首页 > 最新文献

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review最新文献

英文 中文
Prokofiev was (almost) right: A cross-cultural investigation of auditory-conceptual associations in Peter and the Wolf. 普罗科菲耶夫(几乎)是对的:彼得与狼》中听觉-概念关联的跨文化调查。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02435-7
Nicola Di Stefano, Alessandro Ansani, Andrea Schiavio, Charles Spence

Over recent decades, studies investigating cross-modal correspondences have documented the existence of a wide range of consistent cross-modal associations between simple auditory and visual stimuli or dimensions (e.g., pitch-lightness). Far fewer studies have investigated the association between complex and realistic auditory stimuli and visually presented concepts (e.g., musical excerpts-animals). Surprisingly, however, there is little evidence concerning the extent to which these associations are shared across cultures. To address this gap in the literature, two experiments using a set of stimuli based on Prokofiev's symphonic fairy tale Peter and the Wolf are reported. In Experiment 1, 293 participants from several countries and with very different language backgrounds rated the association between the musical excerpts, images and words representing the story's characters (namely, bird, duck, wolf, cat, and grandfather). The results revealed that participants tended to consistently associate the wolf and the bird with the corresponding musical excerpt, while the stimuli of other characters were not consistently matched across participants. Remarkably, neither the participants' cultural background, nor their musical expertise affected the ratings. In Experiment 2, 104 participants were invited to rate each stimulus on eight emotional features. The results revealed that the emotional profiles associated with the music and with the concept of the wolf and the bird were perceived as more consistent between observers than the emotional profiles associated with the music and the concept of the duck, the cat, and the grandpa. Taken together, these findings therefore suggest that certain auditory-conceptual associations are perceived consistently across cultures and may be mediated by emotional associations.

近几十年来,有关跨模态对应关系的研究记录了简单的听觉和视觉刺激或维度(如音调-亮度)之间存在的广泛一致的跨模态关联。而研究复杂逼真的听觉刺激与视觉呈现的概念(如音乐选段-动物)之间关联的研究则少得多。然而,令人惊讶的是,几乎没有证据表明这些关联在多大程度上是跨文化共享的。为了弥补文献中的这一空白,本研究报告使用了一组基于普罗科菲耶夫的交响童话《彼得与狼》的刺激物进行了两项实验。在实验 1 中,来自多个国家、语言背景迥异的 293 名参与者对代表故事人物(即鸟、鸭子、狼、猫和爷爷)的音乐选段、图像和文字之间的关联进行了评分。结果显示,参与者倾向于一致地将狼和鸟与相应的音乐选段联系起来,而其他角色的刺激在不同参与者之间并不一致。值得注意的是,参与者的文化背景和音乐专长都不会影响评分。在实验 2 中,104 名参与者应邀对每个刺激物的八个情感特征进行评分。结果显示,与音乐以及 "狼 "和 "鸟 "的概念相关的情感特征在观察者之间的一致性要高于与音乐以及 "鸭子"、"猫 "和 "爷爷 "的概念相关的情感特征。综上所述,这些研究结果表明,某些听觉-概念关联在不同文化中的感知是一致的,并且可能受情感关联的影响。
{"title":"Prokofiev was (almost) right: A cross-cultural investigation of auditory-conceptual associations in Peter and the Wolf.","authors":"Nicola Di Stefano, Alessandro Ansani, Andrea Schiavio, Charles Spence","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02435-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02435-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Over recent decades, studies investigating cross-modal correspondences have documented the existence of a wide range of consistent cross-modal associations between simple auditory and visual stimuli or dimensions (e.g., pitch-lightness). Far fewer studies have investigated the association between complex and realistic auditory stimuli and visually presented concepts (e.g., musical excerpts-animals). Surprisingly, however, there is little evidence concerning the extent to which these associations are shared across cultures. To address this gap in the literature, two experiments using a set of stimuli based on Prokofiev's symphonic fairy tale Peter and the Wolf are reported. In Experiment 1, 293 participants from several countries and with very different language backgrounds rated the association between the musical excerpts, images and words representing the story's characters (namely, bird, duck, wolf, cat, and grandfather). The results revealed that participants tended to consistently associate the wolf and the bird with the corresponding musical excerpt, while the stimuli of other characters were not consistently matched across participants. Remarkably, neither the participants' cultural background, nor their musical expertise affected the ratings. In Experiment 2, 104 participants were invited to rate each stimulus on eight emotional features. The results revealed that the emotional profiles associated with the music and with the concept of the wolf and the bird were perceived as more consistent between observers than the emotional profiles associated with the music and the concept of the duck, the cat, and the grandpa. Taken together, these findings therefore suggest that certain auditory-conceptual associations are perceived consistently across cultures and may be mediated by emotional associations.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358347/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139547038","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No effect of spatial congruence on rapid temporal recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony. 空间一致性对视听不同步的快速时间重新校准没有影响。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02441-9
Kyuto Uno, Souta Hidaka

The brain integrates multisensory information to construct coherent perceptual representations based on spatial and temporal congruence. Intriguingly, multisensory timing perception can be flexibly calibrated. Repeated exposure to audiovisual asynchrony induces shifts in subjective simultaneity (temporal recalibration). Spatial congruence is known to serve as a grouping cue for recalibration when the audiovisual temporal relationship is ambiguous during exposure. A single exposure to audiovisual asynchrony can also trigger temporal recalibration (rapid recalibration). However, it has been suggested that the underlying mechanisms of these temporal recalibrations differ. Here, we examined whether spatial congruence can be a grouping cue for rapid recalibration when audiovisual pairs are not defined by temporal relationships. Participants made a simultaneity judgment for a pair of audiovisual stimuli after adapting three consecutive stimuli once in a "light-sound-light" or "sound-light-sound" order with an equal temporal interval. The spatial positions of the adapting stimuli were manipulated as an audiovisual pair from the same position (e.g., left) and the remaining stimulus from another position (e.g., right). In three experiments, the spatial congruence of the audiovisual adapting stimuli did not show a modulatory effect, while we replicated the rapid recalibration effects. Rather, rapid recalibration occurred according to the temporal order of the first light and sound. Our findings suggest that, in contrast to temporal recalibration with repeated exposure, the perceptual systems underlying rapid recalibration simply combine individual visual and auditory inputs based on the order in which they arrive.

大脑会整合多感官信息,在空间和时间一致性的基础上构建连贯的感知表征。有趣的是,多感官时间感知可以灵活校准。反复接触视听不同步会诱发主观同时性的变化(时间重新校准)。众所周知,当视听时间关系模糊不清时,空间一致性可作为重新校准的分组线索。单次接触视听不同步也会触发时间重新校准(快速重新校准)。然而,有人认为这些时间重新校准的潜在机制是不同的。在此,我们研究了当视听对没有时间关系时,空间一致性是否可以作为快速重新校准的分组线索。受试者按照 "光-声-光 "或 "声-光-声 "的顺序,以相等的时间间隔调整三个连续的刺激物,然后对一对视听刺激物做出同时性判断。适应刺激的空间位置是由同一位置(如左侧)的一对视听刺激和另一位置(如右侧)的剩余刺激组成的。在三项实验中,视听适应刺激的空间一致性并未显示出调节效应,而我们却复制了快速重新校准效应。相反,快速重新校准是根据第一束光和声的时间顺序发生的。我们的研究结果表明,与重复暴露时的时间重新校准不同,快速重新校准的感知系统只是根据视觉和听觉输入的先后顺序对它们进行组合。
{"title":"No effect of spatial congruence on rapid temporal recalibration to audiovisual asynchrony.","authors":"Kyuto Uno, Souta Hidaka","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02441-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02441-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The brain integrates multisensory information to construct coherent perceptual representations based on spatial and temporal congruence. Intriguingly, multisensory timing perception can be flexibly calibrated. Repeated exposure to audiovisual asynchrony induces shifts in subjective simultaneity (temporal recalibration). Spatial congruence is known to serve as a grouping cue for recalibration when the audiovisual temporal relationship is ambiguous during exposure. A single exposure to audiovisual asynchrony can also trigger temporal recalibration (rapid recalibration). However, it has been suggested that the underlying mechanisms of these temporal recalibrations differ. Here, we examined whether spatial congruence can be a grouping cue for rapid recalibration when audiovisual pairs are not defined by temporal relationships. Participants made a simultaneity judgment for a pair of audiovisual stimuli after adapting three consecutive stimuli once in a \"light-sound-light\" or \"sound-light-sound\" order with an equal temporal interval. The spatial positions of the adapting stimuli were manipulated as an audiovisual pair from the same position (e.g., left) and the remaining stimulus from another position (e.g., right). In three experiments, the spatial congruence of the audiovisual adapting stimuli did not show a modulatory effect, while we replicated the rapid recalibration effects. Rather, rapid recalibration occurred according to the temporal order of the first light and sound. Our findings suggest that, in contrast to temporal recalibration with repeated exposure, the perceptual systems underlying rapid recalibration simply combine individual visual and auditory inputs based on the order in which they arrive.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139088142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Psychometrics in experimental psychology: A case for calibration. 实验心理学中的心理测量学:校准案例。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-26 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02421-z
Dominik R Bach

Psychometrics is historically grounded in the study of individual differences. Consequently, common metrics such as quantitative validity and reliability require between-person variance in a psychological variable to be meaningful. Experimental psychology, in contrast, deals with variance between treatments, and experiments often strive to minimise within-group person variance. In this article, I ask whether and how psychometric evaluation can be performed in experimental psychology. A commonly used strategy is to harness between-person variance in the treatment effect. Using simulated data, I show that this approach can be misleading when between-person variance is low, and in the face of methods variance. I argue that this situation is common in experimental psychology, because low between-person variance is desirable, and because methods variance is no more problematic in experimental settings than any other source of between-person variance. By relating validity and reliability with the corresponding concepts in measurement science outside psychology, I show how experiment-based calibration can serve to compare the psychometric quality of different measurement methods in experimental psychology.

心理测量学历来以研究个体差异为基础。因此,定量效度和信度等常用指标要求心理变量的人际差异才有意义。与此相反,实验心理学研究的是处理方法之间的差异,而实验通常会努力将组内个体差异降到最低。在本文中,我将探讨是否可以以及如何在实验心理学中进行心理测量评估。一种常用的策略是利用治疗效果的人际差异。通过模拟数据,我表明当人与人之间的差异较低时,面对方法差异,这种方法可能会产生误导。我认为,这种情况在实验心理学中很常见,因为低人与人之间的方差是可取的,而且在实验环境中,方法方差并不比人与人之间方差的其他来源更成问题。通过将效度和信度与心理学之外的测量科学中的相应概念联系起来,我说明了基于实验的校准如何能够用于比较实验心理学中不同测量方法的心理测量质量。
{"title":"Psychometrics in experimental psychology: A case for calibration.","authors":"Dominik R Bach","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02421-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02421-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Psychometrics is historically grounded in the study of individual differences. Consequently, common metrics such as quantitative validity and reliability require between-person variance in a psychological variable to be meaningful. Experimental psychology, in contrast, deals with variance between treatments, and experiments often strive to minimise within-group person variance. In this article, I ask whether and how psychometric evaluation can be performed in experimental psychology. A commonly used strategy is to harness between-person variance in the treatment effect. Using simulated data, I show that this approach can be misleading when between-person variance is low, and in the face of methods variance. I argue that this situation is common in experimental psychology, because low between-person variance is desirable, and because methods variance is no more problematic in experimental settings than any other source of between-person variance. By relating validity and reliability with the corresponding concepts in measurement science outside psychology, I show how experiment-based calibration can serve to compare the psychometric quality of different measurement methods in experimental psychology.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358352/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139040449","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-dependent changes in the anger superiority effect: Evidence from a visual search task. 愤怒优势效应随年龄的变化:来自视觉搜索任务的证据
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02401-3
Francesco Ceccarini, Ilaria Colpizzi, Corrado Caudek

The perception of threatening facial expressions is a critical skill necessary for detecting the emotional states of others and responding appropriately. The anger superiority effect hypothesis suggests that individuals are better at processing and identifying angry faces compared with other nonthreatening facial expressions. In adults, the anger superiority effect is present even after controlling for the bottom-up visual saliency, and when ecologically valid stimuli are used. However, it is as yet unclear whether this effect is present in children. To fill this gap, we tested the anger superiority effect in children ages 6-14 years in a visual search task by using emotional dynamic stimuli and equating the visual salience of target and distractors. The results suggest that in childhood, the angry superiority effect consists of improved accuracy in detecting angry faces, while in adolescence, the ability to discriminate angry faces undergoes further development, enabling faster and more accurate threat detection.

感知威胁性面部表情是检测他人情绪状态并做出适当反应的一项重要技能。愤怒优势效应假说认为,与其他非威胁性面部表情相比,个体更善于处理和识别愤怒的面部表情。在成人中,即使控制了自下而上的视觉显著性,并使用生态学上有效的刺激物,愤怒优势效应仍然存在。然而,目前还不清楚儿童是否也存在这种效应。为了填补这一空白,我们在一项视觉搜索任务中使用了情绪动态刺激物,并将目标物和干扰物的视觉显著性等同起来,从而测试了 6-14 岁儿童的愤怒优势效应。结果表明,在儿童时期,愤怒优势效应包括提高检测愤怒面孔的准确性,而在青少年时期,辨别愤怒面孔的能力会进一步发展,从而能够更快、更准确地检测威胁。
{"title":"Age-dependent changes in the anger superiority effect: Evidence from a visual search task.","authors":"Francesco Ceccarini, Ilaria Colpizzi, Corrado Caudek","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02401-3","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02401-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The perception of threatening facial expressions is a critical skill necessary for detecting the emotional states of others and responding appropriately. The anger superiority effect hypothesis suggests that individuals are better at processing and identifying angry faces compared with other nonthreatening facial expressions. In adults, the anger superiority effect is present even after controlling for the bottom-up visual saliency, and when ecologically valid stimuli are used. However, it is as yet unclear whether this effect is present in children. To fill this gap, we tested the anger superiority effect in children ages 6-14 years in a visual search task by using emotional dynamic stimuli and equating the visual salience of target and distractors. The results suggest that in childhood, the angry superiority effect consists of improved accuracy in detecting angry faces, while in adolescence, the ability to discriminate angry faces undergoes further development, enabling faster and more accurate threat detection.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358229/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139491941","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Jumping and leaping estimations using optic flow. 利用光流进行跳跃和飞跃估算。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-29 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02459-7
Lisa P Y Lin, Sally A Linkenauger

Optic flow provides information on movement direction and speed during locomotion. Changing the relationship between optic flow and walking speed via training has been shown to influence subsequent distance and hill steepness estimations. Previous research has shown that experience with slow optic flow at a given walking speed was associated with increased effort and distance overestimation in comparison to experiencing with fast optic flow at the same walking speed. Here, we investigated whether exposure to different optic flow speeds relative to gait influences perceptions of leaping and jumping ability. Participants estimated their maximum leaping and jumping ability after exposure to either fast or moderate optic flow at the same walking speed. Those calibrated to fast optic flow estimated farther leaping and jumping abilities than those calibrated to moderate optic flow. Findings suggest that recalibration between optic flow and walking speed may specify an action boundary when calibrated or scaled to actions such as leaping, and possibly, the manipulation of optic flow speed has resulted in a change in the associated anticipated effort for walking a prescribed distance, which in turn influence one's perceived action capabilities for jumping and leaping.

在运动过程中,视流提供了运动方向和速度的信息。事实证明,通过训练改变视流与步行速度之间的关系会影响随后的距离和山坡陡度估计。以前的研究表明,与相同步行速度下的快速光流相比,在给定步行速度下的慢速光流经验与努力增加和距离估计过高有关。在此,我们研究了相对于步态的不同光流速度是否会影响对跳跃能力的感知。在相同步行速度下,受试者在暴露于快速或中度光流后估计了自己的最大跳跃能力。校准为快速光流的人比校准为中等光流的人估计的跳跃能力更远。研究结果表明,在对跳跃等动作进行校准或缩放时,视流和步行速度之间的重新校准可能会指定一个动作边界,而且,对视流速度的操纵可能会导致步行规定距离的相关预期努力发生变化,进而影响人们对跳跃和跃起动作能力的感知。
{"title":"Jumping and leaping estimations using optic flow.","authors":"Lisa P Y Lin, Sally A Linkenauger","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02459-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02459-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Optic flow provides information on movement direction and speed during locomotion. Changing the relationship between optic flow and walking speed via training has been shown to influence subsequent distance and hill steepness estimations. Previous research has shown that experience with slow optic flow at a given walking speed was associated with increased effort and distance overestimation in comparison to experiencing with fast optic flow at the same walking speed. Here, we investigated whether exposure to different optic flow speeds relative to gait influences perceptions of leaping and jumping ability. Participants estimated their maximum leaping and jumping ability after exposure to either fast or moderate optic flow at the same walking speed. Those calibrated to fast optic flow estimated farther leaping and jumping abilities than those calibrated to moderate optic flow. Findings suggest that recalibration between optic flow and walking speed may specify an action boundary when calibrated or scaled to actions such as leaping, and possibly, the manipulation of optic flow speed has resulted in a change in the associated anticipated effort for walking a prescribed distance, which in turn influence one's perceived action capabilities for jumping and leaping.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358219/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139576401","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Linguistic features of spontaneous speech predict conversational recall. 自发言语的语言特点可预测会话回忆。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02440-w
Evgeniia Diachek, Sarah Brown-Schmidt

Empirical studies of conversational recall show that the amount of conversation that can be recalled after a delay is limited and biased in favor of one's own contributions. What aspects of a conversational interaction shape what will and will not be recalled? This study aims to predict the contents of conversation that will be recalled based on linguistic features of what was said. Across 59 conversational dyads, we observed that two linguistic features that are hallmarks of interactive language use-disfluency (um/uh) and backchannelling (ok, yeah)-promoted recall. Two other features-disagreements between the interlocutors and use of "like"-were not predictive of recall. While self-generated material was better remembered overall, both hearing and producing disfluency and backchannels improved memory for the associated utterances. Finally, the disfluency-related memory boost was similar regardless of the number of disfluencies in the utterance. Overall, we conclude that interactional linguistic features of conversation are predictive of what is and is not recalled following conversation.

对对话回忆的实证研究表明,延迟后能回忆起的对话内容是有限的,而且偏向于自己的贡献。对话互动的哪些方面决定了哪些内容会被回忆起,哪些不会被回忆起?本研究旨在根据谈话内容的语言特点来预测能被回忆起的谈话内容。在 59 个对话二人组中,我们观察到互动语言使用的两个语言特点--不流利(嗯/啊)和反向引导(好的、耶)--促进了回忆。另外两个特征--对话者之间的分歧和 "like "的使用--并不能预测回忆能力。虽然自编材料总体上记忆较好,但听到和产生不流畅和反向渠道都能改善对相关语篇的记忆。最后,无论语篇中有多少不连贯语,与不连贯语相关的记忆提升效果都是相似的。总之,我们得出的结论是,会话中的互动语言特点可以预测会话后能回忆起什么和不能回忆起什么。
{"title":"Linguistic features of spontaneous speech predict conversational recall.","authors":"Evgeniia Diachek, Sarah Brown-Schmidt","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02440-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02440-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Empirical studies of conversational recall show that the amount of conversation that can be recalled after a delay is limited and biased in favor of one's own contributions. What aspects of a conversational interaction shape what will and will not be recalled? This study aims to predict the contents of conversation that will be recalled based on linguistic features of what was said. Across 59 conversational dyads, we observed that two linguistic features that are hallmarks of interactive language use-disfluency (um/uh) and backchannelling (ok, yeah)-promoted recall. Two other features-disagreements between the interlocutors and use of \"like\"-were not predictive of recall. While self-generated material was better remembered overall, both hearing and producing disfluency and backchannels improved memory for the associated utterances. Finally, the disfluency-related memory boost was similar regardless of the number of disfluencies in the utterance. Overall, we conclude that interactional linguistic features of conversation are predictive of what is and is not recalled following conversation.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139432896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Amplitude envelope onset characteristics modulate phase locking for speech auditory-motor synchronization. 振幅包络起始特征调节语音听觉与运动同步的相位锁定。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02446-4
Min Zhu, Fei Chen, Chenxin Shi, Yang Zhang

The spontaneous speech-to-speech synchronization (SSS) test has been shown to be an effective behavioral method to estimate cortical speech auditory-motor coupling strength through phase-locking value (PLV) between auditory input and motor output. This study further investigated how amplitude envelope onset variations of the auditory speech signal may influence the speech auditory-motor synchronization. Sixty Mandarin-speaking adults listened to a stream of randomly presented syllables at an increasing speed while concurrently whispering in synchrony with the rhythm of the auditory stimuli whose onset consistency was manipulated, consisting of aspirated, unaspirated, and mixed conditions. The participants' PLVs for the three conditions in the SSS test were derived and compared. Results showed that syllable rise time affected the speech auditory-motor synchronization in a bifurcated fashion. Specifically, PLVs were significantly higher in the temporally more consistent conditions (aspirated or unaspirated) than those in the less consistent condition (mixed) for high synchronizers. In contrast, low synchronizers tended to be immune to the onset consistency. Overall, these results validated how syllable onset consistency in the rise time of amplitude envelope may modulate the strength of speech auditory-motor coupling. This study supports the application of the SSS test to examine individual differences in the integration of perception and production systems, which has implications for those with speech and language disorders that have difficulty with processing speech onset characteristics such as rise time.

自发言语同步(SSS)测试是通过听觉输入和运动输出之间的锁相值(PLV)来估计大脑皮层言语听觉-运动耦合强度的有效行为方法。本研究进一步探讨了语音听觉信号的振幅包络起始变化如何影响语音听觉-运动同步。六十名说普通话的成年人一边听着随机出现的音节流,速度不断加快,一边与听觉刺激的节奏同步耳语,听觉刺激的起始一致性受到了控制,包括吸气、不吸气和混合条件。得出并比较了 SSS 测试中三种条件下参与者的 PLV。结果表明,音节上升时间对语音听觉-运动同步的影响是分叉式的。具体来说,对于高同步者来说,在时间一致性较高的条件下(吸气或不吸气),PLV 明显高于一致性较低的条件下(混合)。相比之下,同步性低的人往往不受起始一致性的影响。总之,这些结果验证了振幅包络上升时间的音节起始一致性如何调节语音听觉-运动耦合的强度。这项研究支持应用 SSS 测试来检查感知和生产系统整合方面的个体差异,这对那些在处理语音起始特征(如上升时间)方面有困难的语音和语言障碍患者具有重要意义。
{"title":"Amplitude envelope onset characteristics modulate phase locking for speech auditory-motor synchronization.","authors":"Min Zhu, Fei Chen, Chenxin Shi, Yang Zhang","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02446-4","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02446-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The spontaneous speech-to-speech synchronization (SSS) test has been shown to be an effective behavioral method to estimate cortical speech auditory-motor coupling strength through phase-locking value (PLV) between auditory input and motor output. This study further investigated how amplitude envelope onset variations of the auditory speech signal may influence the speech auditory-motor synchronization. Sixty Mandarin-speaking adults listened to a stream of randomly presented syllables at an increasing speed while concurrently whispering in synchrony with the rhythm of the auditory stimuli whose onset consistency was manipulated, consisting of aspirated, unaspirated, and mixed conditions. The participants' PLVs for the three conditions in the SSS test were derived and compared. Results showed that syllable rise time affected the speech auditory-motor synchronization in a bifurcated fashion. Specifically, PLVs were significantly higher in the temporally more consistent conditions (aspirated or unaspirated) than those in the less consistent condition (mixed) for high synchronizers. In contrast, low synchronizers tended to be immune to the onset consistency. Overall, these results validated how syllable onset consistency in the rise time of amplitude envelope may modulate the strength of speech auditory-motor coupling. This study supports the application of the SSS test to examine individual differences in the integration of perception and production systems, which has implications for those with speech and language disorders that have difficulty with processing speech onset characteristics such as rise time.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139472181","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Patterns of saliency and semantic features distinguish gaze of expert and novice viewers of surveillance footage. 突出显示和语义特征模式区分了监控录像专家和新手观众的注视。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02454-y
Yujia Peng, Joseph M Burling, Greta K Todorova, Catherine Neary, Frank E Pollick, Hongjing Lu

When viewing the actions of others, we not only see patterns of body movements, but we also "see" the intentions and social relations of people. Experienced forensic examiners - Closed Circuit Television (CCTV) operators - have been shown to convey superior performance in identifying and predicting hostile intentions from surveillance footage than novices. However, it remains largely unknown what visual content CCTV operators actively attend to, and whether CCTV operators develop different strategies for active information seeking from what novices do. Here, we conducted computational analysis for the gaze-centered stimuli captured by experienced CCTV operators and novices' eye movements when viewing the same surveillance footage. Low-level image features were extracted by a visual saliency model, whereas object-level semantic features were extracted by a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN), AlexNet, from gaze-centered regions. We found that the looking behavior of CCTV operators differs from novices by actively attending to visual contents with different patterns of saliency and semantic features. Expertise in selectively utilizing informative features at different levels of visual hierarchy may play an important role in facilitating the efficient detection of social relationships between agents and the prediction of harmful intentions.

在观察他人行为时,我们不仅能看到肢体动作的模式,还能 "看到 "人们的意图和社会关系。经验丰富的法医检查员--闭路电视(CCTV)操作员--在从监控录像中识别和预测敌意方面的表现优于新手。然而,CCTV 操作员会主动关注哪些视觉内容,以及 CCTV 操作员在主动寻找信息时是否会制定与新手不同的策略,这些在很大程度上仍是未知数。在此,我们对有经验的 CCTV 操作员和新手在观看相同监控录像时捕捉到的以视线为中心的刺激进行了计算分析。低级图像特征由视觉显著性模型提取,而对象级语义特征则由深度卷积神经网络(DCNN)AlexNet 从注视中心区域提取。我们发现,中央电视台操作员的注视行为与新手不同,他们会积极关注具有不同显著性和语义特征模式的视觉内容。有选择性地利用视觉层次结构中不同层次的信息特征的专业技能,可能会在促进有效检测人员之间的社会关系和预测有害意图方面发挥重要作用。
{"title":"Patterns of saliency and semantic features distinguish gaze of expert and novice viewers of surveillance footage.","authors":"Yujia Peng, Joseph M Burling, Greta K Todorova, Catherine Neary, Frank E Pollick, Hongjing Lu","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02454-y","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-024-02454-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When viewing the actions of others, we not only see patterns of body movements, but we also \"see\" the intentions and social relations of people. Experienced forensic examiners - Closed Circuit Television (CCTV) operators - have been shown to convey superior performance in identifying and predicting hostile intentions from surveillance footage than novices. However, it remains largely unknown what visual content CCTV operators actively attend to, and whether CCTV operators develop different strategies for active information seeking from what novices do. Here, we conducted computational analysis for the gaze-centered stimuli captured by experienced CCTV operators and novices' eye movements when viewing the same surveillance footage. Low-level image features were extracted by a visual saliency model, whereas object-level semantic features were extracted by a deep convolutional neural network (DCNN), AlexNet, from gaze-centered regions. We found that the looking behavior of CCTV operators differs from novices by actively attending to visual contents with different patterns of saliency and semantic features. Expertise in selectively utilizing informative features at different levels of visual hierarchy may play an important role in facilitating the efficient detection of social relationships between agents and the prediction of harmful intentions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358171/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139564733","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How experts and novices judge other people's knowledgeability from language use. 专家和新手如何从语言使用中判断他人的知识水平。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-04 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02433-9
Alexander H Bower, Nicole Han, Ansh Soni, Miguel P Eckstein, Mark Steyvers

How accurate are people in judging someone else's knowledge based on their language use, and do more knowledgeable people use different cues to make these judgments? We address this by recruiting a group of participants ("informants") to answer general knowledge questions and describe various images belonging to different categories (e.g., cartoons, basketball). A second group of participants ("evaluators") also answer general knowledge questions and decide who is more knowledgeable within pairs of informants, based on these descriptions. Evaluators perform above chance at identifying the most knowledgeable informants (65% with only one description available). The less knowledgeable evaluators base their decisions on the number of specific statements, regardless of whether the statements are true or false. The more knowledgeable evaluators treat true and false statements differently and penalize the knowledge they attribute to informants who produce specific yet false statements. Our findings demonstrate the power of a few words when assessing others' knowledge and have implications for how misinformation is processed differently between experts and novices.

人们根据别人的语言使用情况来判断其知识水平的准确性有多高,知识更渊博的人是否会使用不同的线索来进行判断?为了解决这个问题,我们招募了一组参与者("信息提供者")回答常识问题,并描述属于不同类别的各种图像(如卡通、篮球)。第二组参与者("评估者")也要回答常识问题,并根据这些描述来决定谁更了解成对的信息提供者。评价者在识别知识最渊博的信息提供者方面的表现高于平均水平(在只有一种描述的情况下为 65%)。知识较少的评价者根据具体陈述的数量做出决定,而不管陈述是真是假。知识较丰富的评估者会区别对待真假陈述,并对提供具体但虚假陈述的信息提供者的知识进行惩罚。我们的研究结果表明,在评估他人的知识时,寥寥数语的力量是巨大的,并对专家和新手如何以不同方式处理错误信息产生了影响。
{"title":"How experts and novices judge other people's knowledgeability from language use.","authors":"Alexander H Bower, Nicole Han, Ansh Soni, Miguel P Eckstein, Mark Steyvers","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02433-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02433-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>How accurate are people in judging someone else's knowledge based on their language use, and do more knowledgeable people use different cues to make these judgments? We address this by recruiting a group of participants (\"informants\") to answer general knowledge questions and describe various images belonging to different categories (e.g., cartoons, basketball). A second group of participants (\"evaluators\") also answer general knowledge questions and decide who is more knowledgeable within pairs of informants, based on these descriptions. Evaluators perform above chance at identifying the most knowledgeable informants (65% with only one description available). The less knowledgeable evaluators base their decisions on the number of specific statements, regardless of whether the statements are true or false. The more knowledgeable evaluators treat true and false statements differently and penalize the knowledge they attribute to informants who produce specific yet false statements. Our findings demonstrate the power of a few words when assessing others' knowledge and have implications for how misinformation is processed differently between experts and novices.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11358192/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139098468","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in inattentional blindness. 注意力不集中盲症的个体差异。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-023-02431-x
Daniel J Simons, Connor M Hults, Yifan Ding

People often fail to notice unexpected objects and events when they are performing an attention-demanding task, a phenomenon known as inattentional blindness. We might expect individual differences in cognitive ability or personality to predict who will and will not notice unexpected objects given that people vary in their ability to perform attention-demanding tasks. We conducted a comprehensive literature search for empirical inattentional blindness reports and identified 38 records that included individual difference measures and met our inclusion criteria. From those, we extracted individual difference effect sizes for 31 records which included a total of 74 distinct, between-groups samples with at least one codable individual difference measure. We conducted separate meta-analyses of the relationship between noticing/missing an unexpected object and scores on each of the 14 cognitive and 19 personality measures in this dataset. We also aggregated across personality measures reflecting positive/negative affectivity or openness/absorption and cognitive measures of interference, attention breadth, and memory. Collectively, these meta-analyses provided little evidence that individual differences in ability or personality predict noticing of an unexpected object. A robustness analysis that excluded samples with extremely low numbers of people who noticed or missed produced similar results. For most measures, the number of samples and the total sample sizes were small, and larger studies are needed to examine individual differences in inattentional blindness more systematically. However, the results are consistent with the idea that noticing of unexpected objects or events differs from deliberate attentional control tasks in that it is not reliably predicted by individual differences in cognitive ability.

人们在执行一项需要集中注意力的任务时,常常会注意不到意想不到的物体和事件,这种现象被称为 "无意盲"。鉴于人们在执行需要集中注意力的任务时能力各不相同,我们可能会认为认知能力或性格方面的个体差异会预测谁会注意到意外物体,谁不会注意到意外物体。我们对注意力盲症的实证研究报告进行了全面的文献检索,发现有 38 条记录包含个体差异测量方法,并符合我们的纳入标准。从中,我们提取了 31 条记录的个体差异效应大小,这些记录共包括 74 个不同的组间样本,其中至少有一个可编码的个体差异测量值。我们对注意/遗漏意外物体与该数据集中 14 项认知测量和 19 项人格测量中每项测量的得分之间的关系进行了单独的元分析。我们还对反映积极/消极情绪或开放/吸收能力的人格测量指标以及干扰、注意力广度和记忆力等认知测量指标进行了汇总。总之,这些荟萃分析几乎没有提供证据表明,个体能力或个性差异可以预测对意外物体的注意。一项稳健性分析排除了注意到或错过的人数极少的样本,也得出了类似的结果。对于大多数测量指标而言,样本数量和样本总量都较小,因此需要进行更大规模的研究,以更系统地考察注意力盲区的个体差异。然而,研究结果与以下观点是一致的,即对意外物体或事件的注意不同于有意注意控制任务,它不能通过认知能力的个体差异进行可靠预测。
{"title":"Individual differences in inattentional blindness.","authors":"Daniel J Simons, Connor M Hults, Yifan Ding","doi":"10.3758/s13423-023-02431-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13423-023-02431-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often fail to notice unexpected objects and events when they are performing an attention-demanding task, a phenomenon known as inattentional blindness. We might expect individual differences in cognitive ability or personality to predict who will and will not notice unexpected objects given that people vary in their ability to perform attention-demanding tasks. We conducted a comprehensive literature search for empirical inattentional blindness reports and identified 38 records that included individual difference measures and met our inclusion criteria. From those, we extracted individual difference effect sizes for 31 records which included a total of 74 distinct, between-groups samples with at least one codable individual difference measure. We conducted separate meta-analyses of the relationship between noticing/missing an unexpected object and scores on each of the 14 cognitive and 19 personality measures in this dataset. We also aggregated across personality measures reflecting positive/negative affectivity or openness/absorption and cognitive measures of interference, attention breadth, and memory. Collectively, these meta-analyses provided little evidence that individual differences in ability or personality predict noticing of an unexpected object. A robustness analysis that excluded samples with extremely low numbers of people who noticed or missed produced similar results. For most measures, the number of samples and the total sample sizes were small, and larger studies are needed to examine individual differences in inattentional blindness more systematically. However, the results are consistent with the idea that noticing of unexpected objects or events differs from deliberate attentional control tasks in that it is not reliably predicted by individual differences in cognitive ability.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139106535","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1