首页 > 最新文献

Memory最新文献

英文 中文
The role of culture and semantic organization in working memory updating. 文化和语义组织在工作记忆更新中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2351062
Krystal R Leger, Hannah R Snyder, Aysecan Boduroglu, Angela Gutchess

Westerners tend to relate items in a categorical manner, whereas Easterners focus more on functional relationships. The present study extended research on semantic organization in long-term memory to working memory. First, Americans' and Turks' preferences for categorical versus functional relationships were tested. Second, working memory interference was assessed using a 2-back working memory paradigm in which lure items were categorically and functionally related to targets. Next, a mediation model tested direct effects of culture and semantic organization on working memory task behaviour, and the indirect effect, whether semantic organization mediated the relationship between culture and working memory interference. Whereas Americans had slower response times to correctly rejecting functional lures compared to categorical lures, conditions did not differ for Turks. However, semantic organization did not mediate cultural difference in working memory interference. Across cultures, there was evidence that semantic organization affected working memory errors, with individuals who endorsed categorical more than functional pairings committing more categorical than functional errors on the 2-back task. Results align with prior research suggesting individual differences in use of different types of semantic relationships, and further that literature by indicating effects on interference in working memory. However, these individual differences may not be culture-dependent.

西方人倾向于以分类的方式将项目联系起来,而东方人则更注重功能关系。本研究将有关长时记忆语义组织的研究扩展到了工作记忆。首先,测试了美国人和土耳其人对分类关系和功能关系的偏好。其次,使用2-back工作记忆范式对工作记忆干扰进行评估,在该范式中,引诱项与目标项存在分类和功能关系。接下来,一个中介模型测试了文化和语义组织对工作记忆任务行为的直接影响,以及间接影响,即语义组织是否中介了文化和工作记忆干扰之间的关系。与分类诱饵相比,美国人正确拒绝功能诱饵的反应时间较慢,而土耳其人的情况则没有差异。然而,语义组织并不能调节工作记忆干扰的文化差异。在不同文化中,有证据表明语义组织影响了工作记忆错误,在2-back任务中,赞同分类配对的个体比赞同功能配对的个体犯的分类错误要多于功能错误。研究结果与之前的研究结果一致,表明在使用不同类型的语义关系时存在个体差异,并通过对工作记忆干扰的影响进一步证实了这一点。然而,这些个体差异可能与文化无关。
{"title":"The role of culture and semantic organization in working memory updating.","authors":"Krystal R Leger, Hannah R Snyder, Aysecan Boduroglu, Angela Gutchess","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2351062","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2351062","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Westerners tend to relate items in a categorical manner, whereas Easterners focus more on functional relationships. The present study extended research on semantic organization in long-term memory to working memory. First, Americans' and Turks' preferences for categorical versus functional relationships were tested. Second, working memory interference was assessed using a 2-back working memory paradigm in which lure items were categorically and functionally related to targets. Next, a mediation model tested direct effects of culture and semantic organization on working memory task behaviour, and the indirect effect, whether semantic organization mediated the relationship between culture and working memory interference. Whereas Americans had slower response times to correctly rejecting functional lures compared to categorical lures, conditions did not differ for Turks. However, semantic organization did not mediate cultural difference in working memory interference. Across cultures, there was evidence that semantic organization affected working memory errors, with individuals who endorsed categorical more than functional pairings committing more categorical than functional errors on the 2-back task. Results align with prior research suggesting individual differences in use of different types of semantic relationships, and further that literature by indicating effects on interference in working memory. However, these individual differences may not be culture-dependent.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11262971/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140897671","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do childhood experiences influence associations between posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and positive autobiographical memories among military veteran students? An exploratory study. 童年经历是否会影响退伍大学生的创伤后应激障碍症状与积极自传体记忆之间的关联?一项探索性研究。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-10 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2348685
Alejandro Miguel-Alvaro, Brett A Messman, Nicole H Weiss, Ateka A Contractor

Background: Evidence links posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms and features of positive autobiographical memories (accessibility, vividness, coherence, sharing, emotional intensity, distancing). There is a knowledge gap on how adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and benevolent childhood experiences (BCEs) may influence these relationships.

Objectives: The current study explored whether the number ACEs or BCEs moderated associations between PTSD symptom severity and features of positive autobiographical memories.

Design and methods: The sample included 124 student military veterans who had experienced a trauma (Mage = 33.90; 77.4% male; 75.0% White).

Results: Path analyses showed more PTSD symptom severity was significantly associated with less positive autobiographical memory vividness (β = -0.26, p = .019, R2 = 0.06). Further, the number of ACEs moderated the relationship between PTSD symptom severity and positive autobiographical memory accessibility (β = -0.25, p = .023, R2 = 0.10) and vividness (β = -0.20, p = .024, R2 = 0.10). Among individuals with more ACEs (1 SD above the mean) compared to those with fewer ACEs (1 SD below the mean), less accessibility and vividness of positive autobiographical memories was associated with greater PTSD symptom severity. The number of BCEs was not a significant moderator.

Conclusions: Positive memory-based interventions may be particularly useful to address PTSD symptoms among military veterans with a history of childhood adversity.

背景:有证据表明,创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)症状与积极自传体记忆的特征(可及性、生动性、连贯性、分享性、情感强度、疏远性)有关。关于不良童年经历(ACEs)和良好童年经历(BCEs)如何影响这些关系,目前还存在知识空白:本研究探讨了ACE或BCE的数量是否会调节创伤后应激障碍症状严重程度与积极自传体记忆特征之间的关系:样本包括 124 名经历过创伤的退伍大学生(Mage = 33.90;77.4% 为男性;75.0% 为白人):路径分析显示,创伤后应激障碍症状严重程度越高,自传体记忆生动程度越低(β = -0.26,p = .019,R2 = 0.06)。此外,ACE的数量调节了创伤后应激障碍症状严重程度与积极自传体记忆易得性(β = -0.25,p = .023,R2 = 0.10)和生动性(β = -0.20,p = .024,R2 = 0.10)之间的关系。在有较多 ACE(高于平均值 1 SD)的人中,与那些有较少 ACE(低于平均值 1 SD)的人相比,正面自传体记忆的可及性和生动性较低与创伤后应激障碍症状的严重程度较高有关。BCE的数量并不是一个重要的调节因素:以积极记忆为基础的干预措施可能对有童年逆境史的退伍军人的创伤后应激障碍症状特别有用。
{"title":"Do childhood experiences influence associations between posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms and positive autobiographical memories among military veteran students? An exploratory study.","authors":"Alejandro Miguel-Alvaro, Brett A Messman, Nicole H Weiss, Ateka A Contractor","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2348685","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2348685","url":null,"abstract":"<p><strong>Background: </strong>Evidence links posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms and features of positive autobiographical memories (accessibility, vividness, coherence, sharing, emotional intensity, distancing). There is a knowledge gap on how adverse childhood experiences (ACEs) and benevolent childhood experiences (BCEs) may influence these relationships.</p><p><strong>Objectives: </strong>The current study explored whether the number ACEs or BCEs moderated associations between PTSD symptom severity and features of positive autobiographical memories.</p><p><strong>Design and methods: </strong>The sample included 124 student military veterans who had experienced a trauma (<i>M</i><sub>age </sub>= 33.90; 77.4% male; 75.0% White).</p><p><strong>Results: </strong>Path analyses showed more PTSD symptom severity was significantly associated with less positive autobiographical memory vividness (β = -0.26, <i>p </i>= .019, <i>R<sup>2</sup></i> = 0.06). Further, the number of ACEs moderated the relationship between PTSD symptom severity and positive autobiographical memory accessibility (β = -0.25, <i>p </i>= .023, <i>R<sup>2 </sup></i>= 0.10) and vividness (β = -0.20, <i>p </i>= .024, <i>R<sup>2 </sup></i>= 0.10). Among individuals with more ACEs (1 <i>SD</i> above the mean) compared to those with fewer ACEs (1 <i>SD</i> below the mean), less accessibility and vividness of positive autobiographical memories was associated with greater PTSD symptom severity. The number of BCEs was not a significant moderator.</p><p><strong>Conclusions: </strong>Positive memory-based interventions may be particularly useful to address PTSD symptoms among military veterans with a history of childhood adversity.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140897474","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Event centrality in social anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder. 社交焦虑症和重度抑郁症的事件中心性。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2341706
Mirjam Vermeulen, Amarendra Gandhi, Filip Van Den Eede, Filip Raes, J. Krans
Event centrality is defined by the extent to which a memory of an event has become central to an individual's identity and life story. Previous research predominantly focused on the link between event centrality and trauma-related symptomatology. Nevertheless, it can be argued that the perception of (adverse) events as central to one's self is not exclusive to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Other disorders where adverse events are linked to the onset of symptoms might also be related to event centrality. This study examined the relevance of event centrality for Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) and for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) separately. Moreover, we examined which cognitive and emotion regulation variables (i.e., trait anxiety, rumination, worry, intrusions and avoidance, and posttraumatic cognitions) mediated these relationships. No significant correlation was found between event centrality and social anxiety. However, a significant positive correlation was found between event centrality and depression. In a combined group, this relation was mediated by all cognitive and emotion regulation variables except for worry.
事件中心性是指对某一事件的记忆在多大程度上成为个人身份和生活故事的中心。以往的研究主要集中于事件中心性与创伤相关症状之间的联系。然而,可以说将(不利)事件视为自我的中心并不是创伤后应激障碍(PTSD)所独有的。不良事件与症状发作有关的其他疾病也可能与事件中心性有关。本研究分别研究了事件中心性与社交焦虑症(SAD)和重度抑郁症(MDD)的相关性。此外,我们还研究了哪些认知和情绪调节变量(即特质焦虑、反刍、担忧、侵扰和回避以及创伤后认知)对这些关系起到了中介作用。研究发现,事件中心性与社交焦虑之间没有明显的相关性。然而,事件中心性与抑郁之间存在明显的正相关。在综合组中,除了担忧之外,所有认知和情绪调节变量都对这种关系起到了中介作用。
{"title":"Event centrality in social anxiety disorder and major depressive disorder.","authors":"Mirjam Vermeulen, Amarendra Gandhi, Filip Van Den Eede, Filip Raes, J. Krans","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2341706","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2341706","url":null,"abstract":"Event centrality is defined by the extent to which a memory of an event has become central to an individual's identity and life story. Previous research predominantly focused on the link between event centrality and trauma-related symptomatology. Nevertheless, it can be argued that the perception of (adverse) events as central to one's self is not exclusive to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD). Other disorders where adverse events are linked to the onset of symptoms might also be related to event centrality. This study examined the relevance of event centrality for Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) and for Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) separately. Moreover, we examined which cognitive and emotion regulation variables (i.e., trait anxiety, rumination, worry, intrusions and avoidance, and posttraumatic cognitions) mediated these relationships. No significant correlation was found between event centrality and social anxiety. However, a significant positive correlation was found between event centrality and depression. In a combined group, this relation was mediated by all cognitive and emotion regulation variables except for worry.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140656238","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Deficits in memory metacognitive efficiency in late adulthood are related to distinct brain profile. 成年后期记忆元认知效率的缺陷与独特的大脑特征有关。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-18 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2341711
Francesco Pupillo, Sandra Düzel, S. Kühn, U. Lindenberger, Y. Shing
The tendency of falsely remembering events that did not happen in the past increases with age. This is particularly evident in cases in which features presented at study are re-presented at test in a recombined constellation (termed rearranged pairs). Interestingly, older adults also express high confidence in such false memories, a tendency that may indicate reduced metacognitive efficiency. Within an existing cohort study, we aimed at investigating age-related differences in memory metacognitive efficiency (as measured by meta d' ratio) in a sample of 1522 older adults and 397 young adults. The analysis showed an age-related deficit in metacognition which was more pronounced for rearranged pairs than for new pairs. We then explored associations between cortical thickness and memory metacognitive efficiency for rearranged pairs in a subsample of 231 older adults. By using partial least square analysis, we found that a multivariate profile composed by ventromedial prefrontal cortex, insula, and parahippocampal cortex was uniquely associated with between-person differences in memory metacognitive efficiency. These results suggest that the impairment in memory metacognitive efficiency for false alarms is a distinct age-related deficit, above and beyond a general age-related decline in memory discrimination, and that it is associated with brain regions involved in metacognitive processes.
随着年龄的增长,错误记忆过去未发生事件的倾向也会增加。这在学习时呈现的特征在测试时以重新组合的方式重新呈现(称为重新排列对)的情况下尤为明显。有趣的是,老年人对这种虚假记忆也表现出很高的信心,这种倾向可能表明元认知效率降低了。在现有的一项队列研究中,我们以 1522 名老年人和 397 名年轻人为样本,旨在调查与年龄相关的记忆元认知效率差异(以元d'比值衡量)。分析表明,元认知的缺陷与年龄有关,在重新排列配对时比在新配对时更为明显。随后,我们在 231 位老年人的子样本中探讨了皮层厚度与重新排列配对的记忆元认知效率之间的关联。通过偏最小二乘法分析,我们发现由腹侧前额叶皮层、岛叶和海马旁皮层组成的多变量特征与记忆元认知效率的人际差异有独特的关联。这些结果表明,对虚假警报的记忆元认知效率的损害是一种与年龄相关的独特缺陷,超出了与年龄相关的记忆辨别能力的普遍下降,而且它与元认知过程相关的脑区有关。
{"title":"Deficits in memory metacognitive efficiency in late adulthood are related to distinct brain profile.","authors":"Francesco Pupillo, Sandra Düzel, S. Kühn, U. Lindenberger, Y. Shing","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2341711","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2341711","url":null,"abstract":"The tendency of falsely remembering events that did not happen in the past increases with age. This is particularly evident in cases in which features presented at study are re-presented at test in a recombined constellation (termed rearranged pairs). Interestingly, older adults also express high confidence in such false memories, a tendency that may indicate reduced metacognitive efficiency. Within an existing cohort study, we aimed at investigating age-related differences in memory metacognitive efficiency (as measured by meta d' ratio) in a sample of 1522 older adults and 397 young adults. The analysis showed an age-related deficit in metacognition which was more pronounced for rearranged pairs than for new pairs. We then explored associations between cortical thickness and memory metacognitive efficiency for rearranged pairs in a subsample of 231 older adults. By using partial least square analysis, we found that a multivariate profile composed by ventromedial prefrontal cortex, insula, and parahippocampal cortex was uniquely associated with between-person differences in memory metacognitive efficiency. These results suggest that the impairment in memory metacognitive efficiency for false alarms is a distinct age-related deficit, above and beyond a general age-related decline in memory discrimination, and that it is associated with brain regions involved in metacognitive processes.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140687631","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The self-bias in working memory: the favorability of self-referential stimuli in resource allocation. 工作记忆中的自我偏差:资源分配中自我参照刺激的有利性。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2341709
Shouhang Yin, Antao Chen
Self-representations guide and shape our thoughts and behaviour. People usually exhibit inherent biases in perception, attention, and memory to favour the information associated with themselves over that associated with others. The present study explored the phenomenon of self-bias in working memory (WM), specifically how self-referential processing impacts WM precision. Four precision-based experiments were conducted to assess the recall precision of self-referential items and items associated with other social agents. The findings revealed a robust self-prioritisation effect in WM precision, wherein self-referential items were recalled with greater precision than items associated with other social agents. Additionally, increased precision for self-referential items did not decrease the precision for simultaneously remembered items. This effect was limited by the total amount of WM resources and not influenced by a perceptual distractor. The inherent self-bias in WM can serve as a proxy to access the role self-representation in goal-oriented cognitive processing, providing a means of exploring the interaction between self-reference and high-level cognitive function.
自我表征引导并塑造着我们的思想和行为。人们通常会在感知、注意力和记忆中表现出固有的偏差,即偏爱与自己相关的信息而非与他人相关的信息。本研究探讨了工作记忆(WM)中的自我偏差现象,特别是自我参照处理如何影响工作记忆的精确度。研究人员进行了四项基于精确度的实验,以评估自我参照项目和与其他社会主体相关项目的回忆精确度。实验结果表明,在 WM 精确度方面存在着强烈的自我优先效应,即自我参照项目的回忆精确度要高于与其他社会主体相关的项目。此外,自我参照项目精确度的提高并不会降低同时记忆项目的精确度。这种效应受到 WM 资源总量的限制,不受知觉分心物的影响。WM中固有的自我偏差可以作为一种替代物,用于了解自我表征在目标导向认知加工中的作用,为探索自我参照与高级认知功能之间的相互作用提供了一种方法。
{"title":"The self-bias in working memory: the favorability of self-referential stimuli in resource allocation.","authors":"Shouhang Yin, Antao Chen","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2341709","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2341709","url":null,"abstract":"Self-representations guide and shape our thoughts and behaviour. People usually exhibit inherent biases in perception, attention, and memory to favour the information associated with themselves over that associated with others. The present study explored the phenomenon of self-bias in working memory (WM), specifically how self-referential processing impacts WM precision. Four precision-based experiments were conducted to assess the recall precision of self-referential items and items associated with other social agents. The findings revealed a robust self-prioritisation effect in WM precision, wherein self-referential items were recalled with greater precision than items associated with other social agents. Additionally, increased precision for self-referential items did not decrease the precision for simultaneously remembered items. This effect was limited by the total amount of WM resources and not influenced by a perceptual distractor. The inherent self-bias in WM can serve as a proxy to access the role self-representation in goal-oriented cognitive processing, providing a means of exploring the interaction between self-reference and high-level cognitive function.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140699479","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Time and memory distrust shape the dynamics of recollection and belief-in-occurrence. 时间和对记忆的不信任决定了回忆和相信发生的动力。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-09 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2336166
Yikang Zhang, H. Otgaar, Robert A. Nash, Linda Rosar
The current study examined how people's metamemory judgments of recollection and belief-in-occurrence change over time. Furthermore, we examined to what extent these judgments are affected by memory distrust - the subjective appraisal of one's memory functioning - as measured by the Memory Distrust Scale (MDS) and the Squire Subjective Memory Scale (SSMQ). Participants (N = 234) studied pictorial stimuli and were tested on some of these stimuli later in the same session, but were tested on other stimuli 1, 2, 4, 8, and 17 days later. Recollection and belief ratings were correlated highly and followed similar declining patterns over time. However, belief decreased relatively more slowly than recollection, such that the discrepancy between recollection and belief increased over time. Memory distrust moderated the association between recollection and belief, with this association being weaker among people who reported greater (versus lower) memory distrust. Memory distrust also interacted with retention period to predict memory judgments. Two measures of memory distrust diverged in their predictive power. In particular, only the MDS predicted the spontaneous reporting of nonbelieved memories. Our results provide support to the theoretical perspective that belief-in-occurrence is a summative judgment informed not only by recollective phenomenology but also by metamemorial beliefs.
本研究考察了人们对回忆和相信发生的元记忆判断是如何随时间变化的。此外,我们还研究了这些判断在多大程度上会受到记忆不信任的影响--记忆不信任量表(MDS)和翰格主观记忆量表(SSMQ)测量的是对个人记忆功能的主观评价。受试者(N = 234)学习了图像刺激,并在同一阶段的晚些时候接受了其中一些刺激的测试,但在 1、2、4、8 和 17 天后又接受了其他刺激的测试。回忆和信念的评分高度相关,并且随着时间的推移呈类似的下降模式。然而,信念的下降速度比回忆慢,因此回忆和信念之间的差异随着时间的推移而增大。记忆不信任感调节了回忆与信念之间的关联,这种关联在记忆不信任感较强(相对于较弱)的人中较弱。记忆不信任感还与记忆保持期相互影响,从而预测记忆判断。记忆不信任的两种测量方法在预测能力上存在差异。尤其是,只有 MDS 预测了不信任记忆的自发报告。我们的研究结果为以下理论观点提供了支持:相信发生是一种总结性判断,不仅受回忆现象学的影响,而且还受元记忆信念的影响。
{"title":"Time and memory distrust shape the dynamics of recollection and belief-in-occurrence.","authors":"Yikang Zhang, H. Otgaar, Robert A. Nash, Linda Rosar","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2336166","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2336166","url":null,"abstract":"The current study examined how people's metamemory judgments of recollection and belief-in-occurrence change over time. Furthermore, we examined to what extent these judgments are affected by memory distrust - the subjective appraisal of one's memory functioning - as measured by the Memory Distrust Scale (MDS) and the Squire Subjective Memory Scale (SSMQ). Participants (N = 234) studied pictorial stimuli and were tested on some of these stimuli later in the same session, but were tested on other stimuli 1, 2, 4, 8, and 17 days later. Recollection and belief ratings were correlated highly and followed similar declining patterns over time. However, belief decreased relatively more slowly than recollection, such that the discrepancy between recollection and belief increased over time. Memory distrust moderated the association between recollection and belief, with this association being weaker among people who reported greater (versus lower) memory distrust. Memory distrust also interacted with retention period to predict memory judgments. Two measures of memory distrust diverged in their predictive power. In particular, only the MDS predicted the spontaneous reporting of nonbelieved memories. Our results provide support to the theoretical perspective that belief-in-occurrence is a summative judgment informed not only by recollective phenomenology but also by metamemorial beliefs.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140720753","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
"I heard it before … or not": time-course of ERP response and behavioural correlates associated with false recognition memory. "我以前听过......或没听过":与错误识别记忆相关的 ERP 反应和行为相关性的时间过程。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2333508
N. Pérez-Mata, J. Albert, L. Carretié, S. López-Martín, A. Sánchez-Carmona
Electrophysiological and behavioural correlates of true and false memories were examined in the Deese/Roediger-McDermont (DRM) paradigm. A mass univariate approach for analysing event-related potentials (ERP) in the temporal domain was used to examine the electrophysiological effects associated with this paradigm precisely (point-by-point) and without bias (data-driven). Behaviourally, true and false recognition did not differ, and the predicted DRM effect was observed, as false recognition of critical lures (i.e., new words semantically related to studied words) was higher than false alarms of new (unrelated) words. Neurally, an expected old/new effect was observed during the time-range of the late positive component (LPC) over left centro-parietal scalp electrodes. Furthermore, true recognition also evoked larger LPC amplitudes than false recognition over both left centro-parietal and fronto-central scalp electrodes. However, we did not observe LPC-related differences between critical lures and new words, nor between correct rejections of critical lures and new words. In contrast, correct rejections of critical lures were accompanied by higher activation of a sustained positive slow wave (SPSW) in right fronto-central electrodes beyond 1200 ms. This result reveals a key role of post-retrieval processes in recognition. Results are discussed in light of theoretical approaches to false memory in the DRM paradigm.
在 Deese/Roediger-McDermont(DRM)范式中研究了真假记忆的电生理和行为相关性。该研究采用了一种大规模单变量方法来分析时域中的事件相关电位(ERP),以精确(逐点)和无偏差(数据驱动)地检查与该范式相关的电生理效应。从行为上看,真假识别没有差异,而且观察到了预测的 DRM 效应,即对关键诱饵(即与所学单词语义相关的新单词)的假识别率高于对新单词(不相关)的假警报率。从神经学角度看,在左侧中央顶叶头皮电极上的晚期阳性成分(LPC)的时间范围内观察到了预期的新旧效应。此外,在左侧顶中央和前中央头皮电极上,真识别也比假识别引起更大的 LPC 振幅。然而,我们并没有观察到临界引诱词和新词之间以及正确拒绝临界引诱词和新词之间存在与 LPC 相关的差异。相反,正确拒绝临界引诱的同时,右前中央电极上的持续正慢波(SPSW)激活超过 1200 毫秒。这一结果揭示了检索后过程在识别中的关键作用。本文根据 DRM 范式中的假记忆理论方法对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"\"I heard it before … or not\": time-course of ERP response and behavioural correlates associated with false recognition memory.","authors":"N. Pérez-Mata, J. Albert, L. Carretié, S. López-Martín, A. Sánchez-Carmona","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2333508","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2333508","url":null,"abstract":"Electrophysiological and behavioural correlates of true and false memories were examined in the Deese/Roediger-McDermont (DRM) paradigm. A mass univariate approach for analysing event-related potentials (ERP) in the temporal domain was used to examine the electrophysiological effects associated with this paradigm precisely (point-by-point) and without bias (data-driven). Behaviourally, true and false recognition did not differ, and the predicted DRM effect was observed, as false recognition of critical lures (i.e., new words semantically related to studied words) was higher than false alarms of new (unrelated) words. Neurally, an expected old/new effect was observed during the time-range of the late positive component (LPC) over left centro-parietal scalp electrodes. Furthermore, true recognition also evoked larger LPC amplitudes than false recognition over both left centro-parietal and fronto-central scalp electrodes. However, we did not observe LPC-related differences between critical lures and new words, nor between correct rejections of critical lures and new words. In contrast, correct rejections of critical lures were accompanied by higher activation of a sustained positive slow wave (SPSW) in right fronto-central electrodes beyond 1200 ms. This result reveals a key role of post-retrieval processes in recognition. Results are discussed in light of theoretical approaches to false memory in the DRM paradigm.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140732071","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Guided recall of positive autobiographical memories increases anticipated pleasure and psychological resources, and reduces depressive symptoms: a replication and extension of a randomised controlled trial of brief positive cognitive-reminiscence therapy. 引导回忆积极的自传体记忆可增加预期愉悦感和心理资源,减轻抑郁症状:简短积极认知回忆疗法随机对照试验的复制和扩展。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2333510
David Hallford, Meg Woolfit, Alicia Follett, Elizabeth Jones, Ollie Harrison, David Austin
Reminiscence-based interventions focus on recalling autobiographical memories and reflective reasoning to develop a healthy and adaptive view of oneself and one's life. This study aimed to replicate the effects of a three-session, group-based, positive-memory version of cognitive-reminiscence therapy (CRT) on psychological resources and mental well-being and extend the findings to anticipated pleasure. The participants (N = 75, Mage = 43.7 (SD = 16.7), 60% females) were randomised to CRT or control group. Anticipated pleasure, psychological resources (schemas of positive self-esteem, self-efficacy, meaning in life, optimism), mental well-being (depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms) and theorised change processes (automatic negative thoughts, awareness of narrative identity) were assessed. Relative to the control group, the CRT group reported significantly higher anticipated pleasure (d = 0.76-0.93) and psychological resources of self-esteem, self-efficacy, and optimism (d's = 0.58-0.99) at post-CRT and follow-up, and lower depressive symptoms post-CRT and at follow-up (d = 0.56-0.67). Findings on meaning in life and negative automatic thinking were partially replicated. This study replicates findings of the effectiveness of this intervention for improving psychological resources such as self-worth, confidence and optimism and depressive symptoms, and indicates additional effects on anticipated pleasure. CRT may serve as a standalone intervention, or as an adjunct "memory booster" for interventions focused on future thinking and related anticipated reward.
以回忆为基础的干预措施侧重于回忆自传体记忆和反思推理,以发展一种健康的、适应性强的自我和人生观。本研究旨在重复三节课、以小组为基础、积极记忆型认知回忆疗法(CRT)对心理资源和心理健康的影响,并将研究结果扩展到预期愉悦。参与者(人数 = 75,年龄 = 43.7 (SD = 16.7),女性占 60%)被随机分配到 CRT 或对照组。对预期愉悦、心理资源(积极自尊、自我效能感、生活意义、乐观主义等图式)、心理健康(抑郁、焦虑和压力症状)以及理论上的变化过程(自动消极想法、对叙事身份的认识)进行了评估。与对照组相比,CRT 组在 CRT 后和随访时报告的预期愉悦度(d = 0.76-0.93)以及自尊、自我效能感和乐观的心理资源(d's = 0.58-0.99)明显更高,CRT 后和随访时的抑郁症状也更低(d = 0.56-0.67)。关于生命意义和消极自动思维的研究结果得到了部分复制。本研究重复了这一干预在改善自我价值、自信和乐观等心理资源以及抑郁症状方面的有效性,并显示了对预期快乐的额外影响。CRT 可以作为一种独立的干预措施,也可以作为一种辅助的 "记忆助推器",用于侧重于未来思维和相关预期奖励的干预措施。
{"title":"Guided recall of positive autobiographical memories increases anticipated pleasure and psychological resources, and reduces depressive symptoms: a replication and extension of a randomised controlled trial of brief positive cognitive-reminiscence therapy.","authors":"David Hallford, Meg Woolfit, Alicia Follett, Elizabeth Jones, Ollie Harrison, David Austin","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2333510","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2333510","url":null,"abstract":"Reminiscence-based interventions focus on recalling autobiographical memories and reflective reasoning to develop a healthy and adaptive view of oneself and one's life. This study aimed to replicate the effects of a three-session, group-based, positive-memory version of cognitive-reminiscence therapy (CRT) on psychological resources and mental well-being and extend the findings to anticipated pleasure. The participants (N = 75, Mage = 43.7 (SD = 16.7), 60% females) were randomised to CRT or control group. Anticipated pleasure, psychological resources (schemas of positive self-esteem, self-efficacy, meaning in life, optimism), mental well-being (depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms) and theorised change processes (automatic negative thoughts, awareness of narrative identity) were assessed. Relative to the control group, the CRT group reported significantly higher anticipated pleasure (d = 0.76-0.93) and psychological resources of self-esteem, self-efficacy, and optimism (d's = 0.58-0.99) at post-CRT and follow-up, and lower depressive symptoms post-CRT and at follow-up (d = 0.56-0.67). Findings on meaning in life and negative automatic thinking were partially replicated. This study replicates findings of the effectiveness of this intervention for improving psychological resources such as self-worth, confidence and optimism and depressive symptoms, and indicates additional effects on anticipated pleasure. CRT may serve as a standalone intervention, or as an adjunct \"memory booster\" for interventions focused on future thinking and related anticipated reward.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140729515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic partitioning facilitates memory for object location through category-partition cueing. 语义分区通过类别分区线索促进对物体位置的记忆
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-08 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2335111
Xinyi Lu, Mona J. H. Zhu, Evan F. Risko
In our lived environments, objects are often semantically organised (e.g., cookware and cutlery are placed close together in the kitchen). Across four experiments, we examined how semantic partitions (that group same-category objects in space) influenced memory for object locations. Participants learned the locations of items in a semantically partitioned display (where each partition contained objects from a single category) as well as a purely visually partitioned display (where each partition contained a scrambled assortment of objects from different categories). Semantic partitions significantly improved location memory accuracy compared to the scrambled display. However, when the correct partition was cued (highlighted) to participants during recall, performance on the semantically partitioned display was similar to the scrambled display. These results suggest that semantic partitions largely benefit memory for location by enhancing the ability to use the given category as a cue for a visually partitioned area (e.g., toys - top left). Our results demonstrate that semantically structured spaces help location memory across partitions, but not items within a partition, providing new insights into the interaction between meaning and memory.
在我们的生活环境中,物体通常都是按语义组织起来的(例如,厨房中的炊具和餐具就摆放在一起)。在四项实验中,我们考察了语义分区(将空间中的同类物品进行分组)如何影响对物品位置的记忆。受试者在语义分区显示(每个分区包含来自单一类别的物品)和纯视觉分区显示(每个分区包含来自不同类别的杂乱物品)中学习物品的位置。与乱码显示相比,语义分区大大提高了位置记忆的准确性。然而,当在回忆过程中向参与者提示(突出显示)正确的分区时,语义分区显示屏的表现与乱码显示屏相似。这些结果表明,语义分区在很大程度上有利于位置记忆,因为语义分区增强了使用给定类别作为视觉分区区域(如玩具--左上角)线索的能力。我们的研究结果表明,语义结构空间有助于跨分区的位置记忆,但对分区内的项目却没有帮助,这为意义与记忆之间的相互作用提供了新的见解。
{"title":"Semantic partitioning facilitates memory for object location through category-partition cueing.","authors":"Xinyi Lu, Mona J. H. Zhu, Evan F. Risko","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2335111","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2024.2335111","url":null,"abstract":"In our lived environments, objects are often semantically organised (e.g., cookware and cutlery are placed close together in the kitchen). Across four experiments, we examined how semantic partitions (that group same-category objects in space) influenced memory for object locations. Participants learned the locations of items in a semantically partitioned display (where each partition contained objects from a single category) as well as a purely visually partitioned display (where each partition contained a scrambled assortment of objects from different categories). Semantic partitions significantly improved location memory accuracy compared to the scrambled display. However, when the correct partition was cued (highlighted) to participants during recall, performance on the semantically partitioned display was similar to the scrambled display. These results suggest that semantic partitions largely benefit memory for location by enhancing the ability to use the given category as a cue for a visually partitioned area (e.g., toys - top left). Our results demonstrate that semantically structured spaces help location memory across partitions, but not items within a partition, providing new insights into the interaction between meaning and memory.","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140731109","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional and temporal order effects - a comparison between word-cued and important autobiographical memories recall orders. 情感和时间顺序效应--单词提示和重要自传体记忆回忆顺序的比较。
IF 2.1 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-04-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1080/09658211.2024.2333507
Lisa Nusser, Tabea Wolf, Daniel Zimprich

The retrieval mechanisms associated with the recall of autobiographical memories (AMs) may differ according to the cueing method used to elicit AMs. In the present study, we provide a systematic comparison between word-cued and important AMs in terms of two recall order effects, namely a temporal and an emotional order effect. The sample comprised 104 adults aged between 59 and 90 years. On two measurement occasions, participants recalled up to 20 word-cued and 15 important AMs. For each memory, participants provided their age at the time when the event occurred (age-at-event) and rated its emotionality. Order effects were analysed based on multivariate multilevel model with autoregressive effects for emotionality and age-at-event, respectively. Whereas word-cued AMs showed a stronger emotional order effect, important AMs were recalled temporarily ordered. Individuals differed in the extent to which they ordered AMs along a temporal or emotional dimension. These differences could partly be explained by personality traits. For instance, higher scores on neuroticism were associated with a stronger emotional order effect in both memory types. Findings are discussed in terms of the retrieval mechanisms that are involved in the recall of word-cued and important memory recall and how they may be affected by personality.

与自传体记忆(AMs)回忆相关的检索机制可能会因诱发自传体记忆的提示方法而有所不同。在本研究中,我们从两种回忆顺序效应(即时间顺序效应和情感顺序效应)的角度对单词提示和重要自传体记忆进行了系统比较。样本由 104 名年龄在 59 岁至 90 岁之间的成年人组成。在两次测量中,受试者回忆了多达 20 个单词提示和 15 个重要 AM。对于每个记忆,参与者都提供了事件发生时的年龄(事件发生时的年龄),并对其情绪性进行了评分。顺序效应是基于多变量多层次模型进行分析的,情感性和事件发生时的年龄分别具有自回归效应。单词提示的 AMs 表现出更强的情绪顺序效应,而重要的 AMs 则是临时有序地回忆。个体在时间或情绪维度上对 AM 排序的程度存在差异。这些差异部分可以用人格特质来解释。例如,在两种记忆类型中,神经质得分越高,情绪排序效应越强。本研究从单词提示记忆和重要记忆回忆所涉及的检索机制,以及这些机制可能如何受人格影响的角度,对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Emotional and temporal order effects - a comparison between word-cued and important autobiographical memories recall orders.","authors":"Lisa Nusser, Tabea Wolf, Daniel Zimprich","doi":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2333507","DOIUrl":"10.1080/09658211.2024.2333507","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The retrieval mechanisms associated with the recall of autobiographical memories (AMs) may differ according to the cueing method used to elicit AMs. In the present study, we provide a systematic comparison between word-cued and important AMs in terms of two recall order effects, namely a temporal and an emotional order effect. The sample comprised 104 adults aged between 59 and 90 years. On two measurement occasions, participants recalled up to 20 word-cued and 15 important AMs. For each memory, participants provided their age at the time when the event occurred (age-at-event) and rated its emotionality. Order effects were analysed based on multivariate multilevel model with autoregressive effects for emotionality and age-at-event, respectively. Whereas word-cued AMs showed a stronger emotional order effect, important AMs were recalled temporarily ordered. Individuals differed in the extent to which they ordered AMs along a temporal or emotional dimension. These differences could partly be explained by personality traits. For instance, higher scores on neuroticism were associated with a stronger emotional order effect in both memory types. Findings are discussed in terms of the retrieval mechanisms that are involved in the recall of word-cued and important memory recall and how they may be affected by personality.</p>","PeriodicalId":18569,"journal":{"name":"Memory","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140293970","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1