首页 > 最新文献

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review最新文献

英文 中文
Taking time: Auditory statistical learning benefits from distributed exposure. 花时间:听觉统计学习受益于分散的接触。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02634-w
Jasper de Waard, Jan Theeuwes, Louisa Bogaerts

In an auditory statistical learning paradigm, listeners learn to partition a continuous stream of syllables by discovering the repeating syllable patterns that constitute the speech stream. Here, we ask whether auditory statistical learning benefits from spaced exposure compared with massed exposure. In a longitudinal online study on Prolific, we exposed 100 participants to the regularities in a spaced way (i.e., with exposure blocks spread out over 3 days) and another 100 in a massed way (i.e., with all exposure blocks lumped together on a single day). In the exposure phase, participants listened to streams composed of pairs while responding to a target syllable. The spaced and massed groups exhibited equal learning during exposure, as indicated by a comparable response-time advantage for predictable target syllables. However, in terms of resulting long-term knowledge, we observed a benefit from spaced exposure. Following a 2-week delay period, we tested participants' knowledge of the pairs in a forced-choice test. While both groups performed above chance, the spaced group had higher accuracy. Our findings speak to the importance of the timing of exposure to structured input and also for statistical learning outside of the laboratory (e.g., in language development), and imply that current investigations of auditory statistical learning likely underestimate human statistical learning abilities.

在听觉统计学习范式中,听者通过发现构成语音流的重复音节模式来学习分割连续的音节流。在这里,我们问听觉统计学习是否受益于间隔暴露与大量暴露。在一项关于多产的纵向在线研究中,我们让100名参与者以间隔的方式(即在3天内分散暴露块)接触这些规律,并让另外100名参与者以大量的方式(即在一天内将所有暴露块集中在一起)接触这些规律。在暴露阶段,参与者在对目标音节做出反应的同时听着由成对组成的音乐流。间隔组和密集组在暴露过程中表现出同等的学习能力,这表明在可预测的目标音节上有相当的反应时间优势。然而,就产生的长期知识而言,我们观察到间隔暴露的好处。在两周的延迟期之后,我们在一个强迫选择测试中测试了参与者对这些配对的知识。虽然两组的表现都高于随机,但间隔组的准确率更高。我们的研究结果说明了接触结构化输入的时间和实验室之外的统计学习(例如,在语言发展中)的重要性,并暗示当前对听觉统计学习的调查可能低估了人类的统计学习能力。
{"title":"Taking time: Auditory statistical learning benefits from distributed exposure.","authors":"Jasper de Waard, Jan Theeuwes, Louisa Bogaerts","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02634-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02634-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In an auditory statistical learning paradigm, listeners learn to partition a continuous stream of syllables by discovering the repeating syllable patterns that constitute the speech stream. Here, we ask whether auditory statistical learning benefits from spaced exposure compared with massed exposure. In a longitudinal online study on Prolific, we exposed 100 participants to the regularities in a spaced way (i.e., with exposure blocks spread out over 3 days) and another 100 in a massed way (i.e., with all exposure blocks lumped together on a single day). In the exposure phase, participants listened to streams composed of pairs while responding to a target syllable. The spaced and massed groups exhibited equal learning during exposure, as indicated by a comparable response-time advantage for predictable target syllables. However, in terms of resulting long-term knowledge, we observed a benefit from spaced exposure. Following a 2-week delay period, we tested participants' knowledge of the pairs in a forced-choice test. While both groups performed above chance, the spaced group had higher accuracy. Our findings speak to the importance of the timing of exposure to structured input and also for statistical learning outside of the laboratory (e.g., in language development), and imply that current investigations of auditory statistical learning likely underestimate human statistical learning abilities.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143010497","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of relative word-length on effects of non-adjacent word transpositions. 相对词长对非相邻词换位效果的影响。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02637-7
Yun Wen, Jonathan Grainger

A recent study (Wen et al., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 50: 934-941, 2024) found no influence of relative word-length on transposed-word effects. However, following the tradition of prior research on effects of transposed words during sentence reading, the transposed words in that study were adjacent words (words at positions 2 and 3 or 3 and 4 in five-word sequences). We surmised that the absence of an influence of relative word-length might be due to word identification being too precise when the two words are located close to eye-fixation location, hence cancelling the impact of more approximate indices of word identity such as word length. We therefore hypothesized that relative word-length might impact on transposed-word effects when the transposition involves non-adjacent words. The present study put this hypothesis to test and found that relative word-length does modify the size of transposed-word effects with non-adjacent transpositions. Transposed-word effects are greater when the transposed words have the same length. Furthermore, a cross-study analysis confirmed that transposed-word effects are greater for adjacent than for non-adjacent transpositions.

最近的一项研究(Wen et al., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 50: 934-941, 2024)发现相对单词长度对转置单词效应没有影响。然而,根据以往对换句词在句子阅读过程中的影响的研究传统,该研究中的换句词是相邻词(五词序列中位置2和3或3和4的词)。我们推测,相对词长没有影响可能是由于当两个词靠近眼睛注视的位置时,单词识别过于精确,从而抵消了更近似的单词身份指标(如单词长度)的影响。因此,我们假设当转置涉及非相邻单词时,相对单词长度可能会影响转置单词效应。本研究对这一假设进行了检验,发现相对词长确实会改变非相邻转置词的转置效应的大小。当调换后的单词长度相同时,调换后的单词效果更大。此外,一项交叉研究分析证实,相邻的转置词效应比非相邻的转置词更大。
{"title":"The impact of relative word-length on effects of non-adjacent word transpositions.","authors":"Yun Wen, Jonathan Grainger","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02637-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02637-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A recent study (Wen et al., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 50: 934-941, 2024) found no influence of relative word-length on transposed-word effects. However, following the tradition of prior research on effects of transposed words during sentence reading, the transposed words in that study were adjacent words (words at positions 2 and 3 or 3 and 4 in five-word sequences). We surmised that the absence of an influence of relative word-length might be due to word identification being too precise when the two words are located close to eye-fixation location, hence cancelling the impact of more approximate indices of word identity such as word length. We therefore hypothesized that relative word-length might impact on transposed-word effects when the transposition involves non-adjacent words. The present study put this hypothesis to test and found that relative word-length does modify the size of transposed-word effects with non-adjacent transpositions. Transposed-word effects are greater when the transposed words have the same length. Furthermore, a cross-study analysis confirmed that transposed-word effects are greater for adjacent than for non-adjacent transpositions.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143010500","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Age-related differences in information, but not task control in the color-word Stroop task. 与年龄相关的信息差异,但与颜色词Stroop任务的任务控制无关。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02631-z
Eldad Keha, Daniela Aisenberg-Shafran, Shachar Hochman, Eyal Kalanthroff

Older adults were found to struggle with tasks that require cognitive control. One task that measures the ability to exert cognitive control is the color-word Stroop task. Almost all studies that tested cognitive control in older adults using the Stroop task have focused on one type of control - Information control. In the present work, we ask whether older adults also show a deficit in another type of cognitive control - Task control. To that end, we tested older and younger adults by isolating and measuring two types of conflict - information conflict and task conflict. Information conflict was measured by the difference between color identification of incongruent color words and color identification of neutral words, while task conflict was measured by the difference between color identification of neutral words and color identification of neutral symbols and by the reverse facilitation effect. We tested how the behavioral markers of these two types of conflicts are affected under low task control conditions, which is essential for measuring task conflict behaviorally. Older adults demonstrated a deficit in information control by showing a larger information conflict marker, but not in task control markers, as no differences in task conflict were found between younger and older adults. These findings supported previous studies that work against theories that link the larger Stroop interference in older adults to a generic slowdown or a generic inhibitory failure. We discussed the relevancy of the results and future research directions in line with other Stroop studies that tested age-related differences in different control mechanisms.

老年人被发现在需要认知控制的任务中挣扎。一项测量认知控制能力的任务是颜色单词Stroop任务。几乎所有使用Stroop任务测试老年人认知控制的研究都集中在一种控制上——信息控制。在目前的工作中,我们询问老年人是否也表现出另一种认知控制-任务控制的缺陷。为此,我们通过分离和测量两种冲突——信息冲突和任务冲突——来测试老年人和年轻人。信息冲突通过不一致颜色词的颜色识别与中性词的颜色识别的差异来衡量,任务冲突通过中性词的颜色识别与中性符号的颜色识别的差异和反向便利化效应来衡量。我们测试了低任务控制条件下这两种冲突的行为标记是如何受到影响的,这对测量任务冲突行为至关重要。老年人通过显示更大的信息冲突标记来显示信息控制缺陷,但在任务控制标记上没有发现差异,因为年轻人和老年人在任务冲突上没有发现差异。这些发现支持了先前的研究,这些研究反对将老年人较大的Stroop干扰与一般减速或一般抑制失败联系起来的理论。我们讨论了结果的相关性和未来的研究方向,与其他Stroop研究一致,测试了不同控制机制的年龄相关差异。
{"title":"Age-related differences in information, but not task control in the color-word Stroop task.","authors":"Eldad Keha, Daniela Aisenberg-Shafran, Shachar Hochman, Eyal Kalanthroff","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02631-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02631-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Older adults were found to struggle with tasks that require cognitive control. One task that measures the ability to exert cognitive control is the color-word Stroop task. Almost all studies that tested cognitive control in older adults using the Stroop task have focused on one type of control - Information control. In the present work, we ask whether older adults also show a deficit in another type of cognitive control - Task control. To that end, we tested older and younger adults by isolating and measuring two types of conflict - information conflict and task conflict. Information conflict was measured by the difference between color identification of incongruent color words and color identification of neutral words, while task conflict was measured by the difference between color identification of neutral words and color identification of neutral symbols and by the reverse facilitation effect. We tested how the behavioral markers of these two types of conflicts are affected under low task control conditions, which is essential for measuring task conflict behaviorally. Older adults demonstrated a deficit in information control by showing a larger information conflict marker, but not in task control markers, as no differences in task conflict were found between younger and older adults. These findings supported previous studies that work against theories that link the larger Stroop interference in older adults to a generic slowdown or a generic inhibitory failure. We discussed the relevancy of the results and future research directions in line with other Stroop studies that tested age-related differences in different control mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143010493","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Distinct detection and discrimination sensitivities in visual processing of real versus unreal optic flow. 真假光流视觉处理中不同的检测和分辨灵敏度。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-14 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02616-y
Li Li, Xuechun Shen, Shuguang Kuai

We examined the intricate mechanisms underlying visual processing of complex motion stimuli by measuring the detection sensitivity to contraction and expansion patterns and the discrimination sensitivity to the location of the center of motion (CoM) in various real and unreal optic flow stimuli. We conducted two experiments (N = 20 each) and compared responses to both "real" optic flow stimuli containing information about self-movement in a three-dimensional scene and "unreal" optic flow stimuli lacking such information. We found that detection sensitivity to contraction surpassed that to expansion patterns for unreal optic flow stimuli, whereas this trend was reversed for real optic flow stimuli. Furthermore, while discrimination sensitivity to the CoM location was not affected by stimulus duration for unreal optic flow stimuli, it showed a significant improvement when stimulus duration increased from 100 to 400 ms for real optic flow stimuli. These findings provide compelling evidence that the visual system employs distinct processing approaches for real versus unreal optic flow even when they are perfectly matched for two-dimensional global features and local motion signals. These differences reveal influences of self-movement in natural environments, enabling the visual system to uniquely process stimuli with significant survival implications.

通过测量不同真实和虚幻光流刺激对收缩和膨胀模式的检测灵敏度以及对运动中心位置的识别灵敏度,研究了复杂运动刺激视觉加工的复杂机制。我们进行了两个实验(每个实验N = 20),并比较了包含三维场景中自我运动信息的“真实”光流刺激和缺乏此类信息的“虚幻”光流刺激的反应。我们发现,对于不真实的光流刺激,对收缩模式的检测灵敏度超过了对膨胀模式的检测灵敏度,而对于真实的光流刺激,这种趋势是相反的。此外,对于非真实光流刺激,当刺激时间从100 ms增加到400 ms时,对CoM位置的识别灵敏度不受刺激时间的影响,但在真实光流刺激下,当刺激时间从100 ms增加到400 ms时,识别灵敏度显著提高。这些发现提供了令人信服的证据,表明视觉系统对真实和不真实的光流采用不同的处理方法,即使它们对二维全局特征和局部运动信号完全匹配。这些差异揭示了自然环境中自我运动的影响,使视觉系统能够独特地处理具有重要生存意义的刺激。
{"title":"Distinct detection and discrimination sensitivities in visual processing of real versus unreal optic flow.","authors":"Li Li, Xuechun Shen, Shuguang Kuai","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02616-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02616-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined the intricate mechanisms underlying visual processing of complex motion stimuli by measuring the detection sensitivity to contraction and expansion patterns and the discrimination sensitivity to the location of the center of motion (CoM) in various real and unreal optic flow stimuli. We conducted two experiments (N = 20 each) and compared responses to both \"real\" optic flow stimuli containing information about self-movement in a three-dimensional scene and \"unreal\" optic flow stimuli lacking such information. We found that detection sensitivity to contraction surpassed that to expansion patterns for unreal optic flow stimuli, whereas this trend was reversed for real optic flow stimuli. Furthermore, while discrimination sensitivity to the CoM location was not affected by stimulus duration for unreal optic flow stimuli, it showed a significant improvement when stimulus duration increased from 100 to 400 ms for real optic flow stimuli. These findings provide compelling evidence that the visual system employs distinct processing approaches for real versus unreal optic flow even when they are perfectly matched for two-dimensional global features and local motion signals. These differences reveal influences of self-movement in natural environments, enabling the visual system to uniquely process stimuli with significant survival implications.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142984613","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The cost of perspective switching: Constraints on simultaneous activation. 透视图切换的代价:同时激活的约束。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02633-x
Dorit Segal

Visual perspective taking often involves transitioning between perspectives, yet the cognitive mechanisms underlying this process remain unclear. The current study draws on insights from task- and language-switching research to address this gap. In Experiment 1, 79 participants judged the perspective of an avatar positioned in various locations, observing either the rectangular or the square side of a rectangular cube hanging from the ceiling. The avatar's perspective was either consistent or inconsistent with the participant's, and its computation sometimes required mental transformation. The task included both single-position blocks, in which the avatar's location remained fixed across all trials, and mixed-position blocks, in which the avatar's position changed across trials. Performance was compared across trial types and positions. In Experiment 2, 126 participants completed a similar task administered online, with more trials, and performance was compared at various points within the response time distribution (vincentile analysis). Results revealed a robust switching cost. However, mixing costs, which reflect the ability to maintain multiple task sets active in working memory, were absent, even in slower response times. Additionally, responses to the avatar's position varied as a function of consistency with the participants' viewpoint and the angular disparity between them. These findings suggest that perspective switching is costly, people cannot activate multiple perspectives simultaneously, and the computation of other people's visual perspectives varies with cognitive demands.

视觉透视通常涉及视角之间的转换,但这一过程背后的认知机制尚不清楚。目前的研究借鉴了任务转换和语言转换研究的见解来解决这一差距。在实验1中,79名参与者通过观察悬挂在天花板上的矩形立方体的矩形面或方形面来判断不同位置的虚拟形象的视角。虚拟角色的视角要么与参与者的一致,要么不一致,它的计算有时需要心理转换。该任务既包括单一位置块,即角色的位置在所有试验中保持固定,也包括混合位置块,即角色的位置在试验中不断变化。在不同的试验类型和岗位上进行了性能比较。在实验2中,126名参与者完成了一个在线管理的类似任务,有更多的试验,并在反应时间分布的不同点上比较表现(诱因分析)。结果显示了稳健的转换成本。然而,即使在较慢的反应时间中,反映在工作记忆中保持多个任务集活跃的能力的混合成本也不存在。此外,对虚拟角色位置的反应会随着参与者观点的一致性和他们之间角度的差异而变化。这些发现表明,视角转换是昂贵的,人们不可能同时激活多个视角,并且对他人视觉视角的计算随认知需求而变化。
{"title":"The cost of perspective switching: Constraints on simultaneous activation.","authors":"Dorit Segal","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02633-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02633-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual perspective taking often involves transitioning between perspectives, yet the cognitive mechanisms underlying this process remain unclear. The current study draws on insights from task- and language-switching research to address this gap. In Experiment 1, 79 participants judged the perspective of an avatar positioned in various locations, observing either the rectangular or the square side of a rectangular cube hanging from the ceiling. The avatar's perspective was either consistent or inconsistent with the participant's, and its computation sometimes required mental transformation. The task included both single-position blocks, in which the avatar's location remained fixed across all trials, and mixed-position blocks, in which the avatar's position changed across trials. Performance was compared across trial types and positions. In Experiment 2, 126 participants completed a similar task administered online, with more trials, and performance was compared at various points within the response time distribution (vincentile analysis). Results revealed a robust switching cost. However, mixing costs, which reflect the ability to maintain multiple task sets active in working memory, were absent, even in slower response times. Additionally, responses to the avatar's position varied as a function of consistency with the participants' viewpoint and the angular disparity between them. These findings suggest that perspective switching is costly, people cannot activate multiple perspectives simultaneously, and the computation of other people's visual perspectives varies with cognitive demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142979863","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Do we feel colours? A systematic review of 128 years of psychological research linking colours and emotions. 我们能感觉到颜色吗?这是对128年来将颜色和情绪联系起来的心理学研究的系统回顾。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02615-z
Domicele Jonauskaite, Christine Mohr

Colour is an integral part of natural and constructed environments. For many, it also has an aesthetic appeal, with some colours being more pleasant than others. Moreover, humans seem to systematically and reliably associate colours with emotions, such as yellow with joy, black with sadness, light colours with positive and dark colours with negative emotions. To systematise such colour-emotion correspondences, we identified 132 relevant peer-reviewed articles published in English between 1895 and 2022. These articles covered a total of 42,266 participants from 64 different countries. We found that all basic colour categories had systematic correspondences with affective dimensions (valence, arousal, power) as well as with discrete affective terms (e.g., love, happy, sad, bored). Most correspondences were many-to-many, with systematic effects driven by lightness, saturation, and hue ('colour temperature'). More specifically, (i) LIGHT and DARK colours were associated with positive and negative emotions, respectively; (ii) RED with empowering, high arousal positive and negative emotions; (iii) YELLOW and ORANGE with positive, high arousal emotions; (iv) BLUE, GREEN, GREEN-BLUE, and WHITE with positive, low arousal emotions; (v) PINK with positive emotions; (vi) PURPLE with empowering emotions; (vii) GREY with negative, low arousal emotions; and (viii) BLACK with negative, high arousal emotions. Shared communication needs might explain these consistencies across studies, making colour an excellent medium for communication of emotion. As most colour-emotion correspondences were tested on an abstract level (i.e., associations), it remains to be seen whether such correspondences translate to the impact of colour on experienced emotions and specific contexts.

色彩是自然和建筑环境的一个组成部分。对许多人来说,它还具有审美吸引力,有些颜色比其他颜色更令人愉快。此外,人类似乎系统而可靠地将颜色与情绪联系起来,比如黄色代表快乐,黑色代表悲伤,浅色代表积极,深色代表消极情绪。为了系统化这种颜色-情感的对应关系,我们确定了1895年至2022年间发表的132篇相关的英文同行评议文章。这些文章涵盖了来自64个不同国家的42,266名参与者。我们发现,所有的基本颜色类别都与情感维度(效价、唤起、力量)以及离散的情感术语(如爱、快乐、悲伤、无聊)有系统的对应关系。大多数通信是多对多的,具有由亮度、饱和度和色调(“色温”)驱动的系统效果。更具体地说,(1)浅色和深色分别与积极和消极情绪有关;(ii)具有授权、高唤醒的积极和消极情绪的RED;(iii)黄色和橙色代表积极、高唤醒情绪;(iv)蓝色、绿色、绿蓝色和白色具有积极、低唤醒情绪;(v)有积极情绪的PINK;(vi)充满激情的紫色;(vii)灰色,消极、低唤醒情绪;(viii)具有消极、高唤醒情绪的黑人。共同的交流需求可以解释这些研究中的一致性,使颜色成为情感交流的绝佳媒介。由于大多数颜色与情绪的对应关系都是在抽象层面上进行的测试(即联想),因此这种对应关系是否转化为颜色对体验情绪和特定情境的影响还有待观察。
{"title":"Do we feel colours? A systematic review of 128 years of psychological research linking colours and emotions.","authors":"Domicele Jonauskaite, Christine Mohr","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02615-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02615-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Colour is an integral part of natural and constructed environments. For many, it also has an aesthetic appeal, with some colours being more pleasant than others. Moreover, humans seem to systematically and reliably associate colours with emotions, such as yellow with joy, black with sadness, light colours with positive and dark colours with negative emotions. To systematise such colour-emotion correspondences, we identified 132 relevant peer-reviewed articles published in English between 1895 and 2022. These articles covered a total of 42,266 participants from 64 different countries. We found that all basic colour categories had systematic correspondences with affective dimensions (valence, arousal, power) as well as with discrete affective terms (e.g., love, happy, sad, bored). Most correspondences were many-to-many, with systematic effects driven by lightness, saturation, and hue ('colour temperature'). More specifically, (i) LIGHT and DARK colours were associated with positive and negative emotions, respectively; (ii) RED with empowering, high arousal positive and negative emotions; (iii) YELLOW and ORANGE with positive, high arousal emotions; (iv) BLUE, GREEN, GREEN-BLUE, and WHITE with positive, low arousal emotions; (v) PINK with positive emotions; (vi) PURPLE with empowering emotions; (vii) GREY with negative, low arousal emotions; and (viii) BLACK with negative, high arousal emotions. Shared communication needs might explain these consistencies across studies, making colour an excellent medium for communication of emotion. As most colour-emotion correspondences were tested on an abstract level (i.e., associations), it remains to be seen whether such correspondences translate to the impact of colour on experienced emotions and specific contexts.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142979847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Increased attention towards progress information near a goal state. 增加对接近目标状态的进度信息的关注。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-13 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02636-8
Sean Devine, Y Doug Dong, Martin Sellier Silva, Mathieu Roy, A Ross Otto

A growing body of evidence across psychology suggests that (cognitive) effort exertion increases in proximity to a goal state. For instance, previous work has shown that participants respond more quickly, but not less accurately, when they near a goal-as indicated by a filling progress bar. Yet it remains unclear when over the course of a cognitively demanding task do people monitor progress information: Do they continuously monitor their goal progress over the course of a task, or attend more frequently to it as they near their goal? To answer this question, we used eye-tracking to examine trial-by-trial changes in progress monitoring as participants completed blocks of an attentionally demanding oddball task. Replicating past work, we found that participants increased cognitive effort exertion near a goal, as evinced by an increase in correct responses per second. More interestingly, we found that the rate at which participants attended to goal progress information-operationalized here as the frequency of gazes towards a progress bar-increased steeply near a goal state. In other words, participants extracted information from the progress bar at a higher rate when goals were proximal (versus distal). In exploratory analysis of tonic pupil diameter, we also found that tonic pupil size increased sharply as participants approached a goal state, mirroring the pattern of gaze. These results support the view that people attend to progress information more as they approach a goal.

心理学领域越来越多的证据表明,(认知)努力在接近目标状态时增加。例如,先前的研究表明,当参与者接近一个目标时,他们的反应会更快,但并不会更准确——正如进度条所显示的那样。然而,在认知要求较高的任务过程中,人们在什么时候监控进度信息仍不清楚:他们是在任务过程中持续监控目标进展,还是在接近目标时更频繁地关注目标进展?为了回答这个问题,我们使用眼动追踪来检查参与者在完成一项要求注意力集中的古怪任务时,进度监测的一次又一次变化。重复过去的研究,我们发现参与者在接近目标时增加了认知努力,这可以通过每秒正确反应的增加来证明。更有趣的是,我们发现,在接近目标状态时,参与者关注目标进度信息的频率(这里指的是注视进度条的频率)急剧增加。换句话说,当目标在近端(而不是远端)时,参与者从进度条中提取信息的速度更快。在对强直瞳孔直径的探索性分析中,我们还发现,当参与者接近目标状态时,强直瞳孔的大小急剧增加,这反映了凝视的模式。这些结果支持了人们在接近目标时更关注进度信息的观点。
{"title":"Increased attention towards progress information near a goal state.","authors":"Sean Devine, Y Doug Dong, Martin Sellier Silva, Mathieu Roy, A Ross Otto","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02636-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02636-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A growing body of evidence across psychology suggests that (cognitive) effort exertion increases in proximity to a goal state. For instance, previous work has shown that participants respond more quickly, but not less accurately, when they near a goal-as indicated by a filling progress bar. Yet it remains unclear when over the course of a cognitively demanding task do people monitor progress information: Do they continuously monitor their goal progress over the course of a task, or attend more frequently to it as they near their goal? To answer this question, we used eye-tracking to examine trial-by-trial changes in progress monitoring as participants completed blocks of an attentionally demanding oddball task. Replicating past work, we found that participants increased cognitive effort exertion near a goal, as evinced by an increase in correct responses per second. More interestingly, we found that the rate at which participants attended to goal progress information-operationalized here as the frequency of gazes towards a progress bar-increased steeply near a goal state. In other words, participants extracted information from the progress bar at a higher rate when goals were proximal (versus distal). In exploratory analysis of tonic pupil diameter, we also found that tonic pupil size increased sharply as participants approached a goal state, mirroring the pattern of gaze. These results support the view that people attend to progress information more as they approach a goal.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142979855","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Product, not process: Metacognitive monitoring of visual performance during sustained attention. 产品,而非过程:持续注意力期间视觉表现的元认知监测。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9
Cheongil Kim, Sang Chul Chong

The performance of the human visual system exhibits moment-to-moment fluctuations influenced by multiple neurocognitive factors. To deal with this instability of the visual system, introspective awareness of current visual performance (metacognitive monitoring) may be crucial. In this study, we investigate whether and how people can monitor their own visual performance during sustained attention by adopting confidence judgments as indicators of metacognitive monitoring - assuming that if participants can monitor visual performance, confidence judgments will accurately track performance fluctuations. In two experiments (N = 40), we found that participants were able to monitor fluctuations in visual performance during sustained attention. Importantly, metacognitive monitoring largely relied on the quality of target perception, a product of visual processing ("I lack confidence in my performance because I only caught a glimpse of the target"), rather than the states of the visual system during visual processing ("I lack confidence because I was not focusing on the task").

人类视觉系统的表现表现出受多种神经认知因素影响的时刻波动。为了处理这种视觉系统的不稳定性,对当前视觉表现的内省意识(元认知监测)可能是至关重要的。在本研究中,我们通过采用信心判断作为元认知监测的指标来研究人们是否以及如何在持续注意过程中监测自己的视觉表现——假设如果参与者能够监测视觉表现,信心判断将准确地跟踪表现波动。在两个实验(N = 40)中,我们发现参与者能够监测持续注意力期间视觉表现的波动。重要的是,元认知监控很大程度上依赖于目标感知的质量,这是视觉处理的产物(“我对自己的表现缺乏信心,因为我只瞥见了目标”),而不是视觉系统在视觉处理过程中的状态(“我缺乏信心,因为我没有专注于任务”)。
{"title":"Product, not process: Metacognitive monitoring of visual performance during sustained attention.","authors":"Cheongil Kim, Sang Chul Chong","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02635-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The performance of the human visual system exhibits moment-to-moment fluctuations influenced by multiple neurocognitive factors. To deal with this instability of the visual system, introspective awareness of current visual performance (metacognitive monitoring) may be crucial. In this study, we investigate whether and how people can monitor their own visual performance during sustained attention by adopting confidence judgments as indicators of metacognitive monitoring - assuming that if participants can monitor visual performance, confidence judgments will accurately track performance fluctuations. In two experiments (N <math><mo>=</mo></math> 40), we found that participants were able to monitor fluctuations in visual performance during sustained attention. Importantly, metacognitive monitoring largely relied on the quality of target perception, a product of visual processing (\"I lack confidence in my performance because I only caught a glimpse of the target\"), rather than the states of the visual system during visual processing (\"I lack confidence because I was not focusing on the task\").</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953964","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cracking arbitrariness: A data-driven study of auditory iconicity in spoken English. 破解随意性:英语口语听觉象似性的数据驱动研究。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-08 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0
Andrea Gregor de Varda, Marco Marelli

Auditory iconic words display a phonological profile that imitates their referents' sounds. Traditionally, those words are thought to constitute a minor portion of the auditory lexicon. In this article, we challenge this assumption by assessing the pervasiveness of onomatopoeia in the English auditory vocabulary through a novel data-driven procedure. We embed spoken words and natural sounds into a shared auditory space through (a) a short-time Fourier transform, (b) a convolutional neural network trained to classify sounds, and (c) a network trained on speech recognition. Then, we employ the obtained vector representations to measure their objective auditory resemblance. These similarity indexes show that imitation is not limited to some circumscribed semantic categories, but instead can be considered as a widespread mechanism underlying the structure of the English auditory vocabulary. We finally empirically validate our similarity indexes as measures of iconicity against human judgments.

听觉上的标志性词汇显示出一种模仿其所指的声音的语音特征。传统上,这些词被认为是听觉词汇的一小部分。在本文中,我们通过一种新颖的数据驱动程序来评估拟声词在英语听觉词汇中的普遍性,从而挑战这一假设。我们通过(a)短时傅立叶变换,(b)训练用于声音分类的卷积神经网络,以及(c)训练用于语音识别的网络,将口语单词和自然声音嵌入到共享的听觉空间中。然后,我们使用得到的向量表示来度量它们的客观听觉相似性。这些相似性指标表明,模仿并不局限于某些限定的语义范畴,而是英语听觉词汇结构背后的一种普遍机制。最后,我们通过经验验证了我们的相似性指数作为对人类判断的象似性度量。
{"title":"Cracking arbitrariness: A data-driven study of auditory iconicity in spoken English.","authors":"Andrea Gregor de Varda, Marco Marelli","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02630-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Auditory iconic words display a phonological profile that imitates their referents' sounds. Traditionally, those words are thought to constitute a minor portion of the auditory lexicon. In this article, we challenge this assumption by assessing the pervasiveness of onomatopoeia in the English auditory vocabulary through a novel data-driven procedure. We embed spoken words and natural sounds into a shared auditory space through (a) a short-time Fourier transform, (b) a convolutional neural network trained to classify sounds, and (c) a network trained on speech recognition. Then, we employ the obtained vector representations to measure their objective auditory resemblance. These similarity indexes show that imitation is not limited to some circumscribed semantic categories, but instead can be considered as a widespread mechanism underlying the structure of the English auditory vocabulary. We finally empirically validate our similarity indexes as measures of iconicity against human judgments.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953962","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual attention matters during word recognition: A Bayesian modeling approach. 视觉注意力在单词识别中的作用:贝叶斯建模方法。
IF 3.2 3区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-07 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4
Thierry Phénix, Émilie Ginestet, Sylviane Valdois, Julien Diard

It is striking that visual attention, the process by which attentional resources are allocated in the visual field so as to locally enhance visual perception, is a pervasive component of models of eye movements in reading, but is seldom considered in models of isolated word recognition. We describe BRAID, a new Bayesian word-Recognition model with Attention, Interference and Dynamics. As most of its predecessors, BRAID incorporates three sensory, perceptual, and orthographic knowledge layers together with a lexical membership submodel. Its originality resides in also including three mechanisms that modulate letter identification within strings: an acuity gradient, lateral interference, and visual attention. We calibrated the model such that its temporal scale was consistent with behavioral data, and then explored the model's capacity to generalize to other, independent effects. We evaluated the model's capacity to account for the word length effect in lexical decision, for the optimal viewing position effect, and for the interaction of crowding and frequency effects in word recognition. We further examined how these effects were modulated by variations in the visual attention distribution. We show that visual attention modulates all three effects and that a narrow distribution of visual attention results in performance patterns that mimic those reported in impaired readers. Overall, the BRAID model could be conceived as a core building block, towards the development of integrated models of reading aloud and eye movement control, or of visual recognition of impaired readers, or any context in which visual attention does matter.

视觉注意是将注意力资源分配到视野中以局部增强视觉感知的过程,它是阅读时眼动模型中普遍存在的组成部分,但在孤立词识别模型中却很少被考虑。本文描述了一种新的贝叶斯词识别模型BRAID,该模型具有注意、干扰和动态特征。与大多数前辈一样,BRAID结合了三个感觉、知觉和正字法知识层,以及一个词法隶属子模型。它的独创性还在于包括三种机制来调节字符串中的字母识别:敏锐度梯度,横向干扰和视觉注意力。我们校准了模型,使其时间尺度与行为数据一致,然后探索模型推广到其他独立效应的能力。我们评估了该模型在词汇决策中的单词长度效应、最佳观看位置效应以及单词识别中拥挤和频率效应的相互作用方面的能力。我们进一步研究了这些影响是如何被视觉注意力分布的变化所调节的。我们发现视觉注意力调节了这三种效应,并且视觉注意力的狭窄分布导致了与受损读者相似的表现模式。总的来说,BRAID模型可以被看作是开发大声朗读和眼球运动控制的综合模型的核心构建块,或者是受损读者的视觉识别,或者是任何视觉注意力重要的环境。
{"title":"Visual attention matters during word recognition: A Bayesian modeling approach.","authors":"Thierry Phénix, Émilie Ginestet, Sylviane Valdois, Julien Diard","doi":"10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-024-02591-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is striking that visual attention, the process by which attentional resources are allocated in the visual field so as to locally enhance visual perception, is a pervasive component of models of eye movements in reading, but is seldom considered in models of isolated word recognition. We describe BRAID, a new Bayesian word-Recognition model with Attention, Interference and Dynamics. As most of its predecessors, BRAID incorporates three sensory, perceptual, and orthographic knowledge layers together with a lexical membership submodel. Its originality resides in also including three mechanisms that modulate letter identification within strings: an acuity gradient, lateral interference, and visual attention. We calibrated the model such that its temporal scale was consistent with behavioral data, and then explored the model's capacity to generalize to other, independent effects. We evaluated the model's capacity to account for the word length effect in lexical decision, for the optimal viewing position effect, and for the interaction of crowding and frequency effects in word recognition. We further examined how these effects were modulated by variations in the visual attention distribution. We show that visual attention modulates all three effects and that a narrow distribution of visual attention results in performance patterns that mimic those reported in impaired readers. Overall, the BRAID model could be conceived as a core building block, towards the development of integrated models of reading aloud and eye movement control, or of visual recognition of impaired readers, or any context in which visual attention does matter.</p>","PeriodicalId":20763,"journal":{"name":"Psychonomic Bulletin & Review","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1