首页 > 最新文献

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review最新文献

英文 中文
Product, not process: Metacognitive monitoring of visual performance during sustained attention. 产品,而非过程:持续注意力期间视觉表现的元认知监测。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9
Cheongil Kim, Sang Chul Chong

The performance of the human visual system exhibits moment-to-moment fluctuations influenced by multiple neurocognitive factors. To deal with this instability of the visual system, introspective awareness of current visual performance (metacognitive monitoring) may be crucial. In this study, we investigate whether and how people can monitor their own visual performance during sustained attention by adopting confidence judgments as indicators of metacognitive monitoring - assuming that if participants can monitor visual performance, confidence judgments will accurately track performance fluctuations. In two experiments (N = 40), we found that participants were able to monitor fluctuations in visual performance during sustained attention. Importantly, metacognitive monitoring largely relied on the quality of target perception, a product of visual processing ("I lack confidence in my performance because I only caught a glimpse of the target"), rather than the states of the visual system during visual processing ("I lack confidence because I was not focusing on the task").

人类视觉系统的表现表现出受多种神经认知因素影响的时刻波动。为了处理这种视觉系统的不稳定性,对当前视觉表现的内省意识(元认知监测)可能是至关重要的。在本研究中,我们通过采用信心判断作为元认知监测的指标来研究人们是否以及如何在持续注意过程中监测自己的视觉表现——假设如果参与者能够监测视觉表现,信心判断将准确地跟踪表现波动。在两个实验(N = 40)中,我们发现参与者能够监测持续注意力期间视觉表现的波动。重要的是,元认知监控很大程度上依赖于目标感知的质量,这是视觉处理的产物(“我对自己的表现缺乏信心,因为我只瞥见了目标”),而不是视觉系统在视觉处理过程中的状态(“我缺乏信心,因为我没有专注于任务”)。
{"title":"Product, not process: Metacognitive monitoring of visual performance during sustained attention.","authors":"Cheongil Kim, Sang Chul Chong","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The performance of the human visual system exhibits moment-to-moment fluctuations influenced by multiple neurocognitive factors. To deal with this instability of the visual system, introspective awareness of current visual performance (metacognitive monitoring) may be crucial. In this study, we investigate whether and how people can monitor their own visual performance during sustained attention by adopting confidence judgments as indicators of metacognitive monitoring - assuming that if participants can monitor visual performance, confidence judgments will accurately track performance fluctuations. In two experiments (N <math><mo>=</mo></math> 40), we found that participants were able to monitor fluctuations in visual performance during sustained attention. Importantly, metacognitive monitoring largely relied on the quality of target perception, a product of visual processing (\"I lack confidence in my performance because I only caught a glimpse of the target\"), rather than the states of the visual system during visual processing (\"I lack confidence because I was not focusing on the task\").</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cracking arbitrariness: A data-driven study of auditory iconicity in spoken English. 破解随意性:英语口语听觉象似性的数据驱动研究。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-08 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0
Andrea Gregor de Varda, Marco Marelli

Auditory iconic words display a phonological profile that imitates their referents' sounds. Traditionally, those words are thought to constitute a minor portion of the auditory lexicon. In this article, we challenge this assumption by assessing the pervasiveness of onomatopoeia in the English auditory vocabulary through a novel data-driven procedure. We embed spoken words and natural sounds into a shared auditory space through (a) a short-time Fourier transform, (b) a convolutional neural network trained to classify sounds, and (c) a network trained on speech recognition. Then, we employ the obtained vector representations to measure their objective auditory resemblance. These similarity indexes show that imitation is not limited to some circumscribed semantic categories, but instead can be considered as a widespread mechanism underlying the structure of the English auditory vocabulary. We finally empirically validate our similarity indexes as measures of iconicity against human judgments.

听觉上的标志性词汇显示出一种模仿其所指的声音的语音特征。传统上,这些词被认为是听觉词汇的一小部分。在本文中,我们通过一种新颖的数据驱动程序来评估拟声词在英语听觉词汇中的普遍性,从而挑战这一假设。我们通过(a)短时傅立叶变换,(b)训练用于声音分类的卷积神经网络,以及(c)训练用于语音识别的网络,将口语单词和自然声音嵌入到共享的听觉空间中。然后,我们使用得到的向量表示来度量它们的客观听觉相似性。这些相似性指标表明,模仿并不局限于某些限定的语义范畴,而是英语听觉词汇结构背后的一种普遍机制。最后,我们通过经验验证了我们的相似性指数作为对人类判断的象似性度量。
{"title":"Cracking arbitrariness: A data-driven study of auditory iconicity in spoken English.","authors":"Andrea Gregor de Varda, Marco Marelli","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Auditory iconic words display a phonological profile that imitates their referents' sounds. Traditionally, those words are thought to constitute a minor portion of the auditory lexicon. In this article, we challenge this assumption by assessing the pervasiveness of onomatopoeia in the English auditory vocabulary through a novel data-driven procedure. We embed spoken words and natural sounds into a shared auditory space through (a) a short-time Fourier transform, (b) a convolutional neural network trained to classify sounds, and (c) a network trained on speech recognition. Then, we employ the obtained vector representations to measure their objective auditory resemblance. These similarity indexes show that imitation is not limited to some circumscribed semantic categories, but instead can be considered as a widespread mechanism underlying the structure of the English auditory vocabulary. We finally empirically validate our similarity indexes as measures of iconicity against human judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953962","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual attention matters during word recognition: A Bayesian modeling approach. 视觉注意力在单词识别中的作用:贝叶斯建模方法。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-07 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4
Thierry Phénix, Émilie Ginestet, Sylviane Valdois, Julien Diard

It is striking that visual attention, the process by which attentional resources are allocated in the visual field so as to locally enhance visual perception, is a pervasive component of models of eye movements in reading, but is seldom considered in models of isolated word recognition. We describe BRAID, a new Bayesian word-Recognition model with Attention, Interference and Dynamics. As most of its predecessors, BRAID incorporates three sensory, perceptual, and orthographic knowledge layers together with a lexical membership submodel. Its originality resides in also including three mechanisms that modulate letter identification within strings: an acuity gradient, lateral interference, and visual attention. We calibrated the model such that its temporal scale was consistent with behavioral data, and then explored the model's capacity to generalize to other, independent effects. We evaluated the model's capacity to account for the word length effect in lexical decision, for the optimal viewing position effect, and for the interaction of crowding and frequency effects in word recognition. We further examined how these effects were modulated by variations in the visual attention distribution. We show that visual attention modulates all three effects and that a narrow distribution of visual attention results in performance patterns that mimic those reported in impaired readers. Overall, the BRAID model could be conceived as a core building block, towards the development of integrated models of reading aloud and eye movement control, or of visual recognition of impaired readers, or any context in which visual attention does matter.

视觉注意是将注意力资源分配到视野中以局部增强视觉感知的过程,它是阅读时眼动模型中普遍存在的组成部分,但在孤立词识别模型中却很少被考虑。本文描述了一种新的贝叶斯词识别模型BRAID,该模型具有注意、干扰和动态特征。与大多数前辈一样,BRAID结合了三个感觉、知觉和正字法知识层,以及一个词法隶属子模型。它的独创性还在于包括三种机制来调节字符串中的字母识别:敏锐度梯度,横向干扰和视觉注意力。我们校准了模型,使其时间尺度与行为数据一致,然后探索模型推广到其他独立效应的能力。我们评估了该模型在词汇决策中的单词长度效应、最佳观看位置效应以及单词识别中拥挤和频率效应的相互作用方面的能力。我们进一步研究了这些影响是如何被视觉注意力分布的变化所调节的。我们发现视觉注意力调节了这三种效应,并且视觉注意力的狭窄分布导致了与受损读者相似的表现模式。总的来说,BRAID模型可以被看作是开发大声朗读和眼球运动控制的综合模型的核心构建块,或者是受损读者的视觉识别,或者是任何视觉注意力重要的环境。
{"title":"Visual attention matters during word recognition: A Bayesian modeling approach.","authors":"Thierry Phénix, Émilie Ginestet, Sylviane Valdois, Julien Diard","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is striking that visual attention, the process by which attentional resources are allocated in the visual field so as to locally enhance visual perception, is a pervasive component of models of eye movements in reading, but is seldom considered in models of isolated word recognition. We describe BRAID, a new Bayesian word-Recognition model with Attention, Interference and Dynamics. As most of its predecessors, BRAID incorporates three sensory, perceptual, and orthographic knowledge layers together with a lexical membership submodel. Its originality resides in also including three mechanisms that modulate letter identification within strings: an acuity gradient, lateral interference, and visual attention. We calibrated the model such that its temporal scale was consistent with behavioral data, and then explored the model's capacity to generalize to other, independent effects. We evaluated the model's capacity to account for the word length effect in lexical decision, for the optimal viewing position effect, and for the interaction of crowding and frequency effects in word recognition. We further examined how these effects were modulated by variations in the visual attention distribution. We show that visual attention modulates all three effects and that a narrow distribution of visual attention results in performance patterns that mimic those reported in impaired readers. Overall, the BRAID model could be conceived as a core building block, towards the development of integrated models of reading aloud and eye movement control, or of visual recognition of impaired readers, or any context in which visual attention does matter.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parameter identifiability in evidence-accumulation models: The effect of error rates on the diffusion decision model and the linear ballistic accumulator. 证据积累模型中的参数可辨识性:错误率对扩散决策模型和线性弹道累积器的影响。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-07 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02621-1
Malte Lüken, Andrew Heathcote, Julia M Haaf, Dora Matzke

A variety of different evidence-accumulation models (EAMs) account for common response time and accuracy patterns in two-alternative forced choice tasks by assuming that subjects collect and sum information from their environment until a response threshold is reached. Estimates of model parameters mapped to components of this decision process can be used to explain the causes of observed behavior. However, such explanations are only meaningful when parameters can be identified, that is, when their values can be uniquely estimated from data generated by the model. Prior studies suggest that parameter identifiability is poor when error rates are low but have not systematically compared this issue across different EAMs. We conducted a simulation study investigating the identifiability and estimation properties of model parameters at low error rates in the two most popular EAMs: The diffusion decision model (DDM) and the linear ballistic accumulator (LBA). We found poor identifiability at low error rates for both models but less so for the DDM and for a larger number of trials. The DDM also showed better identifiability than the LBA at low trial numbers for a design with a manipulation of response caution. Based on our results, we recommend tasks with error rates between 15% and 35% for small, and between 5% and 35% for large trial numbers. We explain the identifiability problem in terms of trade-offs caused by correlations between decision-threshold and accumulation-rate parameters and discuss why the models differ in terms of their estimation properties.

各种不同的证据积累模型(eam)通过假设受试者从其环境中收集和汇总信息直到达到反应阈值,来解释两种选择任务中常见的反应时间和准确性模式。映射到该决策过程组件的模型参数的估计可用于解释观察到的行为的原因。然而,只有当参数可以被识别时,也就是说,当它们的值可以从模型产生的数据中唯一地估计出来时,这种解释才有意义。先前的研究表明,当错误率较低时,参数可辨识性较差,但尚未系统地比较不同eam之间的这一问题。我们进行了一项仿真研究,探讨了两种最流行的EAMs:扩散决策模型(DDM)和线性弹道累加器(LBA)在低错误率下模型参数的可识别性和估计特性。我们发现两种模型在低错误率下的可识别性较差,但DDM模型和大量试验的可识别性较差。在低试验数的情况下,DDM也表现出比LBA更好的可识别性。根据我们的结果,我们建议任务的错误率在小规模试验中为15%到35%,在大规模试验中为5%到35%。我们根据决策阈值和累积率参数之间的相关性引起的权衡来解释可识别性问题,并讨论了模型在其估计属性方面不同的原因。
{"title":"Parameter identifiability in evidence-accumulation models: The effect of error rates on the diffusion decision model and the linear ballistic accumulator.","authors":"Malte Lüken, Andrew Heathcote, Julia M Haaf, Dora Matzke","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02621-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02621-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A variety of different evidence-accumulation models (EAMs) account for common response time and accuracy patterns in two-alternative forced choice tasks by assuming that subjects collect and sum information from their environment until a response threshold is reached. Estimates of model parameters mapped to components of this decision process can be used to explain the causes of observed behavior. However, such explanations are only meaningful when parameters can be identified, that is, when their values can be uniquely estimated from data generated by the model. Prior studies suggest that parameter identifiability is poor when error rates are low but have not systematically compared this issue across different EAMs. We conducted a simulation study investigating the identifiability and estimation properties of model parameters at low error rates in the two most popular EAMs: The diffusion decision model (DDM) and the linear ballistic accumulator (LBA). We found poor identifiability at low error rates for both models but less so for the DDM and for a larger number of trials. The DDM also showed better identifiability than the LBA at low trial numbers for a design with a manipulation of response caution. Based on our results, we recommend tasks with error rates between 15% and 35% for small, and between 5% and 35% for large trial numbers. We explain the identifiability problem in terms of trade-offs caused by correlations between decision-threshold and accumulation-rate parameters and discuss why the models differ in terms of their estimation properties.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953963","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Nudges for people who think. 对有思想的人的鞭策。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02613-1
Aba Szollosi, Nathan Wang-Ly, Ben R Newell

The naiveté of the dominant 'cognitive-miser' metaphor of human thinking hampers theoretical progress in understanding how and why subtle behavioural interventions-'nudges'-could work. We propose a reconceptualization that places the balance in agency between, and the alignment of representations held by, people and choice architects as central to determining the prospect of observing behaviour change. We argue that two aspects of representational (mis)alignment are relevant: cognitive (how people construe the factual structure of a decision environment) and motivational (the importance of a choice to an individual). Nudging thinkers via the alignment of representations provides a framework that offers theoretical and practical advances and avoids disparaging people's cognitive capacities.

“认知吝啬鬼”这一占主导地位的人类思维隐喻的幼稚,阻碍了理解微妙的行为干预(“轻推”)如何以及为何起作用的理论进步。我们提出了一种重新概念化,将人与选择建筑师之间的代理平衡和代表的一致性作为确定观察行为变化前景的核心。我们认为表征(错误)一致性的两个方面是相关的:认知(人们如何解释决策环境的事实结构)和动机(选择对个人的重要性)。通过表征的一致性来推动思考者提供了一个框架,提供了理论和实践的进步,并避免贬低人们的认知能力。
{"title":"Nudges for people who think.","authors":"Aba Szollosi, Nathan Wang-Ly, Ben R Newell","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02613-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02613-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The naiveté of the dominant 'cognitive-miser' metaphor of human thinking hampers theoretical progress in understanding how and why subtle behavioural interventions-'nudges'-could work. We propose a reconceptualization that places the balance in agency between, and the alignment of representations held by, people and choice architects as central to determining the prospect of observing behaviour change. We argue that two aspects of representational (mis)alignment are relevant: cognitive (how people construe the factual structure of a decision environment) and motivational (the importance of a choice to an individual). Nudging thinkers via the alignment of representations provides a framework that offers theoretical and practical advances and avoids disparaging people's cognitive capacities.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142927903","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Generating distant analogies increases metaphor production. 产生遥远的类比会增加隐喻的产生。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02628-8
Tim George, Andriana L Christofalos, Felix S Pambuccian

Although a large body of work has explored the mechanisms underlying metaphor comprehension, less research has focused on spontaneous metaphor production. Previous research suggests that reasoning about analogies can induce a relational mindset, which causes a greater focus on underlying abstract similarities. We explored how inducing a relational mindset may increase the tendency to use metaphors to describe topics. Participants first solved a series of either cross-domain (i.e., far) analogies (kitten:cat::spark-?) to induce a high relational mindset or within-domain (i.e., near) analogies (kitten:cat::puppy-?) (control condition). Next, they received a series of topic descriptions containing either one feature (some jobs are confining) or three features (some jobs are confining, repetitive, and unpleasant), and were asked to provide a summary phrase of the topic. Use of metaphoric language increased when topics contained more features, and was particularly frequent in the high relational mindset condition. This finding suggests that the relational mindset induction may have shifted attention toward abstract comparisons, thereby facilitating the creative use of language involving metaphors.

虽然已有大量研究探讨隐喻理解的机制,但对自发隐喻产生的研究较少。先前的研究表明,对类比的推理可以诱发一种关系思维模式,从而使人们更加关注潜在的抽象相似性。我们探讨了如何诱导关系心态可能会增加使用隐喻来描述主题的倾向。参与者首先解决一系列跨领域(即远)类比(小猫:猫::火花-?)以诱导高度关系思维,或解决领域内(即近)类比(小猫:猫::小狗-?)(控制条件)。接下来,他们收到了一系列主题描述,其中包括一个特征(一些工作是限制性的)或三个特征(一些工作是限制性的、重复的和不愉快的),并被要求提供主题的总结短语。当话题包含更多特征时,隐喻语言的使用就会增加,在高关系思维条件下尤其频繁。这一发现表明,关系思维诱导可能将注意力转移到抽象比较上,从而促进了涉及隐喻的语言的创造性使用。
{"title":"Generating distant analogies increases metaphor production.","authors":"Tim George, Andriana L Christofalos, Felix S Pambuccian","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02628-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02628-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although a large body of work has explored the mechanisms underlying metaphor comprehension, less research has focused on spontaneous metaphor production. Previous research suggests that reasoning about analogies can induce a relational mindset, which causes a greater focus on underlying abstract similarities. We explored how inducing a relational mindset may increase the tendency to use metaphors to describe topics. Participants first solved a series of either cross-domain (i.e., far) analogies (kitten:cat::spark-?) to induce a high relational mindset or within-domain (i.e., near) analogies (kitten:cat::puppy-?) (control condition). Next, they received a series of topic descriptions containing either one feature (some jobs are confining) or three features (some jobs are confining, repetitive, and unpleasant), and were asked to provide a summary phrase of the topic. Use of metaphoric language increased when topics contained more features, and was particularly frequent in the high relational mindset condition. This finding suggests that the relational mindset induction may have shifted attention toward abstract comparisons, thereby facilitating the creative use of language involving metaphors.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142927878","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Memories of hand movements are tied to speech through learning. 手部动作的记忆是通过学习与语言联系在一起的。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02618-w
Daniel R Lametti, Gina L Vaillancourt, Maura A Whitman, Jeremy I Skipper

Hand movements frequently occur with speech. The extent to which the memories that guide co-speech hand movements are tied to the speech they occur with is unclear. Here, we paired the acquisition of a new hand movement with speech. Thirty participants adapted a ballistic hand movement of a joystick to a visuomotor rotation either in isolation or while producing a word in time with their movements. Within participants, the after-effect of adaptation (i.e., the motor memory) was examined with or without co-incident speech. After-effects were greater for hand movements produced in the context in which adaptation occurred - i.e., with or without speech. In a second experiment, 30 new participants adapted a hand movement while saying the words "tap" or "hit". After-effects were greater when hand movements occurred with the specific word produced during adaptation. The results demonstrate that memories of co-speech hand movements are partially tied to the speech they are learned with. The findings have implications for theories of sensorimotor control and our understanding of the relationship between gestures, speech and meaning.

说话时经常会有手部动作。目前还不清楚,指导共同语言的手部运动的记忆在多大程度上与它们所使用的语言相关联。在这里,我们将获得新的手部动作与语言配对。30名参与者将操纵杆的弹道式手部运动调整为视觉运动旋转,要么是孤立的,要么是随着他们的动作及时产生一个单词。在参与者内部,研究了有或没有同步言语的适应后效应(即运动记忆)。在适应发生的环境中——即有或没有语言——产生的手部运动的后遗症更大。在第二项实验中,30名新参与者在说“点击”或“点击”时适应了手部动作。在适应过程中产生特定单词时,手的动作产生的后遗症更大。结果表明,共同语言的手部运动的记忆部分与他们所学的语言有关。这一发现对感觉运动控制理论和我们对手势、言语和意义之间关系的理解具有启示意义。
{"title":"Memories of hand movements are tied to speech through learning.","authors":"Daniel R Lametti, Gina L Vaillancourt, Maura A Whitman, Jeremy I Skipper","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02618-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02618-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Hand movements frequently occur with speech. The extent to which the memories that guide co-speech hand movements are tied to the speech they occur with is unclear. Here, we paired the acquisition of a new hand movement with speech. Thirty participants adapted a ballistic hand movement of a joystick to a visuomotor rotation either in isolation or while producing a word in time with their movements. Within participants, the after-effect of adaptation (i.e., the motor memory) was examined with or without co-incident speech. After-effects were greater for hand movements produced in the context in which adaptation occurred - i.e., with or without speech. In a second experiment, 30 new participants adapted a hand movement while saying the words \"tap\" or \"hit\". After-effects were greater when hand movements occurred with the specific word produced during adaptation. The results demonstrate that memories of co-speech hand movements are partially tied to the speech they are learned with. The findings have implications for theories of sensorimotor control and our understanding of the relationship between gestures, speech and meaning.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142927896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The influence of increasing color variety on numerosity estimation and counting. 增加颜色变化对数量估计和计数的影响。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02625-x
Qi Li, Guo Ting, Yuichiro Kikuno, Yokosawa Kazuhiko

Previous research has suggested that numerosity estimation and counting are closely related to distributed and focused attention, respectively (Chong & Evans, WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(6), 634-638, 2011). Given the critical role of color in guiding attention, this study investigated its effects on numerosity processing by manipulating both color variety (single color, medium variety, high variety) and spatial arrangement (clustered, random). Results from the estimation task revealed that high color variety led to a perceptual bias towards larger quantities, regardless of whether colors were clustered or randomly arranged. This implies that distributed attention may engage in a global assessment of color richness, with less emphasis on spatial arrangement. In contrast, the effect of color on counting was influenced by spatial arrangement: performance improved with clustered colors but declined with random color distribution. This indicates that color interacts with spatial information to modulate focused attention during serial numerosity processing. Taken together, our findings provide new insights into the interaction between numerical cognition and attention, highlighting the need for theories and models of numerical cognition to take into account feature variety and contextual factors, such as the spatial arrangement of features. Additionally, in light of the widespread diversity in real-world environments, our findings could inform strategies to enhance behavioral adaptation to varying environmental conditions.

先前的研究表明,数量估计和计数分别与分布注意力和集中注意力密切相关(Chong & Evans, WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(6), 634-638, 2011)。考虑到颜色在注意力引导中的重要作用,本研究从颜色种类(单色、中色、高色)和空间排列(聚类、随机)两方面考察了颜色对数字加工的影响。估计任务的结果显示,无论颜色是聚类还是随机排列,高颜色多样性都会导致对较大数量的感知偏差。这意味着,分布式注意力可能会参与对色彩丰富度的整体评估,而对空间安排的重视程度较低。相反,颜色对计数的影响受空间排列的影响:聚类颜色的计数性能提高,随机颜色分布的计数性能下降。这表明颜色与空间信息相互作用,在序列数字处理过程中调节集中注意力。综上所述,我们的研究结果为数字认知和注意力之间的相互作用提供了新的见解,强调了数字认知的理论和模型需要考虑特征的多样性和背景因素,如特征的空间排列。此外,鉴于现实环境的广泛多样性,我们的研究结果可以为增强对不同环境条件的行为适应提供策略。
{"title":"The influence of increasing color variety on numerosity estimation and counting.","authors":"Qi Li, Guo Ting, Yuichiro Kikuno, Yokosawa Kazuhiko","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02625-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02625-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research has suggested that numerosity estimation and counting are closely related to distributed and focused attention, respectively (Chong & Evans, WIREs Cognitive Science, 2(6), 634-638, 2011). Given the critical role of color in guiding attention, this study investigated its effects on numerosity processing by manipulating both color variety (single color, medium variety, high variety) and spatial arrangement (clustered, random). Results from the estimation task revealed that high color variety led to a perceptual bias towards larger quantities, regardless of whether colors were clustered or randomly arranged. This implies that distributed attention may engage in a global assessment of color richness, with less emphasis on spatial arrangement. In contrast, the effect of color on counting was influenced by spatial arrangement: performance improved with clustered colors but declined with random color distribution. This indicates that color interacts with spatial information to modulate focused attention during serial numerosity processing. Taken together, our findings provide new insights into the interaction between numerical cognition and attention, highlighting the need for theories and models of numerical cognition to take into account feature variety and contextual factors, such as the spatial arrangement of features. Additionally, in light of the widespread diversity in real-world environments, our findings could inform strategies to enhance behavioral adaptation to varying environmental conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142927904","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical integration of novel words learned through natural reading. 通过自然阅读学习新单词的词汇整合。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02626-w
Anezka Smejkalova, Fabienne Chetail

Lexical competition between newly acquired and already established representations of written words is considered a marker of word integration into the mental lexicon. To date, studies about the emergence of lexical competition involved mostly artificial training procedures based on overexposure and explicit instructions for memorization. Yet, in real life, novel word encounters occur mostly without explicit learning intent, through reading texts with words appearing rarely. This study examined the lexical integration of words learned through text reading. In Experiment 1, two groups of participants read a short book with embedded novel words. Only one group was asked to memorize the unfamiliar words. In the semantic categorization task, we found evidence for lexical competition with slower responses to existing orthographic neighbors (e.g., hublot) of the newly learned words (e.g., hubbot) than to a set of matched items. This effect was found independently of the group 24 h after initial exposure. In addition, a facilitation pattern was observed immediately after the reading session. However, post hoc analyses suggested that the competition effect was mainly driven by the data from the group receiving explicit learning instructions. Experiment 2 aimed to replicate the findings obtained in the group without explicit learning instructions. The results revealed the same pattern, characterized by a facilitatory effect immediately after the reading session and an inhibitory effect 24 h after the exposure. Overall, these results showed that lexical competition emerged from a naturalistic reading after a delay, regardless of whether participants were asked to learn novel words or not.

新获得的和已经建立的书面词汇表征之间的词汇竞争被认为是词汇融入心理词汇的标志。迄今为止,关于词汇竞争产生的研究主要涉及基于过度暴露和明确记忆指令的人工训练程序。然而,在现实生活中,新奇的单词相遇大多是在没有明确的学习意图的情况下发生的,通过阅读很少出现单词的文本。本研究考察了通过文本阅读习得词汇的词汇整合。在实验1中,两组参与者阅读一本嵌入小说单词的小书。只有一组被要求记忆不熟悉的单词。在语义分类任务中,我们发现了词汇竞争的证据,即对新学单词(如hubbot)的现有正字法相邻词(如hublot)的反应比对一组匹配词(如hubbot)的反应慢。这种影响是在初次接触24小时后独立发现的。此外,阅读结束后立即观察到一个促进模式。​​结果显示了相同的模式,即在阅读后立即产生促进效应,而在阅读后24小时产生抑制效应。总的来说,这些结果表明,无论参与者是否被要求学习新单词,词汇竞争都会在一段时间后从自然阅读中出现。
{"title":"Lexical integration of novel words learned through natural reading.","authors":"Anezka Smejkalova, Fabienne Chetail","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02626-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02626-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Lexical competition between newly acquired and already established representations of written words is considered a marker of word integration into the mental lexicon. To date, studies about the emergence of lexical competition involved mostly artificial training procedures based on overexposure and explicit instructions for memorization. Yet, in real life, novel word encounters occur mostly without explicit learning intent, through reading texts with words appearing rarely. This study examined the lexical integration of words learned through text reading. In Experiment 1, two groups of participants read a short book with embedded novel words. Only one group was asked to memorize the unfamiliar words. In the semantic categorization task, we found evidence for lexical competition with slower responses to existing orthographic neighbors (e.g., hublot) of the newly learned words (e.g., hubbot) than to a set of matched items. This effect was found independently of the group 24 h after initial exposure. In addition, a facilitation pattern was observed immediately after the reading session. However, post hoc analyses suggested that the competition effect was mainly driven by the data from the group receiving explicit learning instructions. Experiment 2 aimed to replicate the findings obtained in the group without explicit learning instructions. The results revealed the same pattern, characterized by a facilitatory effect immediately after the reading session and an inhibitory effect 24 h after the exposure. Overall, these results showed that lexical competition emerged from a naturalistic reading after a delay, regardless of whether participants were asked to learn novel words or not.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142882869","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The mnemonic potency of functional facts. 功能性事实的助记能力。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02617-x
Stuart Wilson

Learning and remembering what things are used for is a capacity that is central to successfully living in any human culture. The current paper investigates whether functional facts (information about what an object is used for) are remembered more efficiently compared with nonfunctional facts. Experiment 1 presented participants with images of functionally ambiguous objects associated with a (made-up) name and a (made-up) fact that could relate either to the object's function or to something nonfunctional. Results show that recall of object names did not depend on whether they were associated with a functional or nonfunctional fact, while recall of the functional facts was significantly better than the nonfunctional facts. The second experiment replicated this main effect and further found that functional facts are remembered more efficiently after they have been associated with confirmatory (as opposed to disconfirmatory) feedback. It is suggested that semantic information is not unitary, and that one way of categorising semantic information is in terms of its adaptive relevance. Potential mechanisms are proposed and discussed, along with suggestions for future research.

学习和记住东西的用途是在任何人类文化中成功生活的核心能力。本论文研究了功能性事实(关于物体用途的信息)是否比非功能性事实更有效地被记住。实验1向参与者展示了功能模糊的物体的图像,这些物体与一个(虚构的)名称和一个(虚构的)事实相关联,这些事实可能与物体的功能有关,也可能与一些非功能的东西有关。结果表明,被试对物品名称的记忆不依赖于与功能性事实或非功能性事实的关联,而功能性事实的记忆能力明显优于非功能性事实。第二个实验重复了这一主要效应,并进一步发现,在与证实性反馈(而不是非证实性反馈)联系在一起后,功能性事实被记忆得更有效。认为语义信息不是单一的,语义信息的分类方法之一是根据语义信息的自适应相关性进行分类。提出并讨论了潜在的机制,并对未来的研究提出了建议。
{"title":"The mnemonic potency of functional facts.","authors":"Stuart Wilson","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02617-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02617-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning and remembering what things are used for is a capacity that is central to successfully living in any human culture. The current paper investigates whether functional facts (information about what an object is used for) are remembered more efficiently compared with nonfunctional facts. Experiment 1 presented participants with images of functionally ambiguous objects associated with a (made-up) name and a (made-up) fact that could relate either to the object's function or to something nonfunctional. Results show that recall of object names did not depend on whether they were associated with a functional or nonfunctional fact, while recall of the functional facts was significantly better than the nonfunctional facts. The second experiment replicated this main effect and further found that functional facts are remembered more efficiently after they have been associated with confirmatory (as opposed to disconfirmatory) feedback. It is suggested that semantic information is not unitary, and that one way of categorising semantic information is in terms of its adaptive relevance. Potential mechanisms are proposed and discussed, along with suggestions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142872641","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1