评估自传体记忆的准确性和完整性。九名大学生佩戴了 SenseCam,每隔 10 秒钟拍摄一次照片,持续两天。一周和一个月后,参与者首先回忆特定时间段(时间片)内的经历,然后查看相应的图片,对最初回忆中遗漏的信息进行更正和报告。研究结果证明了可穿戴相机作为研究工具的实用性,并说明了日常记忆的几个特点。回忆内容反映了大学生的生活结构。报告了三种不同类型的遗漏:忽略、提醒和遗忘。即使是非视觉信息(如感觉、想法),图片也能刺激记忆,使回忆率提高 23%。初始记忆的平均完整率为 79%(上限),遗忘率至少为 21%。准确率由参与者自我评分(M = 89%),平均错误率(11%)提供了与记忆的重建和复制理论相悖的证据。错误的特征揭示了错误背后的认知过程。对回忆的评分(信心、重温、知识和频率)支持情节/语义区分、重复的双重过程理论和重构意象。元记忆测量结果显示,信心和准确性之间存在正相关。
{"title":"Accuracy and completeness of autobiographical memory: evidence from a wearable camera study.","authors":"Jason R Finley, William F Brewer","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2377193","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2377193","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A small wearable camera, SenseCam, passively captured pictures from everyday experience that were later used to evaluate the accuracy and completeness of autobiographical memory. Nine undergraduates wore SenseCams that took pictures every 10 s for two days. After one week and one month, participants first recalled their experiences from specific time periods (timeslices), then reviewed the corresponding pictures to make corrections and report information omitted from initial recall. Results demonstrated the utility of wearable cameras as research tools, and illustrated several characteristics of everyday memory. Recall contents reflected the structure of undergraduate lives. Three different types of omissions were reported: neglected, reminded, and forgotten. Pictures stimulated memory, even for non-visual information (e.g., feelings, thoughts), increasing recall by 23%. The mean completeness of initial recall was 79% (upper bound), with at least 21% forgetting. Accuracy was self-scored by participants (<i>M</i> = 89%), and the mean error rate (11%) provided evidence against strong reconstructive and copy theories of memory. The characteristics of errors shed light on the cognitive processes underlying them. Ratings of recall (confidence, reliving, knowledge, and frequency) supported the episodic/semantic distinction, the dual-process theory of repetition, and reconstructive imagery. Metamemory measures showed a positive correlation between confidence and accuracy.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141633900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1