首页 > 最新文献

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review最新文献

英文 中文
Setting the scene for boundary extension: Methods, findings, connections, and theories. 为边界扩展做好准备:方法、发现、联系和理论。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02545-w
Timothy L Hubbard

A previously viewed scene is often remembered as containing a larger extent of the background than was actually present, and information that was likely present just outside the boundaries of that view is often incorporated into the representation of that scene. This has been referred to as boundary extension. Methodologies used in studies on boundary extension (terminology, stimulus presentation, response measures) are described. Empirical findings regarding effects of characteristics of the stimulus (whether the stimulus depicts a scene, semantics of the scene, view angle, object size, object cropping, object orientation, object color, number of objects, depth of field, object distance, viewpoint production, scene orientation, motion, faces, emotions, modality, whether the scene is multimodal), characteristics of the display (aperture shape, aperture size, target duration, retention interval), and characteristics of the observer (allocation of attention, imagination, age, expectations and strategies, eye fixation, eye movements, monocular or binocular view, vantage point, confinement, prior exposure, expertise, arousal, pathology) on boundary extension are reviewed. Connections of boundary extension to other cognitive phenomena and processes (evolutionary adaptation, Gestalt principles, illusions, psychophysics, invariant physical principles, aesthetics, temporal boundary extension, normalization) are noted, and theories and theoretical considerations regarding boundary extension (multisource model, boundary transformation, mental imagery, 4E cognition, cognitive modularity, neurological mechanisms of scene representation) are discussed.

在人们的记忆中,以前观看过的场景所包含的背景范围往往比实际存在的要大,而在该景 观边界之外可能存在的信息往往会被纳入到该场景的表征中。这种现象被称为边界扩展。本文介绍了边界扩展研究中使用的方法(术语、刺激呈现、反应测量)。有关刺激物特征(刺激物是否描绘了一个场景、场景的语义、视角、物体大小、物体裁剪、物体方位、物体颜色、物体数量、景深、物体距离、视点产生、场景方位、运动、面孔、情绪、模式、场景是否多模态)影响的实证研究结果、本研究回顾了显示的特征(光圈形状、光圈大小、目标持续时间、保持间隔)和观察者的特征(注意力分配、想象力、年龄、期望和策略、眼球固定、眼球运动、单眼或双眼视角、有利位置、限制、先前接触、专业知识、唤醒、病理)对边界扩展的影响。还指出了边界扩展与其他认知现象和过程的联系(进化适应、格式塔原理、幻觉、心理物理学、不变物理原理、美学、时间边界扩展、正常化),并讨论了有关边界扩展的理论和理论考虑(多源模型、边界转换、心理想象、4E 认知、认知模块化、场景表征的神经机制)。
{"title":"Setting the scene for boundary extension: Methods, findings, connections, and theories.","authors":"Timothy L Hubbard","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02545-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02545-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A previously viewed scene is often remembered as containing a larger extent of the background than was actually present, and information that was likely present just outside the boundaries of that view is often incorporated into the representation of that scene. This has been referred to as boundary extension. Methodologies used in studies on boundary extension (terminology, stimulus presentation, response measures) are described. Empirical findings regarding effects of characteristics of the stimulus (whether the stimulus depicts a scene, semantics of the scene, view angle, object size, object cropping, object orientation, object color, number of objects, depth of field, object distance, viewpoint production, scene orientation, motion, faces, emotions, modality, whether the scene is multimodal), characteristics of the display (aperture shape, aperture size, target duration, retention interval), and characteristics of the observer (allocation of attention, imagination, age, expectations and strategies, eye fixation, eye movements, monocular or binocular view, vantage point, confinement, prior exposure, expertise, arousal, pathology) on boundary extension are reviewed. Connections of boundary extension to other cognitive phenomena and processes (evolutionary adaptation, Gestalt principles, illusions, psychophysics, invariant physical principles, aesthetics, temporal boundary extension, normalization) are noted, and theories and theoretical considerations regarding boundary extension (multisource model, boundary transformation, mental imagery, 4E cognition, cognitive modularity, neurological mechanisms of scene representation) are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"97-138"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894137","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Group efficiency based on the termination rule in the multiple-targets visual search task. 多目标视觉搜索任务中基于终止规则的分组效率
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-15 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02558-5
Hanshu Zhang, Peng-Fei Zhu, Cheng-Ta Yang

In practical visual search fields, observers often encounter errors that result from an unknown number of targets, which may induce reduced accuracy and speed. Our current study addresses the potential enhancement of collaborative search efficiency as a dyad to mitigate such incurred search costs. Utilizing the capacity coefficient, we evaluated search efficiency and explored the interplay of task difficulty and termination rule in collaborative visual search. Our prediction that collaborative benefits increased with elevated task difficulty was not supported in Experiment 1, where participants were tasked with judging the presence of any target. In contrast, Experiment 2 demonstrated that dyads exhibited greater search efficiency during exhaustive searches for multiple targets with elevated task difficulty. Notably, our findings indicated an advantage in dyad searches compared to baseline predictions from individual searches. Our results underscored the significance of task difficulty and termination rules in leveraging human resources for improved collaborative visual search performance.

在实际的视觉搜索领域中,观察者经常会遇到目标数量未知而导致的错误,这可能会降低搜索的准确性和速度。我们目前的研究探讨了如何提高二人协作搜索的效率,以减轻这种搜索成本。我们利用容量系数评估了搜索效率,并探讨了协作视觉搜索中任务难度和终止规则的相互作用。在实验 1 中,我们预测协作效益会随着任务难度的增加而增加,但这一预测在实验 1 中并没有得到支持,在实验 1 中,被试的任务是判断是否存在任何目标。相反,实验 2 表明,在任务难度增加的情况下,二人组在对多个目标进行穷举搜索时表现出更高的搜索效率。值得注意的是,我们的研究结果表明,与个体搜索的基线预测相比,双人搜索具有优势。我们的结果凸显了任务难度和终止规则在利用人力资源提高协作视觉搜索性能方面的重要性。
{"title":"Group efficiency based on the termination rule in the multiple-targets visual search task.","authors":"Hanshu Zhang, Peng-Fei Zhu, Cheng-Ta Yang","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02558-5","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02558-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In practical visual search fields, observers often encounter errors that result from an unknown number of targets, which may induce reduced accuracy and speed. Our current study addresses the potential enhancement of collaborative search efficiency as a dyad to mitigate such incurred search costs. Utilizing the capacity coefficient, we evaluated search efficiency and explored the interplay of task difficulty and termination rule in collaborative visual search. Our prediction that collaborative benefits increased with elevated task difficulty was not supported in Experiment 1, where participants were tasked with judging the presence of any target. In contrast, Experiment 2 demonstrated that dyads exhibited greater search efficiency during exhaustive searches for multiple targets with elevated task difficulty. Notably, our findings indicated an advantage in dyad searches compared to baseline predictions from individual searches. Our results underscored the significance of task difficulty and termination rules in leveraging human resources for improved collaborative visual search performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"463-471"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141988733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unraveling the mechanism of semantic object-based attention: The role of top-down search strategies. 揭示基于语义对象的注意机制:自上而下搜索策略的作用
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02542-z
Zhuyang Li, Jie Chen, Qiaoming Zhang, Lu Liu

There is an ongoing debate between the sensory enhancement account and the attentional prioritization account concerning the mechanism underlying object-based attention. This debate remains unresolved because of the predominant use of geometric objects in previous studies, which made it difficult to experimentally dissociate the two accounts due to the prominent boundaries of these objects. The current study investigated the mechanism underlying semantic object-based attention by utilizing Chinese two-character words with different word frequencies and the event-related potentials (ERPs) technique to elucidate the ongoing debate. The behavioral results showed that the semantic object effect was observed only in the high-frequency condition. The ERP results revealed the following: (1) the N1 component was larger for the high-frequency condition compared with the low-frequency condition. However, there was no significant difference in amplitude between the N1 component elicited by invalid same object locations and invalid different object locations, irrespective of whether it occurred in high- or low-frequency conditions. (2) The P3 component elicited by invalid same object locations was larger than that elicited by invalid different object locations only in the high-frequency condition. (3) The N400 amplitude elicited by invalid same object locations was smaller than that elicited by invalid different object locations only in the high-frequency condition. These results suggest that in the absence of obvious object boundaries, the production of object-based attention is primarily driven by search strategies. Attentional prioritization, rather than sensory enhancement, is the dominant mechanism underlying object-based attention.

关于基于物体的注意力的基本机制,感觉增强说和注意力优先说之间一直存在争论。这一争论至今仍未解决,原因是以往的研究主要使用几何物体,由于这些物体的边界突出,很难在实验中将这两种说法区分开来。本研究利用不同词频的中文双字词和事件相关电位(ERPs)技术,研究了基于语义对象的注意机制,以阐明正在进行的争论。行为结果显示,语义对象效应仅在高频条件下出现。ERP结果显示了以下几点:(1) 与低频条件相比,高频条件下的 N1 分量更大。然而,无论在高频还是低频条件下,相同物体位置无效和不同物体位置无效所引起的 N1 分量在振幅上没有明显差异。(2)只有在高频条件下,无效相同物体位置引起的 P3 分量才大于无效不同物体位置引起的 P3 分量。(3)只有在高频条件下,无效相同物体位置引起的 N400 振幅才小于无效不同物体位置引起的 N400 振幅。这些结果表明,在没有明显物体边界的情况下,产生基于物体的注意主要是由搜索策略驱动的。注意力优先化,而不是感觉增强,是基于物体的注意力的主导机制。
{"title":"Unraveling the mechanism of semantic object-based attention: The role of top-down search strategies.","authors":"Zhuyang Li, Jie Chen, Qiaoming Zhang, Lu Liu","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02542-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02542-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is an ongoing debate between the sensory enhancement account and the attentional prioritization account concerning the mechanism underlying object-based attention. This debate remains unresolved because of the predominant use of geometric objects in previous studies, which made it difficult to experimentally dissociate the two accounts due to the prominent boundaries of these objects. The current study investigated the mechanism underlying semantic object-based attention by utilizing Chinese two-character words with different word frequencies and the event-related potentials (ERPs) technique to elucidate the ongoing debate. The behavioral results showed that the semantic object effect was observed only in the high-frequency condition. The ERP results revealed the following: (1) the N1 component was larger for the high-frequency condition compared with the low-frequency condition. However, there was no significant difference in amplitude between the N1 component elicited by invalid same object locations and invalid different object locations, irrespective of whether it occurred in high- or low-frequency conditions. (2) The P3 component elicited by invalid same object locations was larger than that elicited by invalid different object locations only in the high-frequency condition. (3) The N400 amplitude elicited by invalid same object locations was smaller than that elicited by invalid different object locations only in the high-frequency condition. These results suggest that in the absence of obvious object boundaries, the production of object-based attention is primarily driven by search strategies. Attentional prioritization, rather than sensory enhancement, is the dominant mechanism underlying object-based attention.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"352-362"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724357","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship of speech perception and speech production: It's complicated. 语音感知与语音生成之间的关系:这很复杂
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-09-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02561-w
Melissa M Baese-Berk, Efthymia C Kapnoula, Arthur G Samuel

A widely held belief is that speech perception and speech production are tightly linked, with each modality available to help with learning in the other modality. This positive relationship is often summarized as perception and production being "two sides of the same coin." There are, indeed, many situations that have shown this mutually supportive relationship. However, there is a growing body of research showing very different results, with the modalities operating independently, or even in opposition to each other. We review the now-sizeable literature demonstrating the negative effect that speech production can have on perceptual learning of speech, at multiple levels (particularly at the lexical and sublexical levels). By comparing the situations that show this pattern with ones in which more positive interactions occur, we provide an initial account of why the different outcomes are found, identifying factors that lead to either positive or negative effects of production on perception. The review clarifies the complex relationship that exists between the two modalities: They are indeed linked, but their relationship is more complicated than is suggested by the notion that they are two sides of the same coin.

人们普遍认为,语音感知和语音生成是紧密联系在一起的,每一种模式都可以帮助另一种模式的学习。这种积极的关系通常被概括为感知和语音生成是 "一枚硬币的两面"。的确,有许多情况表明了这种相互支持的关系。然而,也有越来越多的研究显示了截然不同的结果,这两种模式各自独立运行,甚至相互对立。我们回顾了现已有相当规模的文献,这些文献表明,在多个层面(尤其是词法和次词法层面)上,语音生成可能对语音的感知学习产生负面影响。通过比较显示出这种模式的情况和发生更多积极互动的情况,我们初步解释了为什么会出现不同的结果,找出了导致语音制作对感知产生积极或消极影响的因素。这篇综述阐明了两种模式之间存在的复杂关系:它们之间确实存在联系,但其关系比 "它们是一枚硬币的两面 "这一概念所暗示的更为复杂。
{"title":"The relationship of speech perception and speech production: It's complicated.","authors":"Melissa M Baese-Berk, Efthymia C Kapnoula, Arthur G Samuel","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02561-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02561-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A widely held belief is that speech perception and speech production are tightly linked, with each modality available to help with learning in the other modality. This positive relationship is often summarized as perception and production being \"two sides of the same coin.\" There are, indeed, many situations that have shown this mutually supportive relationship. However, there is a growing body of research showing very different results, with the modalities operating independently, or even in opposition to each other. We review the now-sizeable literature demonstrating the negative effect that speech production can have on perceptual learning of speech, at multiple levels (particularly at the lexical and sublexical levels). By comparing the situations that show this pattern with ones in which more positive interactions occur, we provide an initial account of why the different outcomes are found, identifying factors that lead to either positive or negative effects of production on perception. The review clarifies the complex relationship that exists between the two modalities: They are indeed linked, but their relationship is more complicated than is suggested by the notion that they are two sides of the same coin.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"226-242"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142126517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The time course of Temporal Binding in social and nonsocial interactions. 社交和非社交互动中时间绑定的时间过程。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02540-1
Crystal A Silver, Benjamin W Tatler, Ramakrishna Chakravarthi, Bert Timmermans

Temporal Binding (TB) is the subjective compression of action-effect intervals. While the effects of nonsocial actions are highly predictable, it is not the case when interacting with conspecifics, who often act under their own volition, at a time of their choosing. Given the relative differences in action-effect predictability in non-social and social interactions, it is plausible that TB and its properties differ across these situations. To examine this, in two experiments, we compared the time course of TB in social and nonsocial interactions, systematically varying action-effect intervals (200-2,100 ms). Participants were told they were (a) interacting with another person via a live webcam, who was in fact a confederate (social condition), (b) interacting with pre-recorded videos (nonsocial condition), or (c) observing two pre-recorded videos (control condition; Experiment 2). Results across experiments showed greater TB for social compared to nonsocial conditions, and the difference was proportional to the action-effect intervals. Further, in Experiment 1, TB was consistently observed throughout the experiment for social interactions, whereas nonsocial TB decreased from the first to the second half of the experiment. In Experiment 2, the nonsocial condition did not differ from control, whereas the social condition did, exhibiting enhanced binding. We argue these results suggest that the sociality of an interaction modulates the 'internal clock' of time perception.

时间约束(TB)是对行动效果间隔的主观压缩。虽然非社会性行动的效果具有很高的可预测性,但在与同类互动时却并非如此,因为同类通常会根据自己的意愿在自己选择的时间内采取行动。鉴于非社会性和社会性互动中行动效果可预测性的相对差异,TB 及其特性在这些情况下有所不同是有道理的。为了研究这一点,我们在两个实验中比较了社会性和非社会性互动中 TB 的时间过程,并系统地改变了动作效果的时间间隔(200-2100 毫秒)。参与者被告知他们正在(a)通过实时网络摄像头与另一个人互动,而这个人实际上是一个密友(社交条件);(b)与预先录制的视频互动(非社交条件);或(c)观察两个预先录制的视频(对照条件;实验 2)。各次实验的结果显示,社交条件下的 TB 比非社交条件下的 TB 大,而且这种差异与动作效应时间间隔成正比。此外,在实验 1 中,社交互动的 TB 在整个实验过程中持续存在,而非社交互动的 TB 则从实验的前半部分到后半部分逐渐减少。在实验 2 中,非社交条件与对照组没有差异,而社交条件则有差异,表现出更强的约束力。我们认为这些结果表明,互动的社会性会调节时间感知的 "内部时钟"。
{"title":"The time course of Temporal Binding in social and nonsocial interactions.","authors":"Crystal A Silver, Benjamin W Tatler, Ramakrishna Chakravarthi, Bert Timmermans","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02540-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02540-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Temporal Binding (TB) is the subjective compression of action-effect intervals. While the effects of nonsocial actions are highly predictable, it is not the case when interacting with conspecifics, who often act under their own volition, at a time of their choosing. Given the relative differences in action-effect predictability in non-social and social interactions, it is plausible that TB and its properties differ across these situations. To examine this, in two experiments, we compared the time course of TB in social and nonsocial interactions, systematically varying action-effect intervals (200-2,100 ms). Participants were told they were (a) interacting with another person via a live webcam, who was in fact a confederate (social condition), (b) interacting with pre-recorded videos (nonsocial condition), or (c) observing two pre-recorded videos (control condition; Experiment 2). Results across experiments showed greater TB for social compared to nonsocial conditions, and the difference was proportional to the action-effect intervals. Further, in Experiment 1, TB was consistently observed throughout the experiment for social interactions, whereas nonsocial TB decreased from the first to the second half of the experiment. In Experiment 2, the nonsocial condition did not differ from control, whereas the social condition did, exhibiting enhanced binding. We argue these results suggest that the sociality of an interaction modulates the 'internal clock' of time perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"326-341"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11836101/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634302","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Untangling the threads of motivated memory: Independent influences of reward and emotion. 解开动机记忆的线索:奖赏和情感的独立影响
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-31 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02546-9
Holly J Bowen, Christopher R Madan

Motivational and emotional influences on memory have been studied extensively; however, despite the link between these constructs, they have been studied in separate lines of research, with very little work examining their effects concurrently. The current study takes a novel approach to manipulate motivational and emotional influences orthogonally, and within the same task, to test their interplay on intentional memory formation. If emotion and reward motivation are tightly linked, they may rely on overlapping cognitive mechanisms, thus we would not expect emotion and reward to interact in memory. Alternatively, they could be distinct constructs and therefore would boost memory when both are included in the same experimental trial, above and beyond additive effects. To test these competing predictions, in Experiment 1, participants (n = 180) completed an old/new recognition memory task with emotional (negative, positive) and neutral words intentionally encoded with high or low reward anticipation cues. In Experiment 2, participants (n = 159) encoded emotional and neutral words with a high or low reward cue, but memory was tested with free recall using study-test blocks. The findings from both experiments converged. There were main effects of emotion and reward in generally hypothesized directions, but no evidence of an interaction between these factors. This is in line with the prediction that emotion and reward motivation are similar constructs. Their combination within a trial does not boost memory above and beyond either of these factors alone perhaps indicating these constructs have similar cognitive mechanisms.

动机和情绪对记忆的影响已经得到了广泛的研究;然而,尽管这两个因素之间存在联系,但它们一直是分开研究的,很少有研究同时考察它们的影响。目前的研究采用了一种新颖的方法,在同一任务中正交操纵动机和情绪影响,以检验它们对有意记忆形成的相互作用。如果情感和奖赏动机紧密相连,它们可能依赖于重叠的认知机制,因此我们不会期望情感和奖赏在记忆中产生相互作用。另一种情况是,它们可能是不同的建构,因此当两者都包含在同一实验试验中时,会促进记忆,而不会产生叠加效应。为了验证这些相互竞争的预测,在实验 1 中,参与者(n = 180)完成了一项新旧识别记忆任务,其中包括有意编码的带有高或低奖励预期线索的情感词(消极词、积极词)和中性词。在实验 2 中,参与者(n = 159)用高或低奖励线索对情绪词和中性词进行编码,但使用学习测试块对记忆进行自由回忆测试。两个实验的结果趋于一致。情绪和奖励在一般假设的方向上存在主效应,但没有证据表明这些因素之间存在交互作用。这与情感和奖赏动机是相似结构的预测是一致的。在一次试验中,这两个因素结合在一起对记忆的促进作用不会超过单独使用其中一个因素的效果,这或许表明这两个因素具有相似的认知机制。
{"title":"Untangling the threads of motivated memory: Independent influences of reward and emotion.","authors":"Holly J Bowen, Christopher R Madan","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02546-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02546-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Motivational and emotional influences on memory have been studied extensively; however, despite the link between these constructs, they have been studied in separate lines of research, with very little work examining their effects concurrently. The current study takes a novel approach to manipulate motivational and emotional influences orthogonally, and within the same task, to test their interplay on intentional memory formation. If emotion and reward motivation are tightly linked, they may rely on overlapping cognitive mechanisms, thus we would not expect emotion and reward to interact in memory. Alternatively, they could be distinct constructs and therefore would boost memory when both are included in the same experimental trial, above and beyond additive effects. To test these competing predictions, in Experiment 1, participants (n = 180) completed an old/new recognition memory task with emotional (negative, positive) and neutral words intentionally encoded with high or low reward anticipation cues. In Experiment 2, participants (n = 159) encoded emotional and neutral words with a high or low reward cue, but memory was tested with free recall using study-test blocks. The findings from both experiments converged. There were main effects of emotion and reward in generally hypothesized directions, but no evidence of an interaction between these factors. This is in line with the prediction that emotion and reward motivation are similar constructs. Their combination within a trial does not boost memory above and beyond either of these factors alone perhaps indicating these constructs have similar cognitive mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"396-406"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11836132/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141860722","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Removing information from working memory with a delay: Effective but not beneficial. 延迟从工作记忆中删除信息:有效但无益
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02550-z
Hannah Dames, Chenyu Li, Gidon T Frischkorn, Klaus Oberauer

Ideally, removing outdated information from working memory (WM) should have two consequences: The removed content should be less accessible (removal costs), and other WM content should benefit from the freeing up of WM capacity (removal benefits). Robust removal benefits and removal costs have been demonstrated when people are told to forget items shortly after they were encoded (immediate removal). However, other studies suggest that people might be unable to selectively remove items from an already encoded set of items (delayed removal). In two experiments (n = 219; n = 241), we investigated the effectiveness and consequences of delayed removal by combining a modified version of Ecker's et al. (Journal of Memory and Language, 74, 77-90, 2014) letter updating task with a directed-forgetting in WM paradigm. We found that while delayed removal resulted in reduced memory for the to-be-forgotten item-location relations (removal costs), it failed to enhance performance for existing WM content. This contrasts sharply with immediate removal, where removal benefits can be observed. A fine-grained analysis of removal benefits shows that removal from WM proactively facilitates the subsequent encoding of new information but does not retroactively aid stored WM content.

理想情况下,从工作记忆(WM)中移除过时信息会产生两种结果:被移除的内容应该更难获取(移除成本),而其他工作记忆内容应该从释放的工作记忆容量中获益(移除收益)。当人们被告知遗忘编码后不久的项目(立即删除)时,已经证明了强大的删除收益和删除成本。然而,其他研究表明,人们可能无法从已经编码的项目集中选择性地删除项目(延迟删除)。在两项实验(n = 219;n = 241)中,我们将 Ecker 等人(《记忆与语言杂志》,74, 77-90, 2014 年)的字母更新任务与定向遗忘 WM 范式相结合,研究了延迟移除的效果和后果。我们发现,虽然延迟删除会导致对即将遗忘的项目-位置关系的记忆减少(删除成本),但却无法提高对现有 WM 内容的记忆效果。这与立即移除形成了鲜明对比,后者可以观察到移除的益处。对移除益处的精细分析表明,从 WM 中移除会主动促进新信息的后续编码,但不会追溯性地帮助存储的 WM 内容。
{"title":"Removing information from working memory with a delay: Effective but not beneficial.","authors":"Hannah Dames, Chenyu Li, Gidon T Frischkorn, Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02550-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02550-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Ideally, removing outdated information from working memory (WM) should have two consequences: The removed content should be less accessible (removal costs), and other WM content should benefit from the freeing up of WM capacity (removal benefits). Robust removal benefits and removal costs have been demonstrated when people are told to forget items shortly after they were encoded (immediate removal). However, other studies suggest that people might be unable to selectively remove items from an already encoded set of items (delayed removal). In two experiments (n = 219; n = 241), we investigated the effectiveness and consequences of delayed removal by combining a modified version of Ecker's et al. (Journal of Memory and Language, 74, 77-90, 2014) letter updating task with a directed-forgetting in WM paradigm. We found that while delayed removal resulted in reduced memory for the to-be-forgotten item-location relations (removal costs), it failed to enhance performance for existing WM content. This contrasts sharply with immediate removal, where removal benefits can be observed. A fine-grained analysis of removal benefits shows that removal from WM proactively facilitates the subsequent encoding of new information but does not retroactively aid stored WM content.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"430-441"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11836233/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894136","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to: A novel image database for social concepts reveals preference biases in autistic spectrum in adults and children.
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02629-7
David Soto, Amaia Salazar, Patxi Elosegi, Antje Walter, Ning Mei, Ekaine Rodriguez, Valentina Petrollini, Agustín Vicente
{"title":"Correction to: A novel image database for social concepts reveals preference biases in autistic spectrum in adults and children.","authors":"David Soto, Amaia Salazar, Patxi Elosegi, Antje Walter, Ning Mei, Ekaine Rodriguez, Valentina Petrollini, Agustín Vicente","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02629-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02629-7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":"32 1","pages":"492"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11836083/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143450202","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cross-modal interactions in language production: evidence from word learning. 语言生成中的跨模态互动:单词学习的证据
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02552-x
Svetlana Pinet, Clara D Martin

Literate adults are able to produce the same word in different language modalities-for instance, through speaking and writing. Yet how speaking and writing interact is not well understood. The present study takes a new perspective on the question of the co-activation of phonological and orthographic representations in speaking and writing by examining the acquisition of novel words. We tested how novel words get integrated into modality-specific lexicons by biasing novel word acquisition toward speaking or writing and assessing cross-modal transfer at the first stages of learning. Participants learned novel words paired with pictures of novel objects and practiced them overtly through speaking or typing. At test, typed training led to higher recall accuracy than spoken training whether words were recalled through typing or speaking. Performance in typing (RT and durations) benefited more from typed than spoken training. Crucially, performance in speaking did not benefit specifically from spoken training and was similar after spoken or typed training. Results are compatible with an asymmetric integration in the phonological and orthographic lexicons according to the modality of training, with representations created in the orthographic lexicon directly transferring to the phonological lexicon, while the opposite doesn't seem to occur. Cross-modal transfer dynamics are discussed according to the level of lexical activation.

识字的成年人能够通过不同的语言模式--例如,通过说和写--造出同一个词。然而,人们对说和写如何相互作用还不甚了解。本研究通过考察新词的习得,从一个新的角度探讨了语音表征和正字法表征在说和写中的共同作用问题。我们通过使新词学习偏向于说或写,并在学习的最初阶段评估跨模态迁移,来测试新词是如何整合到特定模态词典中的。参与者学习与新奇物体图片配对的新词,并通过说话或打字进行公开练习。在测试中,无论是通过打字还是口语回忆单词,打字训练都比口语训练的回忆准确率高。打字训练比口语训练更有利于提高打字成绩(RT 和持续时间)。最重要的是,口语成绩并没有特别从口语训练中获益,口语或打字训练后的成绩相差无几。研究结果表明,根据训练模式,语音词典和正字法词典的整合是不对称的,正字法词典中创建的表征会直接转移到语音词典中,而相反的情况似乎不会发生。跨模态迁移的动态变化将根据词汇激活的程度进行讨论。
{"title":"Cross-modal interactions in language production: evidence from word learning.","authors":"Svetlana Pinet, Clara D Martin","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02552-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02552-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Literate adults are able to produce the same word in different language modalities-for instance, through speaking and writing. Yet how speaking and writing interact is not well understood. The present study takes a new perspective on the question of the co-activation of phonological and orthographic representations in speaking and writing by examining the acquisition of novel words. We tested how novel words get integrated into modality-specific lexicons by biasing novel word acquisition toward speaking or writing and assessing cross-modal transfer at the first stages of learning. Participants learned novel words paired with pictures of novel objects and practiced them overtly through speaking or typing. At test, typed training led to higher recall accuracy than spoken training whether words were recalled through typing or speaking. Performance in typing (RT and durations) benefited more from typed than spoken training. Crucially, performance in speaking did not benefit specifically from spoken training and was similar after spoken or typed training. Results are compatible with an asymmetric integration in the phonological and orthographic lexicons according to the modality of training, with representations created in the orthographic lexicon directly transferring to the phonological lexicon, while the opposite doesn't seem to occur. Cross-modal transfer dynamics are discussed according to the level of lexical activation.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"452-462"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141907578","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The neural network of sensory attenuation: A neuroimaging meta-analysis. 感觉衰减的神经网络:神经成像荟萃分析。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-02-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02532-1
Jingjin Gu, Tatia Buidze, Ke Zhao, Jan Gläscher, Xiaolan Fu

Sensory attenuation refers to the reduction in sensory intensity resulting from self-initiated actions compared to stimuli initiated externally. A classic example is scratching oneself without feeling itchy. This phenomenon extends across various sensory modalities, including visual, auditory, somatosensory, and nociceptive stimuli. The internal forward model proposes that during voluntary actions, an efferent copy of the action command is sent out to predict sensory feedback. This predicted sensory feedback is then compared with the actual sensory feedback, leading to the suppression or reduction of sensory stimuli originating from self-initiated actions. To further elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying sensory attenuation effect, we conducted an extensive meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) studies. Utilizing activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analysis, our results revealed significant activations in a prominent cluster encompassing the right superior temporal gyrus (rSTG), right middle temporal gyrus (rMTG), and right insula when comparing external-generated with self-generated conditions. Additionally, significant activation was observed in the right anterior cerebellum when comparing self-generated to external-generated conditions. Further analysis using meta-analytic connectivity modeling (MACM) unveiled distinct brain networks co-activated with the rMTG and right cerebellum, respectively. Based on these findings, we propose that sensory attenuation arises from the suppression of reflexive inputs elicited by self-initiated actions through the internal forward modeling of a cerebellum-centered action prediction network, enabling the "sensory conflict detection" regions to effectively discriminate between inputs resulting from self-induced actions and those originating externally.

感觉衰减指的是与外部刺激相比,由自身引发的行为导致的感觉强度降低。一个典型的例子就是自己挠痒却不觉得痒。这种现象延伸到各种感觉模式,包括视觉、听觉、躯体感觉和痛觉刺激。内部前向模型提出,在自主行动过程中,行动指令的传出副本会被发送出去,以预测感觉反馈。然后将预测的感觉反馈与实际的感觉反馈进行比较,从而抑制或减少源自自主行动的感觉刺激。为了进一步阐明感觉衰减效应的神经机制,我们对功能磁共振成像(fMRI)和正电子发射断层扫描(PET)研究进行了广泛的荟萃分析。利用激活似然估计(ALE)分析,我们的结果显示,在比较外部产生和自身产生的条件时,包括右侧颞上回(rSTG)、右侧颞中回(rMTG)和右侧岛叶在内的一个突出集群出现了显著激活。此外,在比较外部生成和自身生成条件时,在右侧小脑前部观察到了明显的激活。使用元分析连接建模(MACM)进行的进一步分析揭示了分别与 rMTG 和右侧小脑共同激活的不同大脑网络。基于这些发现,我们提出,感觉衰减是通过以小脑为中心的行动预测网络的内部前向建模,抑制由自我引发的行动所引起的反射性输入,从而使 "感觉冲突检测 "区域能够有效区分由自我引发的行动和外部输入。
{"title":"The neural network of sensory attenuation: A neuroimaging meta-analysis.","authors":"Jingjin Gu, Tatia Buidze, Ke Zhao, Jan Gläscher, Xiaolan Fu","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02532-1","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02532-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Sensory attenuation refers to the reduction in sensory intensity resulting from self-initiated actions compared to stimuli initiated externally. A classic example is scratching oneself without feeling itchy. This phenomenon extends across various sensory modalities, including visual, auditory, somatosensory, and nociceptive stimuli. The internal forward model proposes that during voluntary actions, an efferent copy of the action command is sent out to predict sensory feedback. This predicted sensory feedback is then compared with the actual sensory feedback, leading to the suppression or reduction of sensory stimuli originating from self-initiated actions. To further elucidate the neural mechanisms underlying sensory attenuation effect, we conducted an extensive meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and positron emission tomography (PET) studies. Utilizing activation likelihood estimation (ALE) analysis, our results revealed significant activations in a prominent cluster encompassing the right superior temporal gyrus (rSTG), right middle temporal gyrus (rMTG), and right insula when comparing external-generated with self-generated conditions. Additionally, significant activation was observed in the right anterior cerebellum when comparing self-generated to external-generated conditions. Further analysis using meta-analytic connectivity modeling (MACM) unveiled distinct brain networks co-activated with the rMTG and right cerebellum, respectively. Based on these findings, we propose that sensory attenuation arises from the suppression of reflexive inputs elicited by self-initiated actions through the internal forward modeling of a cerebellum-centered action prediction network, enabling the \"sensory conflict detection\" regions to effectively discriminate between inputs resulting from self-induced actions and those originating externally.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":"31-51"},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493182","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1