首页 > 最新文献

Memory最新文献

英文 中文
Development of self-derivation through memory integration and relations with world knowledge. 通过记忆整合以及与世界知识的关系发展自我激励。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2373898
Patricia J Bauer, Jessica A Dugan, Lucy M Cronin-Golomb, Katherine A Lee, Britney Del Solar, Melanie Hanft, Alissa G Miller

Accumulating world knowledge is a major task of development and education. The productive process of self-derivation through memory integration seemingly is a valid model of the process. To test the model, we examined relations between generation and retention of new factual knowledge via self-derivation through integration and world knowledge as measured by standardised assessments. We also tested whether the productive process of self-derivation predicted world knowledge even when a measure of learning through direct instruction also was considered. Participants were 162 children ages 8-12 years (53% female; 15% Black, 6% Asian, 1% Arab, 66% White, 5% mixed race, 7% unreported; 1% Latinx). Age accounted for a maximum of 4% of variance in self-derivation and retention. In contrast, substantial individual variability related to general knowledge and content knowledge in several domains, explaining 20-40% variance. In each domain for which self-derivation performance was a unique predictor, it explained a nominally greater share of the variance than the measure of learning through direct instruction. The findings imply that individual variability in self-derivation has functional consequences for accumulation of semantic knowledge across the elementary-school years.

积累世界知识是发展和教育的主要任务。通过记忆整合进行自我减负的生产过程似乎是这一过程的有效模式。为了检验这一模型,我们研究了通过整合自我减退产生和保留新的事实知识与通过标准化评估衡量的世界知识之间的关系。我们还测试了即使考虑到通过直接指导学习的衡量标准,自我衍生的生产过程是否也能预测世界知识。参与者为 162 名 8-12 岁的儿童(53% 为女性;15% 为黑人,6% 为亚洲人,1% 为阿拉伯人,66% 为白人,5% 为混血儿,7% 未报告;1% 为拉丁裔)。年龄最多占自我激励和保持率差异的 4%。与此相反,个体差异与几个领域的常识和内容知识有关,占 20-40% 的差异。在每一个以自我激励表现为独特预测指标的领域中,自我激励表现所能解释的方差份额都要比直接指导学习所能解释的方差份额大。研究结果表明,自我激励的个体差异对小学阶段语义知识的积累具有功能性影响。
{"title":"Development of self-derivation through memory integration and relations with world knowledge.","authors":"Patricia J Bauer, Jessica A Dugan, Lucy M Cronin-Golomb, Katherine A Lee, Britney Del Solar, Melanie Hanft, Alissa G Miller","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2373898","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2373898","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Accumulating world knowledge is a major task of development and education. The productive process of self-derivation through memory integration seemingly is a valid model of the process. To test the model, we examined relations between generation and retention of new factual knowledge via self-derivation through integration and world knowledge as measured by standardised assessments. We also tested whether the productive process of self-derivation predicted world knowledge even when a measure of learning through direct instruction also was considered. Participants were 162 children ages 8-12 years (53% female; 15% Black, 6% Asian, 1% Arab, 66% White, 5% mixed race, 7% unreported; 1% Latinx). Age accounted for a maximum of 4% of variance in self-derivation and retention. In contrast, substantial individual variability related to general knowledge and content knowledge in several domains, explaining 20-40% variance. In each domain for which self-derivation performance was a unique predictor, it explained a nominally greater share of the variance than the measure of learning through direct instruction. The findings imply that individual variability in self-derivation has functional consequences for accumulation of semantic knowledge across the elementary-school years.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11347116/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141538128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Associative asymmetry of the recognition without cued-recall effect in thematic relations. 主题关系中无诱导回忆效应的联想不对称识别。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2397037
Yongping Jia, Guixiong Liu, Lingling Xue, Shengfeng Zhang, Zhengwei Lai, Chunhuan Huang

In the present study, two experiments were conducted to examine whether thematic relation can produce recognition without cued-recall effect and whether the direction of the association has an influence on this effect. The participants provided higher familiarity ratings for studied items than for unstudied items during target retrieval failure. Additionally, the thematic relation-elicited recognition without cued-recall effect was larger in the forward association than in the backward association. Collectively, these results indicated that thematic relations can elicit the recognition without cued-recall effect, and this effect is asymmetrical. The current findings support the features overlap hypothesis described in the global match model.

在本研究中,我们进行了两项实验来考察主题关系是否能产生无提示唤回效应的识别,以及联想的方向是否会对这种效应产生影响。在目标检索失败时,被试对研究过的项目的熟悉度评分高于未研究过的项目。此外,在前向联想中,主题关系引发的无提示唤回识别效应要大于后向联想。总之,这些结果表明,主题关系可以引起无提示唤醒识别效应,而且这种效应是不对称的。目前的研究结果支持全局匹配模型中描述的特征重叠假说。
{"title":"Associative asymmetry of the recognition without cued-recall effect in thematic relations.","authors":"Yongping Jia, Guixiong Liu, Lingling Xue, Shengfeng Zhang, Zhengwei Lai, Chunhuan Huang","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2397037","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2397037","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the present study, two experiments were conducted to examine whether thematic relation can produce recognition without cued-recall effect and whether the direction of the association has an influence on this effect. The participants provided higher familiarity ratings for studied items than for unstudied items during target retrieval failure. Additionally, the thematic relation-elicited recognition without cued-recall effect was larger in the forward association than in the backward association. Collectively, these results indicated that thematic relations can elicit the recognition without cued-recall effect, and this effect is asymmetrical. The current findings support the features overlap hypothesis described in the global match model.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142080867","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pupil old/new effect as an objective measure of recognition memory: a meta-analysis of 17 eye-tracking experiments. 瞳孔新旧效应作为识别记忆的客观衡量标准:对 17 项眼球跟踪实验的荟萃分析。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-27 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2395367
Alexandra Lapteva, Corinna S Martarelli

Recognition memory, the ability to recognise previously encountered information, correlates with pupil diameter changes during the recognition period. This physiological response, known as the pupil old/new effect, generally reflects the variation in pupil dilation when encountering previously studied (old) stimuli compared to new stimuli. To develop a more precise understanding of the pupil old/new effect, we conducted a meta-analysis of 17 eye-tracking experiments (across 12 articles spanning from 2008 to 2023) involving 560 healthy adults with a mean age of 22.31 years. Analysis of publication bias showed a rather low risk of bias in the selected articles. The main meta-analysis revealed a significant and large pooled pupil old/new effect (Cohen's dz = 0.73, 95% CI [0.50, 0.95]). Further analysis of moderators showed that the number of participants included in the experiments and the criteria for selecting trials (only correct trials vs. all trials) had a significant impact on the meta-analytic results. In general, the analyses revealed a robust pupil old/new effect across all selected articles. This finding underscores its potential utility as a marker of recognition memory across different stimuli type, and various experimental designs.

识别记忆,即识别以前遇到的信息的能力,与识别期间瞳孔直径的变化有关。这种生理反应被称为瞳孔新旧效应,一般反映了在遇到以前学习过的(旧的)刺激与新刺激时瞳孔放大的变化。为了更准确地理解瞳孔新旧效应,我们对 17 项眼球追踪实验(涉及 12 篇文章,时间跨度从 2008 年到 2023 年)进行了荟萃分析,这些实验涉及 560 名健康成年人,他们的平均年龄为 22.31 岁。发表偏倚分析表明,所选文章的偏倚风险较低。主要的荟萃分析表明,瞳孔新旧效应显著且较大(Cohen's dz = 0.73,95% CI [0.50,0.95])。对调节因子的进一步分析表明,实验中的参与者人数和试验选择标准(仅正确试验与所有试验)对元分析结果有显著影响。总的来说,分析结果表明,在所有选定的文章中,瞳孔新旧效应都很明显。这一发现强调了瞳孔旧/新效应作为识别记忆标记的潜在作用,可以跨越不同的刺激类型和各种实验设计。
{"title":"Pupil old/new effect as an objective measure of recognition memory: a meta-analysis of 17 eye-tracking experiments.","authors":"Alexandra Lapteva, Corinna S Martarelli","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2395367","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2395367","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recognition memory, the ability to recognise previously encountered information, correlates with pupil diameter changes during the recognition period. This physiological response, known as the pupil old/new effect, generally reflects the variation in pupil dilation when encountering previously studied (old) stimuli compared to new stimuli. To develop a more precise understanding of the pupil old/new effect, we conducted a meta-analysis of 17 eye-tracking experiments (across 12 articles spanning from 2008 to 2023) involving 560 healthy adults with a mean age of 22.31 years. Analysis of publication bias showed a rather low risk of bias in the selected articles. The main meta-analysis revealed a significant and large pooled pupil old/new effect (Cohen's <i>d<sub>z</sub></i> = 0.73, 95% CI [0.50, 0.95]). Further analysis of moderators showed that the number of participants included in the experiments and the criteria for selecting trials (only correct trials vs. all trials) had a significant impact on the meta-analytic results. In general, the analyses revealed a robust pupil old/new effect across all selected articles. This finding underscores its potential utility as a marker of recognition memory across different stimuli type, and various experimental designs.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142080868","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Autobiographical memory phenomenology in transgender and cisgender individuals. 变性人和同性人的自传体记忆现象学。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2387106
Azriel Grysman, Caleb Schlaupitz, Jennifer G Bohanek, Angela F Lukowski

Transgender individuals face challenges to identity as they transition from their sex assigned at birth to their affirmed gender. Memories may support a sense of self through the recall of events with more phenomenological detail than others, making them feel closer to the current self. Autobiographical memories of 90 transgender and 90 cisgender adults were compared on self-reported memory phenomenology. Memory phenomenology was more variable in transgender individuals, with a larger difference between phenomenological ratings of recent and distant memories. Memory phenomenology specifically varied in relation to the timing of coming out to a parent. High points reported after this time were rated with higher phenomenological quality and these ratings were linked to positive well-being. Results affirm the relevance of phenomenological continuity to the identity of transgender individuals, suggesting that events from before coming out are recalled with less phenomenological quality than events after coming out.

变性人在从出生时指定的性别过渡到确认的性别时,面临着身份认同的挑战。记忆可能会通过回忆比其他人更多现象细节的事件来支持自我意识,使他们感觉更接近当前的自我。我们对 90 名变性成人和 90 名顺性成人的自传体记忆进行了自我报告记忆现象学方面的比较。变性人的记忆现象学差异更大,对近期记忆和远期记忆的现象学评价差异更大。记忆现象学的具体差异与向父母出柜的时间有关。在这一时间之后报告的高点被评为具有较高的现象学质量,这些评定与积极的幸福感有关。研究结果肯定了现象学连续性与变性人身份的相关性,表明变性人在回忆出柜前的事件时,其现象学质量低于出柜后的事件。
{"title":"Autobiographical memory phenomenology in transgender and cisgender individuals.","authors":"Azriel Grysman, Caleb Schlaupitz, Jennifer G Bohanek, Angela F Lukowski","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2387106","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2387106","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Transgender individuals face challenges to identity as they transition from their sex assigned at birth to their affirmed gender. Memories may support a sense of self through the recall of events with more phenomenological detail than others, making them feel closer to the current self. Autobiographical memories of 90 transgender and 90 cisgender adults were compared on self-reported memory phenomenology. Memory phenomenology was more variable in transgender individuals, with a larger difference between phenomenological ratings of recent and distant memories. Memory phenomenology specifically varied in relation to the timing of coming out to a parent. High points reported after this time were rated with higher phenomenological quality and these ratings were linked to positive well-being. Results affirm the relevance of phenomenological continuity to the identity of transgender individuals, suggesting that events from before coming out are recalled with less phenomenological quality than events after coming out.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142073248","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence for separate backward recall and n-back working memory factors: a large-scale latent variable analysis. 单独的后向回忆和 n-back 工作记忆因素的证据:大规模潜变量分析。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2393388
Elizabeth M Byrne, Rebecca A Gilbert, Rogier A Kievit, Joni Holmes

Multiple studies have explored the factor structure of working memory (WM) tasks, yet few have done so controlling for both the domain and category of the memory items in a single study. In the current pre-registered study, we conducted a large-scale latent variable analysis using variant forms of n-back and backward recall tasks to test whether they measured a single underlying construct, or were distinguished by stimuli-, domain-, or paradigm-specific factors. Exploratory analyses investigated how the resulting WM factor(s) were linked to fluid intelligence. Participants (N = 703) completed a fluid reasoning test and multiple n-back and backward recall tasks containing memoranda that varied across (spatial or verbal material) and within (verbal digits or letters) domain, allowing the variance specific to task content and paradigm to be assessed. Two distinct but related backward recall and n-back constructs best captured the data, in comparison to other plausible model constructions (single WM factor, two-factor domain, and three-factor materials models). Common variance associated with WM was a stronger predictor of fluid reasoning than a residual n-back factor, but the backward recall factor predicted fluid reasoning as strongly as the common WM factor. These data emphasise the distinctiveness between backward recall and n-back tasks.

已有多项研究探讨了工作记忆(WM)任务的因素结构,但很少有研究在一项研究中同时控制记忆项目的领域和类别。在当前的预注册研究中,我们使用不同形式的正向后退和倒退回忆任务进行了大规模的潜变量分析,以检验它们是测量单一的基本结构,还是由刺激、领域或范式特定的因素区分开来。探索性分析研究了由此得出的WM因子与流体智力之间的联系。参与者(N = 703)完成了一项流体推理测试和多项n-back及倒退回忆任务,这些任务包含不同领域(空间或语言材料)和不同领域内(语言数字或字母)的记忆,从而评估了任务内容和范式的特定变异。与其他可能的模型结构(单一 WM 因子、双因子领域和三因子材料模型)相比,两个不同但相关的后向回忆和 n-back 结构最能捕捉数据。与残余的 n-back 因子相比,与 WM 相关的共同方差对流体推理的预测作用更强,但后向回忆因子对流体推理的预测作用与共同 WM 因子一样强。这些数据强调了后向回忆任务和正向回溯任务之间的区别。
{"title":"Evidence for separate backward recall and <i>n</i>-back working memory factors: a large-scale latent variable analysis.","authors":"Elizabeth M Byrne, Rebecca A Gilbert, Rogier A Kievit, Joni Holmes","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2393388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2393388","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multiple studies have explored the factor structure of working memory (WM) tasks, yet few have done so controlling for both the domain and category of the memory items in a single study. In the current pre-registered study, we conducted a large-scale latent variable analysis using variant forms of n-back and backward recall tasks to test whether they measured a single underlying construct, or were distinguished by stimuli-, domain-, or paradigm-specific factors. Exploratory analyses investigated how the resulting WM factor(s) were linked to fluid intelligence. Participants (<i>N</i> = 703) completed a fluid reasoning test and multiple n-back and backward recall tasks containing memoranda that varied across (spatial or verbal material) and within (verbal digits or letters) domain, allowing the variance specific to task content and paradigm to be assessed. Two distinct but related backward recall and n-back constructs best captured the data, in comparison to other plausible model constructions (single WM factor, two-factor domain, and three-factor materials models). Common variance associated with WM was a stronger predictor of fluid reasoning than a residual n-back factor, but the backward recall factor predicted fluid reasoning as strongly as the common WM factor. These data emphasise the distinctiveness between backward recall and n-back tasks.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142073249","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming: the role of cue repetition. 从语义到自传的记忆引物:线索重复的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-21 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2393782
John H Mace, Sophia R Keller, Kenneth E Ingle

It is now well established that general information processing causes the activation of memories in the autobiographical memory system, and these memories on occasion emerge as involuntary autobiographical memories. This priming phenomenon has been dubbed semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming, and our goal in the current study was to examine the effects of cue/prime repetition on the production of involuntary autobiographical memories that were primed with semantic stimuli. In three experiments, participants were primed with words (e.g., cat), and then they were given an involuntary memory task (the vigilance task), which contained cues related to the primed stimuli. In Experiment 1, the cues were phrases containing the primes (e.g., getting a cat), which were presented one or five times. In Experiment 2, the cues were also phrases containing the primes (e.g., getting a cat), but they changed their context (e.g., feeding a cat), every time they repeated in the five-presentation condition. Experiment 3 also presented the cues one or five times, but the cues were replicas of the primes (e.g., cat). Consistent with predictions, greater priming was found in the five-presentation cue conditions in all three experiments, and Experiment 3 failed to find priming in the one-presentation cue condition, also consistent with predictions. We explain the findings in terms of semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming theory, and also argue that the results help explain the production of involuntary memories in everyday life.

一般信息处理会激活自传体记忆系统中的记忆,这些记忆有时会以非自主自传体记忆的形式出现,这一点现已得到公认。这种引物现象被称为语义到自传体记忆引物,我们目前的研究目标是考察线索/引物重复对产生以语义刺激为引物的非自主自传体记忆的影响。在三项实验中,我们先用单词(如猫)作为诱导,然后给被试布置一项非自主记忆任务(警觉任务),其中包含与诱导刺激相关的线索。在实验 1 中,提示词是包含引物的短语(例如:得到一只猫),这些短语会出现一次或五次。在实验 2 中,提示语也是包含引物的短语(例如:得到一只猫),但在五次呈现条件下,每次重复呈现时,提示语的语境都会发生变化(例如:喂猫)。实验 3 也是将线索呈现一次或五次,但线索是原语(如猫)的复制品。与预测结果一致的是,在所有三个实验中,五次呈现线索条件下的引诱作用更大,而实验 3 在一次呈现线索条件下没有发现引诱作用,这也与预测结果一致。我们从语义到自传体记忆引物理论的角度解释了这些发现,并认为这些结果有助于解释日常生活中非自主记忆的产生。
{"title":"Semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming: the role of cue repetition.","authors":"John H Mace, Sophia R Keller, Kenneth E Ingle","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2393782","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2393782","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is now well established that general information processing causes the activation of memories in the autobiographical memory system, and these memories on occasion emerge as involuntary autobiographical memories. This priming phenomenon has been dubbed semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming, and our goal in the current study was to examine the effects of cue/prime repetition on the production of involuntary autobiographical memories that were primed with semantic stimuli. In three experiments, participants were primed with words (e.g., <i>cat</i>), and then they were given an involuntary memory task (the vigilance task), which contained cues related to the primed stimuli. In Experiment 1, the cues were phrases containing the primes (e.g., <i>getting a cat</i>), which were presented one or five times. In Experiment 2, the cues were also phrases containing the primes (e.g., <i>getting a cat</i>), but they changed their context (e.g., <i>feeding a cat</i>), every time they repeated in the five-presentation condition. Experiment 3 also presented the cues one or five times, but the cues were replicas of the primes (e.g., <i>cat</i>). Consistent with predictions, greater priming was found in the five-presentation cue conditions in all three experiments, and Experiment 3 failed to find priming in the one-presentation cue condition, also consistent with predictions. We explain the findings in terms of semantic-to-autobiographical memory priming theory, and also argue that the results help explain the production of involuntary memories in everyday life.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142017968","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Did I tell you something personal? The influence of the distinctive features on destination memory. 我跟你说了什么私事吗?独特特征对目的地记忆的影响
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2391407
Raquel Pinto, Pedro B Albuquerque

Several studies observed that a worse destination memory (i.e., capacity to remember to whom we said something) occurs when personal facts are shared, which was explained based on the internal attentional focus - the attentional focus is on the information and not on the recipient of the information. So, with two experiments, we aimed to mitigate the negative influence of the internal attentional focus on destination memory. Since it was previously observed that sharing information with distinctive faces leads to a better destination memory, in Experiment 1, participants (N = 30) were asked to transmit personal facts to distinctive and undistinctive faces. No differences were observed. To increase the attentional focus on the recipient of the information, in Experiment 2, participants (N = 30) were also asked to evaluate the distinctiveness of the recipients' faces. A better destination memory was not observed in Experiment 2 compared with Experiment 1. This leads us to conclude that asking participants to evaluate the faces did not promote a better destination memory when personal facts were shared. Nevertheless, by asking to evaluate the faces, the attentional focus was on the faces, where distinctive faces attracted more attention and led to a better destination memory.

有几项研究发现,在分享个人事实时,目的地记忆(即记住我们对谁说过什么的能力)会变差,这是由内部注意焦点引起的--注意焦点集中在信息上,而不是信息的接收者。因此,我们通过两个实验来减轻内部注意焦点对目的地记忆的负面影响。由于之前已经观察到与独特面孔分享信息会带来更好的目的地记忆,因此在实验一中,我们要求参与者(N = 30)向独特面孔和非独特面孔传递个人事实。结果没有发现差异。为了将注意力更多地集中在信息接收者身上,实验 2 还要求参与者(30 人)评估接收者面孔的独特性。与实验 1 相比,实验 2 中没有观察到更好的目的地记忆。由此我们可以得出结论:在分享个人事实时,要求参与者对面孔进行评价并不能促进更好的目的地记忆。然而,通过要求对面孔进行评价,注意力集中到了面孔上,而与众不同的面孔吸引了更多的注意力,并导致了更好的目的地记忆。
{"title":"Did I tell you something personal? The influence of the distinctive features on destination memory.","authors":"Raquel Pinto, Pedro B Albuquerque","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2391407","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2391407","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several studies observed that a worse destination memory (i.e., capacity to remember to whom we said something) occurs when personal facts are shared, which was explained based on the internal attentional focus - the attentional focus is on the information and not on the recipient of the information. So, with two experiments, we aimed to mitigate the negative influence of the internal attentional focus on destination memory. Since it was previously observed that sharing information with distinctive faces leads to a better destination memory, in Experiment 1, participants (<i>N</i> = 30) were asked to transmit personal facts to distinctive and undistinctive faces. No differences were observed. To increase the attentional focus on the recipient of the information, in Experiment 2, participants (<i>N</i> = 30) were also asked to evaluate the distinctiveness of the recipients' faces. A better destination memory was not observed in Experiment 2 compared with Experiment 1. This leads us to conclude that asking participants to evaluate the faces did not promote a better destination memory when personal facts were shared. Nevertheless, by asking to evaluate the faces, the attentional focus was on the faces, where distinctive faces attracted more attention and led to a better destination memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142000389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An exploration of Italian laypeople's belief in how human memory works. 探讨意大利普通人对人类记忆运作方式的看法。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2387093
Mara Stockner, Gianmarco Convertino, Jessica Talbot, Michela Marchetti, Danilo Mitaritonna, Marta Vicario, Giuliana Mazzoni

We present the first study to measure the beliefs held by Italian laypeople about how human memory works, using a newly developed tool: the Italian Memory Belief Questionnaire (IMBQ). Research conducted in other countries has demonstrated that beliefs about memory vary widely between different professional and non-professional groups, indicating that limitations exist regarding the dissemination of empirically researched scientific knowledge. To ascertain what Italian people understand about memory-related topics, including eyewitness testimony, repression of traumatic memories and factors influencing memory recall, 301 native Italian participants completed the IMBQ in Study 1. In Study 2, 346 additional participants completed the IMBQ, alongside various additional measures, to examine the construct validity of our new instrument and investigate socio-demographic predictors of memory beliefs. Exploratory factor analysis in Study 1 identified three distinct belief factors that were present in the dataset: eyewitness and memory reliability, trauma and remembering and aspects that improve remembering. Study 2 partially confirmed this factor structure and found IMBQ scores to correlate with existing memory belief questionnaires. Correlations were also found between the IMBQ subscales and measures of fantasy proneness, but not dissociation. In both studies, many Italian laypeople strongly endorsed the notion that controversial topics (i.e., repression) are possible. Contrastingly, Italian laypeople do appear to understand the conceivable inaccuracies of memory in eyewitness settings. Sex, age and education were shown to predict beliefs about memory. Findings are discussed in relation to the importance of addressing misinformation about memory, especially in clinical and forensic settings.

我们利用新开发的工具:意大利记忆信念问卷(IMBQ),首次对意大利非专业人士对人类记忆如何发挥作用所持有的信念进行了测量。在其他国家进行的研究表明,不同的专业和非专业群体对记忆的看法大相径庭,这表明在传播经过实证研究的科学知识方面存在着局限性。为了弄清意大利人对记忆相关主题的理解,包括目击证人的证词、创伤记忆的压抑和影响记忆回忆的因素,301 名意大利本土参与者在研究 1 中完成了 IMBQ。在研究 2 中,又有 346 名参与者完成了 IMBQ 以及各种附加测量,以检验我们新工具的构建有效性,并调查记忆信念的社会人口预测因素。研究 1 的探索性因子分析确定了数据集中存在的三个不同的信念因子:目击者和记忆可靠性、创伤和记忆以及改善记忆的方面。研究 2 部分证实了这一因素结构,并发现 IMBQ 分数与现有的记忆信念问卷相关。研究还发现,IMBQ 的子量表与幻想倾向的测量之间存在相关性,但不存在分离性。在这两项研究中,许多意大利普通人都强烈赞同有争议的话题(即压抑)是可能存在的。与此相反,意大利的非专业人士似乎确实理解目击者记忆中可能存在的不准确性。性别、年龄和教育程度被证明可以预测对记忆的看法。研究结果与解决记忆错误信息的重要性有关,特别是在临床和法医环境中的重要性。
{"title":"An exploration of Italian laypeople's belief in how human memory works.","authors":"Mara Stockner, Gianmarco Convertino, Jessica Talbot, Michela Marchetti, Danilo Mitaritonna, Marta Vicario, Giuliana Mazzoni","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2387093","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2387093","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present the first study to measure the beliefs held by Italian laypeople about how human memory works, using a newly developed tool: the Italian Memory Belief Questionnaire (IMBQ). Research conducted in other countries has demonstrated that beliefs about memory vary widely between different professional and non-professional groups, indicating that limitations exist regarding the dissemination of empirically researched scientific knowledge. To ascertain what Italian people understand about memory-related topics, including eyewitness testimony, repression of traumatic memories and factors influencing memory recall, 301 native Italian participants completed the IMBQ in Study 1. In Study 2, 346 additional participants completed the IMBQ, alongside various additional measures, to examine the construct validity of our new instrument and investigate socio-demographic predictors of memory beliefs. Exploratory factor analysis in Study 1 identified three distinct belief factors that were present in the dataset: eyewitness and memory reliability, trauma and remembering and aspects that improve remembering. Study 2 partially confirmed this factor structure and found IMBQ scores to correlate with existing memory belief questionnaires. Correlations were also found between the IMBQ subscales and measures of fantasy proneness, but not dissociation. In both studies, many Italian laypeople strongly endorsed the notion that controversial topics (i.e., repression) are possible. Contrastingly, Italian laypeople do appear to understand the conceivable inaccuracies of memory in eyewitness settings. Sex, age and education were shown to predict beliefs about memory. Findings are discussed in relation to the importance of addressing misinformation about memory, especially in clinical and forensic settings.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141907051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of attention and verbal rehearsal in remembering more valuable item-colour binding. 注意力和语言预演在记忆更有价值的物品-颜色结合中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-08 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2389177
Xiaotong Yin, Jelena Havelka, Richard J Allen

Selectively remembering more valuable information can improve memory efficiency. Such value effects have been observed on long-term memory for item-colour binding, but the possible contributory factors are unclear. The current study explored contributions from attention (Experiment 1) and verbal rehearsal (Experiment 2). Across two experiments, memory was superior for item-colour bindings that were associated with high (relative to low) point values at encoding, both in an immediate test and a delayed re-test. When availability of attentional resources was reduced during encoding, value only influenced immediate and not delayed memory (Experiment 1). This indicates that a transient value effect can be obtained with little attentional resources, but attentional resources are involved in creating a longer lasting effect. When articulatory suppression was implemented during encoding (Experiment 2), value effects were somewhat reduced in the immediate test and abolished in the delayed re-test, suggesting a role for verbal rehearsal in value effects on item-colour binding memory. These patterns of value effects did not interact with encoding presentation format (i.e., sequential vs. simultaneous presentation of objects). Together, these results suggest that attentional resources and verbal rehearsal both contribute to value effects on item-colour binding memory, with varying impacts on the durability of these effects.

选择性记忆更有价值的信息可以提高记忆效率。这种价值效应已在物品-颜色结合的长期记忆中被观察到,但可能的促成因素尚不清楚。本研究探讨了注意力(实验 1)和言语预演(实验 2)的贡献。在两个实验中,无论是即时测试还是延迟再测试,对于编码时与高点值(相对于低点值)相关的项目-颜色绑定的记忆都更出色。当编码时注意力资源减少时,价值只影响即时记忆,而不影响延迟记忆(实验 1)。这表明,只需很少的注意力资源就能获得短暂的价值效应,但注意力资源参与产生更持久的效应。当在编码过程中实施发音抑制时(实验 2),价值效应在即时测试中有所减弱,而在延迟再测试中则消失了,这表明言语排练在价值效应对项目-颜色结合记忆的影响中起了作用。这些价值效应模式与编码呈现形式(即顺序呈现与同时呈现对象)并无相互作用。总之,这些结果表明,注意资源和言语预演都会对物品-颜色结合记忆的价值效应产生影响,但对这些效应的持久性有不同的影响。
{"title":"The role of attention and verbal rehearsal in remembering more valuable item-colour binding.","authors":"Xiaotong Yin, Jelena Havelka, Richard J Allen","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2389177","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2389177","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Selectively remembering more valuable information can improve memory efficiency. Such value effects have been observed on long-term memory for item-colour binding, but the possible contributory factors are unclear. The current study explored contributions from attention (Experiment 1) and verbal rehearsal (Experiment 2). Across two experiments, memory was superior for item-colour bindings that were associated with high (relative to low) point values at encoding, both in an immediate test and a delayed re-test. When availability of attentional resources was reduced during encoding, value only influenced immediate and not delayed memory (Experiment 1). This indicates that a transient value effect can be obtained with little attentional resources, but attentional resources are involved in creating a longer lasting effect. When articulatory suppression was implemented during encoding (Experiment 2), value effects were somewhat reduced in the immediate test and abolished in the delayed re-test, suggesting a role for verbal rehearsal in value effects on item-colour binding memory. These patterns of value effects did not interact with encoding presentation format (i.e., sequential vs. simultaneous presentation of objects). Together, these results suggest that attentional resources and verbal rehearsal both contribute to value effects on item-colour binding memory, with varying impacts on the durability of these effects.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141907052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of prior familiarisation and meaningfulness of verbal and visual stimuli on directed forgetting. 语言和视觉刺激的先前熟悉程度和意义对定向遗忘的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-24 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2358126
Yi-Pei Lo, Huiyu Ding, Jonathon Whitlock, Lili Sahakyan

ABSTRACTIntentional forgetting of unwanted information is a crucial cognitive function that is often studied with directed forgetting (DF) procedure, whereby cuing some study materials with Forget (F) instruction impairs their memory compared to cuing with Remember (R) instruction. This study investigates how the nature of information (verbal or pictorial), its semantic significance (meaningful or meaningless), and the degree of prior episodic familiarity influence DF. Before the DF phase, stimuli were familiarised by pre-exposing them 0, 2, or 6 times in a prior preview phase. Finally, memory for all items was assessed with old/new recognition test. Experiment 1 employed words, Experiment 2 utilised fractal images, Experiment 3 featured both meaningful and meaningless object images, and Experiment 4 used words and nonwords. Our results indicate that materials that produced better memory performance are not always harder to intentionally forget. Previewed items showed reduced DF compared to non-previewed items regardless of the nature of information, and meaningless stimuli are challenging to intentionally forget regardless of their degrees of familiarisation unless they are meaningless verbal materials. Collectively, the results highlight the importance of joint consideration of the stimulus format, its meaningfulness, and its episodic familiarity in understanding conditions that interact with intentional forgetting.

ABSTRACT Intentional forgetting of unwanted information is a crucial cognitive function that is often studied with directed forgetting (DF) procedure, which cuing some study materials with Forget (F) instruction compared to cuing with Remember (R) instruction.本研究探讨了信息的性质(语言或图像)、语义意义(有意义或无意义)以及先前的记忆熟悉程度如何影响定向遗忘。在 DF 阶段之前,先在预览阶段对刺激物进行 0 次、2 次或 6 次预览,以熟悉刺激物。最后,通过新旧识别测试评估对所有项目的记忆。实验 1 使用单词,实验 2 使用分形图像,实验 3 同时使用有意义和无意义的物体图像,实验 4 使用单词和非单词。我们的结果表明,记忆效果更好的材料并不总是更难被有意遗忘。与非预览项目相比,无论信息的性质如何,预览项目的DF值都有所降低;而无论熟悉程度如何,除非是无意义的语言材料,否则无意义的刺激都很难被有意遗忘。总之,这些结果突出表明,在理解与有意遗忘相互作用的条件时,必须同时考虑刺激形式、刺激的意义和刺激的偶发性熟悉程度。
{"title":"The role of prior familiarisation and meaningfulness of verbal and visual stimuli on directed forgetting.","authors":"Yi-Pei Lo, Huiyu Ding, Jonathon Whitlock, Lili Sahakyan","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2358126","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2358126","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>ABSTRACT</b>Intentional forgetting of unwanted information is a crucial cognitive function that is often studied with directed forgetting (DF) procedure, whereby cuing some study materials with Forget (F) instruction impairs their memory compared to cuing with Remember (R) instruction. This study investigates how the nature of information (verbal or pictorial), its semantic significance (meaningful or meaningless), and the degree of prior episodic familiarity influence DF. Before the DF phase, stimuli were familiarised by pre-exposing them 0, 2, or 6 times in a prior preview phase. Finally, memory for all items was assessed with old/new recognition test. Experiment 1 employed words, Experiment 2 utilised fractal images, Experiment 3 featured both meaningful and meaningless object images, and Experiment 4 used words and nonwords. Our results indicate that materials that produced better memory performance are not always harder to intentionally forget. Previewed items showed reduced DF compared to non-previewed items regardless of the nature of information, and meaningless stimuli are challenging to intentionally forget regardless of their degrees of familiarisation unless they are meaningless verbal materials. Collectively, the results highlight the importance of joint consideration of the stimulus format, its meaningfulness, and its episodic familiarity in understanding conditions that interact with intentional forgetting.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141093660","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1