首页 > 最新文献

Law & Psychology eJournal最新文献

英文 中文
A Poverty of the Spirit? Law, Property and Addiction 精神贫困?法律、财产和成瘾
Pub Date : 2020-11-03 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3730916
Lucy Finchett-Maddock
What I would like to speak of in this piece is not only to discuss the way addiction is formulated by the practices and tactics of law whether through poverty law or otherwise, but to say that law itself is addition, and addiction is law. Combining desire and destruction through the work of Gilles Deleuze and Catherine Malabou, both addiction and law are explored as cumulative processes of material and immaterial yearning emanating from and within thermodynamic movements of order/disorder, destruction/creation and the tightrope of equilibrium known better as entropy, within and outside, human and other bodies. Addiction is described as rule-making, through the funneling of attention to sediment layers of law as habit, routine and custom through repetition, leading to the ultimate expression of law, that of subjectivity and the crystallization of form - the institutionalization of property and the overcoming of uncertainty through control. Addiction is argued as the very extremity, the ultimate meaning, the very motor of legal morphology itself; the striving of life against death, a speculative genesis and the baroque pathways carved in the process.
在这篇文章中,我想说的不仅是讨论成瘾是如何通过法律的实践和策略形成的,无论是通过贫困法还是其他方式,而且要说法律本身就是加法,成瘾就是法律。通过Gilles Deleuze和Catherine Malabou的作品将欲望和毁灭结合起来,成瘾和法律都是作为物质和非物质渴望的累积过程来探索的,这些渴望来自于有序/无序、破坏/创造的热力学运动,以及被称为熵的平衡绳索,在人类和其他身体的内部和外部。成瘾被描述为规则制定,通过将注意力集中到作为习惯的法律沉积物层,通过重复的惯例和习俗,导致法律的最终表达,主观性和形式的结晶-财产的制度化和通过控制克服不确定性。成瘾被认为是极端的,终极的意义,法律形态本身的动力;生命与死亡的斗争,一个推测的起源和在这个过程中雕刻的巴洛克式的道路。
{"title":"A Poverty of the Spirit? Law, Property and Addiction","authors":"Lucy Finchett-Maddock","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3730916","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3730916","url":null,"abstract":"What I would like to speak of in this piece is not only to discuss the way addiction is formulated by the practices and tactics of law whether through poverty law or otherwise, but to say that law itself is addition, and addiction is law. Combining desire and destruction through the work of Gilles Deleuze and Catherine Malabou, both addiction and law are explored as cumulative processes of material and immaterial yearning emanating from and within thermodynamic movements of order/disorder, destruction/creation and the tightrope of equilibrium known better as entropy, within and outside, human and other bodies. Addiction is described as rule-making, through the funneling of attention to sediment layers of law as habit, routine and custom through repetition, leading to the ultimate expression of law, that of subjectivity and the crystallization of form - the institutionalization of property and the overcoming of uncertainty through control. Addiction is argued as the very extremity, the ultimate meaning, the very motor of legal morphology itself; the striving of life against death, a speculative genesis and the baroque pathways carved in the process.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-11-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130370688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Capacitas Ex Machina: Are Computerised Assessmets of Mental Capacity a Benchmark or 'Red Line' for Artificial Intelligence? 机器能力:计算机化的智力评估是人工智能的基准还是“红线”?
Pub Date : 2020-04-20 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3582424
Christopher Markou, Lily Hands
This paper traces the history of computers in medicine, focusing on the rise of Expert Systems (ES) in the mid-20th century, to the rise of connectionist AI research in its latter half, and ultimately the development of Automated Mental State Detection (AMSD), fMRI scanning, and human brain interfaces. Following the critique of Joseph Weizenbaum in Computer Power and Human Reason (1976), the paper examines theoretical, practical, and ethical problems for implementing these systems in the real world, and how mental health law and psychiatry are likely to be impacted by near term technological advances which will increasingly 'objectify' the diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders. It concludes with a discussion for how computational reasoning could--and indeed should--operate in the context of mental capacity decisions in England and Wales, and suggests that the use of machines to assess mental capacity should be a "Red Line" for the ingress of AI and related technologies. It concludes with reflections on the legal implications of this claim, and identifies opportunities for further research.
本文追溯了计算机在医学领域的历史,重点关注20世纪中期专家系统(ES)的兴起,到20世纪后半叶连接主义人工智能研究的兴起,最后是自动精神状态检测(AMSD)、功能磁共振成像(fMRI)扫描和人脑接口的发展。继Joseph Weizenbaum在《计算机能力与人类理性》(1976)中的批评之后,本文探讨了在现实世界中实施这些系统的理论、实践和伦理问题,以及精神卫生法和精神病学如何可能受到近期技术进步的影响,这些技术进步将日益“客观化”精神障碍的诊断和治疗。报告最后讨论了计算推理在英格兰和威尔士的心理能力决策背景下可以(实际上应该)如何运作,并建议使用机器评估心理能力应该是人工智能和相关技术进入的“红线”。它总结了对这一主张的法律含义的反思,并确定了进一步研究的机会。
{"title":"Capacitas Ex Machina: Are Computerised Assessmets of Mental Capacity a Benchmark or 'Red Line' for Artificial Intelligence?","authors":"Christopher Markou, Lily Hands","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3582424","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3582424","url":null,"abstract":"This paper traces the history of computers in medicine, focusing on the rise of Expert Systems (ES) in the mid-20th century, to the rise of connectionist AI research in its latter half, and ultimately the development of Automated Mental State Detection (AMSD), fMRI scanning, and human brain interfaces. \u0000 \u0000Following the critique of Joseph Weizenbaum in Computer Power and Human Reason (1976), the paper examines theoretical, practical, and ethical problems for implementing these systems in the real world, and how mental health law and psychiatry are likely to be impacted by near term technological advances which will increasingly 'objectify' the diagnosis and treatment of mental disorders. \u0000 \u0000It concludes with a discussion for how computational reasoning could--and indeed should--operate in the context of mental capacity decisions in England and Wales, and suggests that the use of machines to assess mental capacity should be a \"Red Line\" for the ingress of AI and related technologies. It concludes with reflections on the legal implications of this claim, and identifies opportunities for further research.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"144 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-04-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"123296128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Conflict, Profit, Bias, Misuse of Power: Dimensions of Governance 冲突、利润、偏见、权力滥用:治理的维度
Pub Date : 2020-01-04 DOI: 10.2139/SSRN.3686644
Lionel R. Smith
Norms prohibiting conflicts of interest apply in private fiduciary relationships and also to many public office holders. Whether or not such relationships are founded on trust, such norms can cultivate trust towards those holding governance authority, whether in interpersonal, civic or political relationships. In legal and philosophical discourse, however, conflicts of interest are rarely carefully defined. It has become a commonplace that ‘not every breach of duty by a fiduciary is a breach of fiduciary duty’. We must now go on to realize that ‘not every improper action by one who must avoid conflicts of interest is a conflict of interest’. A conflict of interest arises when duty- bound judgment is exercised in the presence of a conflicting interest. There can be conflicts that are not conflicts of interest; and there can be potential conflicts as well as actual ones. The simple misuse of power is not itself conflict of interest; on the contrary, it is the problem that the rules that forbid acting in a conflict situation aim to avoid. Bias is not identical with conflict of interest, although some kinds of bias arise from conflicts. Unauthorized profits are none of the above. They represent a different dimension of the relationship: not the supervision of powers, but the simple fact that when acting for another, one may not extract benefits for oneself. The rules about conflicts can foster trust in proper decision-making, while the rule against unauthorized profits can foster trust in the proper separation between a person’s private and official actions.
禁止利益冲突的规范适用于私人信托关系,也适用于许多公职人员。无论这种关系是否建立在信任的基础上,这种规范都可以培养对拥有治理权力的人的信任,无论是在人际关系、公民关系还是政治关系中。然而,在法律和哲学话语中,利益冲突很少被仔细定义。“并非信义人每一次违反义务都是违反信义义务”这一说法已成为老生常谈。我们现在必须继续认识到,“并不是每个必须避免利益冲突的人的不当行为都是利益冲突”。利益冲突是指在存在利益冲突的情况下作出义不容辞的判断。可能存在不是利益冲突的冲突;不仅存在实际冲突,也存在潜在冲突。简单地滥用权力本身并不构成利益冲突;相反,禁止在冲突情况下采取行动的规则旨在避免的问题。偏见并不等同于利益冲突,尽管有些偏见产生于冲突。未经授权的利润不属于上述任何一种。它们代表了这种关系的另一个维度:不是对权力的监督,而是一个简单的事实,即在为他人行事时,一个人可能不会为自己谋取利益。关于冲突的规则可以促进对正确决策的信任,而反对未经授权的利润的规则可以促进对个人私人行为和官方行为之间适当分离的信任。
{"title":"Conflict, Profit, Bias, Misuse of Power: Dimensions of Governance","authors":"Lionel R. Smith","doi":"10.2139/SSRN.3686644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/SSRN.3686644","url":null,"abstract":"Norms prohibiting conflicts of interest apply in private fiduciary relationships and also to many public office holders. Whether or not such relationships are founded on trust, such norms can cultivate trust towards those holding governance authority, whether in interpersonal, civic or political relationships. In legal and philosophical discourse, however, conflicts of interest are rarely carefully defined. It has become a commonplace that ‘not every breach of duty by a fiduciary is a breach of fiduciary duty’. We must now go on to realize that ‘not every improper action by one who must avoid conflicts of interest is a conflict of interest’. A conflict of interest arises when duty- bound judgment is exercised in the presence of a conflicting interest. There can be conflicts that are not conflicts of interest; and there can be potential conflicts as well as actual ones. The simple misuse of power is not itself conflict of interest; on the contrary, it is the problem that the rules that forbid acting in a conflict situation aim to avoid. Bias is not identical with conflict of interest, although some kinds of bias arise from conflicts. Unauthorized profits are none of the above. They represent a different dimension of the relationship: not the supervision of powers, but the simple fact that when acting for another, one may not extract benefits for oneself. The rules about conflicts can foster trust in proper decision-making, while the rule against unauthorized profits can foster trust in the proper separation between a person’s private and official actions.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"129422822","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effects of the Putative Confession and Evidence Presentation on Maltreated and Non-Maltreated 9- to 12- year-olds' Coached Concealment of a Minor Transgression 推定供述和证据出示对9 ~ 12岁受虐待和未受虐待儿童轻微犯罪的教唆隐瞒的影响
Pub Date : 2019-12-02 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3425511
Angela D. Evans, T. Lyon
The present study examined the influence of the putative confession (in which children are told that the suspect told them “everything that happened” and “wants [the child] to tell the truth”) and evidence presentation on 9- to 12-year-old maltreated and non-maltreated children’s disclosure (N = 321). Half of the children played a forbidden game with an adult confederate which resulted in a laptop breaking (no transgression occurred for the other half of children), followed by coaching to conceal the forbidden game and to falsely disclose the sanctioned game. Children were then interviewed about the interaction with the confederate. Among the 9- to 10-year-olds, the putative confession led to a higher rate of breakage disclosure (62%) than the control condition (13%), and higher rates of leakage of incriminating details during recall (47% compared to 9%). Older children were more likely to disclose than younger children, and uninfluenced by the putative confession. Among all ages, evidence presentation elicited disclosures from 63% of children who had not previously disclosed, without eliciting any false disclosures.
本研究考察了假定供述(儿童被告知嫌疑人告诉他们“所发生的一切”和“希望[儿童]说出真相”)和证据出示对9- 12岁受虐待和未受虐待儿童披露的影响(N = 321)。一半的孩子和一个成年同伙一起玩了一个被禁止的游戏,结果导致了笔记本电脑的损坏(另一半孩子没有发生违规行为),然后指导他们隐瞒被禁止的游戏,并错误地透露被禁止的游戏。然后,孩子们就与同盟者的互动接受采访。在9到10岁的孩子中,假定的坦白导致了更高的破损率(62%)比对照条件(13%),并且在回忆过程中更高的犯罪细节泄露率(47%比9%)。年龄较大的孩子比年龄较小的孩子更容易坦白,而且不受假定的坦白的影响。在所有年龄段中,有63%的儿童在出示证据后披露了之前没有披露的信息,而没有引发任何虚假披露。
{"title":"The Effects of the Putative Confession and Evidence Presentation on Maltreated and Non-Maltreated 9- to 12- year-olds' Coached Concealment of a Minor Transgression","authors":"Angela D. Evans, T. Lyon","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3425511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3425511","url":null,"abstract":"The present study examined the influence of the putative confession (in which children are told that the suspect told them “everything that happened” and “wants [the child] to tell the truth”) and evidence presentation on 9- to 12-year-old maltreated and non-maltreated children’s disclosure (N = 321). Half of the children played a forbidden game with an adult confederate which resulted in a laptop breaking (no transgression occurred for the other half of children), followed by coaching to conceal the forbidden game and to falsely disclose the sanctioned game. Children were then interviewed about the interaction with the confederate. Among the 9- to 10-year-olds, the putative confession led to a higher rate of breakage disclosure (62%) than the control condition (13%), and higher rates of leakage of incriminating details during recall (47% compared to 9%). Older children were more likely to disclose than younger children, and uninfluenced by the putative confession. Among all ages, evidence presentation elicited disclosures from 63% of children who had not previously disclosed, without eliciting any false disclosures.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"58 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"114203902","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Credibility in Empirical Legal Analysis 实证法律分析中的可信度
Pub Date : 2019-08-07 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3434095
Hillel J. Bavli
Quantitative empirical research is central to both legal scholarship and litigation, but there is little confidence in it. This is because researchers can manipulate data to arrive at any result they seek to find. The root of the problem is data fishing, the practice of using data to search for and selectively report results that are statistically significant or otherwise favorable to the researcher. For reasons explained in this article, data fishing invalidates statistical results by causing false positives and false impressions. It creates an environment in which, at best, readers are highly skeptical of statistical analysis and, at worst, they base important decisions, such as policy decisions and jury verdicts, on incorrect information. The practice is nevertheless prevalent in law—often committed by well-intentioned researchers who are unaware of its harms or unaware that their analysis constitutes data fishing. This article exposes the harm that data fishing in empirical legal research causes. It then develops a framework for eliminating data fishing and restoring confidence in empirical analysis in legal scholarship and litigation. This framework, which I call DASS (an acronym for Design, Analyze, Scrutinize, and Substantiate), builds on methods in statistics and is designed for researchers to use to safeguard against data fishing and for consumers of empirical research—including scholars, courts, policymakers, and members of the public—to use to evaluate the reliability of a researcher’s statistical claims. DASS is designed to be simple and flexible, tailored to suit empirical research in law, and a substantial advancement over current anti-data-fishing practices in the social sciences, which have generally been ineffective. It can be applied broadly as a framework for credibility in empirical legal research, as well as to address a range of classical challenges in litigation, such as the hired-gun and battle-of-the-experts problems in evidence law.
定量实证研究是法律学术和诉讼的核心,但人们对它缺乏信心。这是因为研究人员可以操纵数据来得到他们想要的任何结果。问题的根源在于数据钓鱼,即使用数据搜索并有选择地报告具有统计意义或对研究人员有利的结果的做法。由于本文中解释的原因,数据钓鱼会导致误报和错误印象,从而使统计结果无效。它创造了一种环境,在这种环境中,读者最好的情况是对统计分析持高度怀疑态度,最坏的情况是,他们将政策决定和陪审团裁决等重要决策建立在不正确的信息之上。然而,这种做法在法律上很普遍——往往是出于善意的研究人员犯下的,他们没有意识到它的危害,或者没有意识到他们的分析构成了数据钓鱼。本文揭示了实证法学研究中数据捞取所带来的危害。然后,它开发了一个框架,以消除数据钓鱼和恢复对法律学术和诉讼中的实证分析的信心。这个框架,我称之为DASS(设计、分析、审查和证实的首字母缩写),建立在统计学方法的基础上,是为研究人员设计的,用于防止数据钓鱼,也为实证研究的消费者——包括学者、法院、政策制定者和公众——设计的,用于评估研究人员统计主张的可靠性。DASS的设计简单而灵活,适合法律的实证研究,是对社会科学领域目前普遍无效的反数据捕捞做法的重大进步。它可以广泛地应用于实证法律研究中的可信度框架,以及解决诉讼中的一系列经典挑战,例如证据法中的雇佣枪手和专家之战问题。
{"title":"Credibility in Empirical Legal Analysis","authors":"Hillel J. Bavli","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3434095","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3434095","url":null,"abstract":"Quantitative empirical research is central to both legal scholarship and litigation, but there is little confidence in it. This is because researchers can manipulate data to arrive at any result they seek to find. The root of the problem is data fishing, the practice of using data to search for and selectively report results that are statistically significant or otherwise favorable to the researcher. For reasons explained in this article, data fishing invalidates statistical results by causing false positives and false impressions. It creates an environment in which, at best, readers are highly skeptical of statistical analysis and, at worst, they base important decisions, such as policy decisions and jury verdicts, on incorrect information. The practice is nevertheless prevalent in law—often committed by well-intentioned researchers who are unaware of its harms or unaware that their analysis constitutes data fishing. \u0000 \u0000This article exposes the harm that data fishing in empirical legal research causes. It then develops a framework for eliminating data fishing and restoring confidence in empirical analysis in legal scholarship and litigation. This framework, which I call DASS (an acronym for Design, Analyze, Scrutinize, and Substantiate), builds on methods in statistics and is designed for researchers to use to safeguard against data fishing and for consumers of empirical research—including scholars, courts, policymakers, and members of the public—to use to evaluate the reliability of a researcher’s statistical claims. DASS is designed to be simple and flexible, tailored to suit empirical research in law, and a substantial advancement over current anti-data-fishing practices in the social sciences, which have generally been ineffective. It can be applied broadly as a framework for credibility in empirical legal research, as well as to address a range of classical challenges in litigation, such as the hired-gun and battle-of-the-experts problems in evidence law.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"140 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130459292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The Social Psychology of Financial Regulatory Governance 金融监管治理的社会心理学
Pub Date : 2019-06-11 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3402265
Giuliano G. Castellano, G. Helleringer
This paper contributes to addressing a fundamental question: how do institutions, in general, and financial regulators, in particular, “think”? To this end, the analytical tools of social psychology are applied to the regulatory framework for financial services in the European Union. The paper reveals a relationship between the constitutional status of EU regulators and the dominant group dynamics typified in the literature of social psychology. Such a relationship indicates that institutional structures might favour the emergence of specific behavioural patterns and modus operandi within regulatory bodies. Furthermore, the identification of dominant group dynamics paves the way to a more profound understanding of conflictual dynamics within groups of decision-makers. Such a novel analytical map is, then, applied to the context of the ongoing debate as to whether, following Brexit, the decision-making process of EU regulators is poised to be marked by a divide separating eurozone and non-eurozone Member States.
本文有助于解决一个基本问题:一般来说,机构,特别是金融监管机构是如何“思考”的?为此,社会心理学的分析工具被应用于欧盟金融服务的监管框架。本文揭示了欧盟监管机构的宪法地位与社会心理学文献中典型的主导群体动力学之间的关系。这种关系表明,体制结构可能有利于在管理机构内出现特定的行为模式和操作方式。此外,主导群体动力学的识别为更深刻地理解决策者群体内的冲突动力学铺平了道路。因此,这种新颖的分析地图适用于正在进行的辩论,即在英国脱欧之后,欧盟监管机构的决策过程是否将以欧元区成员国和非欧元区成员国之间的分歧为标志。
{"title":"The Social Psychology of Financial Regulatory Governance","authors":"Giuliano G. Castellano, G. Helleringer","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3402265","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3402265","url":null,"abstract":"This paper contributes to addressing a fundamental question: how do institutions, in general, and financial regulators, in particular, “think”? To this end, the analytical tools of social psychology are applied to the regulatory framework for financial services in the European Union. The paper reveals a relationship between the constitutional status of EU regulators and the dominant group dynamics typified in the literature of social psychology. Such a relationship indicates that institutional structures might favour the emergence of specific behavioural patterns and modus operandi within regulatory bodies. Furthermore, the identification of dominant group dynamics paves the way to a more profound understanding of conflictual dynamics within groups of decision-makers. Such a novel analytical map is, then, applied to the context of the ongoing debate as to whether, following Brexit, the decision-making process of EU regulators is poised to be marked by a divide separating eurozone and non-eurozone Member States.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"9 9 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-06-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"123803591","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Thin Empirics: Comment on Allen & Pardo Relative Plausibility and its Critics 薄经验:评艾伦和帕尔多的相对合理性及其批评者
Pub Date : 2019-02-28 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3289699
Dan Simon
In the target article of this symposium, Ron Allen and Michael Pardo advance the empirical claim that Relative Plausibility is the best account of juridical proof. While I tend to agree with the relative plausibility approach and endorse its holistic underpinnings, the article suffers from three weaknesses. First, the authors fail to substantiate their empirical claim. Second, the authors cite too casually to the Story Model. For all its brilliance, the story model provides too narrow a basis to serve as a general model of legal fact-finding. Finally, the authors fail to appreciate the adverse effects of holistic cognition on legal fact-finding.
在本次研讨会的目标文章中,罗恩·艾伦和迈克尔·帕尔多提出了一种经验主义主张,即相对合理性是司法证据的最佳解释。虽然我倾向于同意相对合理性的方法,并赞同其整体基础,但这篇文章有三个弱点。首先,作者未能证实他们的经验主义主张。其次,作者对故事模型的引用过于随意。尽管故事模式非常出色,但它提供的基础过于狭窄,无法作为法律事实发现的一般模式。最后,笔者没有认识到整体认知对法律事实认定的不利影响。
{"title":"Thin Empirics: Comment on Allen & Pardo Relative Plausibility and its Critics","authors":"Dan Simon","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3289699","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3289699","url":null,"abstract":"In the target article of this symposium, Ron Allen and Michael Pardo advance the empirical claim that Relative Plausibility is the best account of juridical proof. While I tend to agree with the relative plausibility approach and endorse its holistic underpinnings, the article suffers from three weaknesses. First, the authors fail to substantiate their empirical claim. Second, the authors cite too casually to the Story Model. For all its brilliance, the story model provides too narrow a basis to serve as a general model of legal fact-finding. Finally, the authors fail to appreciate the adverse effects of holistic cognition on legal fact-finding.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"9 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-02-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127828293","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do Constructive Trusts Deter Disloyalty? 建设性信任能阻止不忠吗?
Pub Date : 2018-03-10 DOI: 10.53386/nilq.v69i2.90
A. Hicks
Constructive trusts of disloyal fiduciary gain often are justified by the argument of deterrence. For there to be effective deterrence, two conditions must be satisfied: first, potentially disloyal fiduciaries must be sufficiently informed, directly or indirectly, of the properties of the constructive trust; secondly, fiduciaries must respond by accurately weighing the costs/benefits of disloyalty and other options before choosing the option that maximises their self-interest. Typically, one or both of these conditions will not be satisfied. Drawing upon insights from the behavioural sciences we find that fiduciaries contemplating disloyalty generally cannot be expected to be cognisant of the properties of the constructive trust and therefore cannot be influenced by them. Even when known, such properties will not necessarily influence fiduciary behaviour due to the way well-informed fiduciaries are likely to perceive and process the risk that their disloyalty will be detected. The deterrence gains generated by the recognition of a constructive trust are therefore likely to be negligible.
不忠实信托收益的建设性信托常常以威慑为理由。为了产生有效的威慑,必须满足两个条件:首先,潜在的不忠受托人必须直接或间接地充分了解建设性信托的性质;其次,受托人必须准确权衡不忠和其他选择的成本/收益,然后选择使自身利益最大化的选择。通常,这些条件中的一个或两个都不满足。根据行为科学的见解,我们发现考虑不忠的受托人通常不能被期望认识到建设性信任的属性,因此不会受到它们的影响。即使知道,这些属性也不一定会影响受托行为,因为消息灵通的受托人可能会感知和处理他们的不忠行为被发现的风险。因此,承认建设性信任所产生的威慑收益可能是微不足道的。
{"title":"Do Constructive Trusts Deter Disloyalty?","authors":"A. Hicks","doi":"10.53386/nilq.v69i2.90","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.53386/nilq.v69i2.90","url":null,"abstract":"Constructive trusts of disloyal fiduciary gain often are justified by the argument of deterrence. For there to be effective deterrence, two conditions must be satisfied: first, potentially disloyal fiduciaries must be sufficiently informed, directly or indirectly, of the properties of the constructive trust; secondly, fiduciaries must respond by accurately weighing the costs/benefits of disloyalty and other options before choosing the option that maximises their self-interest. Typically, one or both of these conditions will not be satisfied. Drawing upon insights from the behavioural sciences we find that fiduciaries contemplating disloyalty generally cannot be expected to be cognisant of the properties of the constructive trust and therefore cannot be influenced by them. Even when known, such properties will not necessarily influence fiduciary behaviour due to the way well-informed fiduciaries are likely to perceive and process the risk that their disloyalty will be detected. The deterrence gains generated by the recognition of a constructive trust are therefore likely to be negligible.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-03-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"130521625","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Politics of Legal Empirics: Do Political Attitudes Predict the Results of Empirical Legal Scholarship? 法律经验的政治学:政治态度能否预测实证法学研究的结果?
Pub Date : 2018-02-26 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3130086
J. Rachlinski
Empirical legal scholarship has emerged as a dominant trend in legal scholarship. At its best, empirical scholarship subjects assertions about the effect of legal rules to a neutral test. But is empirical inquiry truly neutral? The validity of an empirical study should rest on the reliability of the methods used, rather than the political implications of its conclusions. Scholars might choose targets of inquiry, sources of data, or methods of analysis that support their political allegiances. This paper tests this thesis by matching the political beliefs of authors of empirical legal scholarship with the results of their research. The political allegiances of authors mildly correlate with the results of empirical inquiry in legal scholarship.
实证法学研究已成为法学研究的主导趋势。在最好的情况下,经验主义学术对法律规则效果的断言进行了中立的检验。但是,实证调查真的是中立的吗?一项实证研究的有效性应取决于所使用方法的可靠性,而不是其结论的政治含义。学者可能会选择支持其政治忠诚的调查目标、数据来源或分析方法。本文通过将实证法学学者的政治信仰与他们的研究结果相匹配来检验这一论点。作者的政治忠诚与法律学术实证调查的结果略有关联。
{"title":"The Politics of Legal Empirics: Do Political Attitudes Predict the Results of Empirical Legal Scholarship?","authors":"J. Rachlinski","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3130086","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3130086","url":null,"abstract":"Empirical legal scholarship has emerged as a dominant trend in legal scholarship. At its best, empirical scholarship subjects assertions about the effect of legal rules to a neutral test. But is empirical inquiry truly neutral? The validity of an empirical study should rest on the reliability of the methods used, rather than the political implications of its conclusions. Scholars might choose targets of inquiry, sources of data, or methods of analysis that support their political allegiances. This paper tests this thesis by matching the political beliefs of authors of empirical legal scholarship with the results of their research. The political allegiances of authors mildly correlate with the results of empirical inquiry in legal scholarship.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"1 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"115401550","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strategic Complexity and the Value of Thinking 战略复杂性与思维价值
Pub Date : 2017-09-22 DOI: 10.2139/ssrn.3041519
David Gill, Victoria Prowse
We leverage response-time data from repeated strategic interactions to measure the strategic complexity of a situation by how long people think on average when they face that situation (where we categorize situations according to characteristics of play in the previous round). We find that strategic complexity varies significantly across situations, and we find considerable heterogeneity in how responsive subjects’ thinking times are to complexity. We also study how variation in response times at the individual level affects success: when a subject thinks for longer than she would normally do in a particular situation, she wins less frequently and earns less.
我们利用来自重复战略互动的响应时间数据,通过人们在面对情境时的平均思考时间来衡量情境的战略复杂性(我们根据前一轮游戏的特征对情境进行分类)。我们发现战略复杂性在不同情况下差异很大,我们发现受试者对复杂性的反应时间存在相当大的异质性。我们还研究了个体层面上反应时间的变化是如何影响成功的:当一个受试者在特定情况下比正常情况下思考的时间更长时,她获胜的频率就会降低,赚的钱也会减少。
{"title":"Strategic Complexity and the Value of Thinking","authors":"David Gill, Victoria Prowse","doi":"10.2139/ssrn.3041519","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.3041519","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 We leverage response-time data from repeated strategic interactions to measure the strategic complexity of a situation by how long people think on average when they face that situation (where we categorize situations according to characteristics of play in the previous round). We find that strategic complexity varies significantly across situations, and we find considerable heterogeneity in how responsive subjects’ thinking times are to complexity. We also study how variation in response times at the individual level affects success: when a subject thinks for longer than she would normally do in a particular situation, she wins less frequently and earns less.","PeriodicalId":191231,"journal":{"name":"Law & Psychology eJournal","volume":"18 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"127714755","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
期刊
Law & Psychology eJournal
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1