首页 > 最新文献

Psychological methods最新文献

英文 中文
Comparison of two independent populations of compositional data with positive correlations among components using a nested dirichlet distribution. 使用嵌套狄利克雷分布比较成分间正相关的两个独立总体组成数据。
IF 7 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-16 DOI: 10.1037/met0000702
Jacob A Turner,Bianca A Luedeker,Monnie McGee
Compositional data are multivariate data made up of components that sum to a fixed value. Often the data are presented as proportions of a whole, where the value of each component is constrained to be between 0 and 1 and the sum of the components is 1. There are many applications in psychology and other disciplines that yield compositional data sets including Morris water maze experiments, psychological well-being scores, analysis of daily physical activity times, and components of household expenditures. Statistical methods exist for compositional data and typically consist of two approaches. The first is to use transformation strategies, such as log ratios, which can lead to results that are challenging to interpret. The second involves using an appropriate distribution, such as the Dirichlet distribution, that captures the key characteristics of compositional data, and allows for ready interpretation of downstream analysis. Unfortunately, the Dirichlet distribution has constraints on variance and correlation that render it inappropriate for some applications. As a result, practicing researchers will often resort to standard two-sample t test or analysis of variance models for each variable in the composition to detect differences in means. We show that a recently published method using the Dirichlet distribution can drastically inflate Type I error rates, and we introduce a global two-sample test to detect differences in mean proportion of components for two independent groups where both groups are from either a Dirichlet or a more flexible nested Dirichlet distribution. We also derive confidence interval formulas for individual components for post hoc testing and further interpretation of results. We illustrate the utility of our methods using a recent Morris water maze experiment and human activity data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).
组合数据是由和为固定值的组件组成的多变量数据。数据通常以整体的比例表示,其中每个成分的值被限制在0到1之间,成分的总和为1。在心理学和其他学科中,有许多应用产生了组成数据集,包括莫里斯水迷宫实验、心理健康评分、日常身体活动时间分析和家庭支出组成部分。存在用于组合数据的统计方法,通常包括两种方法。第一种是使用转换策略,例如对数比率,这可能导致难以解释的结果。第二种方法涉及使用适当的分布,例如Dirichlet分布,它捕获了成分数据的关键特征,并允许对下游分析进行现成的解释。不幸的是,狄利克雷分布对方差和相关性有限制,使得它不适合某些应用。因此,实践研究人员通常会对组成中的每个变量采用标准的双样本t检验或方差分析模型来检测平均值的差异。我们表明,最近发表的一种使用狄利克雷分布的方法可以大大提高I型错误率,并且我们引入了一个全局双样本检验来检测两个独立组的平均成分比例的差异,其中两个组都来自狄利克雷分布或更灵活的嵌套狄利克雷分布。我们还推导了用于事后测试和进一步解释结果的单个组件的置信区间公式。我们用最近的莫里斯水迷宫实验和人类活动数据来说明我们的方法的实用性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Comparison of two independent populations of compositional data with positive correlations among components using a nested dirichlet distribution.","authors":"Jacob A Turner,Bianca A Luedeker,Monnie McGee","doi":"10.1037/met0000702","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000702","url":null,"abstract":"Compositional data are multivariate data made up of components that sum to a fixed value. Often the data are presented as proportions of a whole, where the value of each component is constrained to be between 0 and 1 and the sum of the components is 1. There are many applications in psychology and other disciplines that yield compositional data sets including Morris water maze experiments, psychological well-being scores, analysis of daily physical activity times, and components of household expenditures. Statistical methods exist for compositional data and typically consist of two approaches. The first is to use transformation strategies, such as log ratios, which can lead to results that are challenging to interpret. The second involves using an appropriate distribution, such as the Dirichlet distribution, that captures the key characteristics of compositional data, and allows for ready interpretation of downstream analysis. Unfortunately, the Dirichlet distribution has constraints on variance and correlation that render it inappropriate for some applications. As a result, practicing researchers will often resort to standard two-sample t test or analysis of variance models for each variable in the composition to detect differences in means. We show that a recently published method using the Dirichlet distribution can drastically inflate Type I error rates, and we introduce a global two-sample test to detect differences in mean proportion of components for two independent groups where both groups are from either a Dirichlet or a more flexible nested Dirichlet distribution. We also derive confidence interval formulas for individual components for post hoc testing and further interpretation of results. We illustrate the utility of our methods using a recent Morris water maze experiment and human activity data. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142989142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamic factor analysis with multivariate time series of multiple individuals: An error-corrected estimation method. 多个体多变量时间序列动态因子分析:一种误差校正估计方法。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-09 DOI: 10.1037/met0000722
Guangjian Zhang, Dayoung Lee, Yilin Li, Anthony Ong

Intensive longitudinal data, increasingly common in social and behavioral sciences, often consist of multivariate time series from multiple individuals. Dynamic factor analysis, combining factor analysis and time series analysis, has been used to uncover individual-specific processes from single-individual time series. However, integrating these processes across individuals is challenging due to estimation errors in individual-specific parameter estimates. We propose a method that integrates individual-specific processes while accommodating the corresponding estimation error. This method is computationally efficient and robust against model specification errors and nonnormal data. We compare our method with a Naive approach that ignores estimation error using both empirical and simulated data. The two methods produced similar estimates for fixed effect parameters, but the proposed method produced more satisfactory estimates for random effects than the Naive method. The relative advantage of the proposed method was more substantial for short to moderately long time series (T = 56-200). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

密集的纵向数据在社会和行为科学中越来越常见,通常由来自多个个体的多元时间序列组成。动态因子分析,结合因子分析和时间序列分析,已经被用来揭示个体特定的过程,从单个个体的时间序列。然而,由于个体特定参数估计中的估计误差,跨个体集成这些过程是具有挑战性的。我们提出了一种集成个体特定过程的方法,同时容纳相应的估计误差。该方法计算效率高,对模型规范误差和非正态数据具有较强的鲁棒性。我们将我们的方法与使用经验和模拟数据忽略估计误差的朴素方法进行了比较。两种方法对固定效应参数的估计相似,但对随机效应的估计比Naive方法更令人满意。该方法的相对优势在较短至较长的时间序列(T = 56-200)中更为明显。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Dynamic factor analysis with multivariate time series of multiple individuals: An error-corrected estimation method.","authors":"Guangjian Zhang, Dayoung Lee, Yilin Li, Anthony Ong","doi":"10.1037/met0000722","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000722","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intensive longitudinal data, increasingly common in social and behavioral sciences, often consist of multivariate time series from multiple individuals. Dynamic factor analysis, combining factor analysis and time series analysis, has been used to uncover individual-specific processes from single-individual time series. However, integrating these processes across individuals is challenging due to estimation errors in individual-specific parameter estimates. We propose a method that integrates individual-specific processes while accommodating the corresponding estimation error. This method is computationally efficient and robust against model specification errors and nonnormal data. We compare our method with a Naive approach that ignores estimation error using both empirical and simulated data. The two methods produced similar estimates for fixed effect parameters, but the proposed method produced more satisfactory estimates for random effects than the Naive method. The relative advantage of the proposed method was more substantial for short to moderately long time series (<i>T</i> = 56-200). (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2025-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142953930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A causal research pipeline and tutorial for psychologists and social scientists. 心理学家和社会科学家的因果研究管道和教程。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1037/met0000673
Matthew James Vowels

Causality is a fundamental part of the scientific endeavor to understand the world. Unfortunately, causality is still taboo in much of psychology and social science. Motivated by a growing number of recommendations for the importance of adopting causal approaches to research, we reformulate the typical approach to research in psychology to harmonize inevitably causal theories with the rest of the research pipeline. We present a new process which begins with the incorporation of techniques from the confluence of causal discovery and machine learning for the development, validation, and transparent formal specification of theories. We then present methods for reducing the complexity of the fully specified theoretical model into the fundamental submodel relevant to a given target hypothesis. From here, we establish whether or not the quantity of interest is estimable from the data, and if so, propose the use of semi-parametric machine learning methods for the estimation of causal effects. The overall goal is the presentation of a new research pipeline which can (a) facilitate scientific inquiry compatible with the desire to test causal theories (b) encourage transparent representation of our theories as unambiguous mathematical objects, (c) tie our statistical models to specific attributes of the theory, thus reducing under-specification problems frequently resulting from the theory-to-model gap, and (d) yield results and estimates which are causally meaningful and reproducible. The process is demonstrated through didactic examples with real-world data, and we conclude with a summary and discussion of limitations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

因果关系是理解世界的科学努力的基本组成部分。不幸的是,因果关系在很多心理学和社会科学领域仍然是禁忌。由于越来越多的人建议采用因果研究方法的重要性,我们重新制定了心理学研究的典型方法,以不可避免地使因果理论与其他研究管道协调一致。我们提出了一个新的过程,从结合因果发现和机器学习的融合技术开始,用于理论的开发、验证和透明的形式规范。然后,我们提出了将完全指定的理论模型的复杂性降低到与给定目标假设相关的基本子模型的方法。从这里,我们确定感兴趣的数量是否可以从数据中估计,如果是,建议使用半参数机器学习方法来估计因果效应。总体目标是呈现一个新的研究管道,它可以(a)促进与测试因果理论的愿望相容的科学探究(b)鼓励我们的理论作为明确的数学对象的透明表示,(c)将我们的统计模型与理论的特定属性联系起来,从而减少由于理论与模型之间的差距而经常导致的规格不足问题,以及(d)产生因果意义和可重复的结果和估计。该过程通过具有实际数据的教学示例进行演示,并以总结和讨论局限性作为结论。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"A causal research pipeline and tutorial for psychologists and social scientists.","authors":"Matthew James Vowels","doi":"10.1037/met0000673","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000673","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Causality is a fundamental part of the scientific endeavor to understand the world. Unfortunately, causality is still taboo in much of psychology and social science. Motivated by a growing number of recommendations for the importance of adopting causal approaches to research, we reformulate the typical approach to research in psychology to harmonize inevitably causal theories with the rest of the research pipeline. We present a new process which begins with the incorporation of techniques from the confluence of causal discovery and machine learning for the development, validation, and transparent formal specification of theories. We then present methods for reducing the complexity of the fully specified theoretical model into the fundamental submodel relevant to a given target hypothesis. From here, we establish whether or not the quantity of interest is estimable from the data, and if so, propose the use of semi-parametric machine learning methods for the estimation of causal effects. The overall goal is the presentation of a new research pipeline which can (a) facilitate scientific inquiry compatible with the desire to test causal theories (b) encourage transparent representation of our theories as unambiguous mathematical objects, (c) tie our statistical models to specific attributes of the theory, thus reducing under-specification problems frequently resulting from the theory-to-model gap, and (d) yield results and estimates which are causally meaningful and reproducible. The process is demonstrated through didactic examples with real-world data, and we conclude with a summary and discussion of limitations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142932515","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dynamic structural equation modeling with floor effects. 考虑楼板效应的动力结构方程建模。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2025-01-06 DOI: 10.1037/met0000720
Bengt Muthén, Tihomir Asparouhov, Saul Shiffman

Intensive longitudinal data analysis, commonly used in psychological studies, often concerns outcomes that have strong floor effects, that is, a large percentage at its lowest value. Ignoring a strong floor effect, using regular analysis with modeling assumptions suitable for a continuous-normal outcome, is likely to give misleading results. This article suggests that two-part modeling may provide a solution. It can avoid potential biasing effects due to ignoring the floor effect. It can also provide a more detailed description of the relationships between the outcome and covariates allowing different covariate effects for being at the floor or not and the value above the floor. A smoking cessation example is analyzed to demonstrate available analysis techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).

密集的纵向数据分析通常用于心理学研究,通常涉及具有强底效应的结果,即在其最低值处的百分比很大。忽略强底效应,使用适合于连续正态结果的建模假设的常规分析,可能会给出误导性的结果。本文建议两部分建模可以提供一个解决方案。它可以避免由于忽略地板效应而产生的潜在偏置效应。它还可以对结果和协变量之间的关系提供更详细的描述,允许不同的协变量影响是否处于下限和高于下限的值。本文分析了一个戒烟的例子,以演示可用的分析技术。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Dynamic structural equation modeling with floor effects.","authors":"Bengt Muthén, Tihomir Asparouhov, Saul Shiffman","doi":"10.1037/met0000720","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000720","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intensive longitudinal data analysis, commonly used in psychological studies, often concerns outcomes that have strong floor effects, that is, a large percentage at its lowest value. Ignoring a strong floor effect, using regular analysis with modeling assumptions suitable for a continuous-normal outcome, is likely to give misleading results. This article suggests that two-part modeling may provide a solution. It can avoid potential biasing effects due to ignoring the floor effect. It can also provide a more detailed description of the relationships between the outcome and covariates allowing different covariate effects for being at the floor or not and the value above the floor. A smoking cessation example is analyzed to demonstrate available analysis techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2025 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2025-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142932518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A guided tutorial on linear mixed-effects models for the analysis of accuracies and response times in experiments with fully crossed design. 线性混合效应模型指导教程,用于分析完全交叉设计实验中的精确度和响应时间。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000708
Ottavia M Epifania, Pasquale Anselmi, Egidio Robusto

Experiments with fully crossed designs are often used in experimental psychology spanning several fields, from cognitive psychology to social cognition. These experiments consist in the presentation of stimuli representing super-ordinate categories, which have to be sorted into the correct category in two contrasting conditions. This tutorial presents a linear mixed-effects model approach for obtaining Rasch-like parameterizations of response times and accuracies of fully crossed design data. The modeling framework for the analysis of fully crossed design data is outlined along with a step-by-step guide of its application, which is further illustrated with two practical examples based on empirical data. The first example regards a cognitive psychology experiment and pertains to the evaluation of a spatial-numerical association of response codes effect. The second one is based on a social cognition experiment for the implicit evaluation of racial attitudes. A fully commented R script for reproducing the analyses illustrated in the examples is available in the online supplemental materials. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在认知心理学和社会认知等多个领域的实验心理学中,经常会用到完全交叉设计的实验。这些实验包括呈现代表上位类别的刺激物,这些刺激物必须在两种对比条件下被分类到正确的类别中。本教程介绍了一种线性混合效应模型方法,用于获取完全交叉设计数据的响应时间和准确率的 Rasch 类参数。本教程概述了用于分析完全交叉设计数据的建模框架,并提供了应用该框架的分步指南。第一个例子涉及认知心理学实验,与反应代码的空间-数字关联效应评估有关。第二个例子是基于种族态度内隐评估的社会认知实验。在线补充材料中提供了一个完整注释的 R 脚本,用于重现示例中的分析。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"A guided tutorial on linear mixed-effects models for the analysis of accuracies and response times in experiments with fully crossed design.","authors":"Ottavia M Epifania, Pasquale Anselmi, Egidio Robusto","doi":"10.1037/met0000708","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000708","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Experiments with fully crossed designs are often used in experimental psychology spanning several fields, from cognitive psychology to social cognition. These experiments consist in the presentation of stimuli representing super-ordinate categories, which have to be sorted into the correct category in two contrasting conditions. This tutorial presents a linear mixed-effects model approach for obtaining Rasch-like parameterizations of response times and accuracies of fully crossed design data. The modeling framework for the analysis of fully crossed design data is outlined along with a step-by-step guide of its application, which is further illustrated with two practical examples based on empirical data. The first example regards a cognitive psychology experiment and pertains to the evaluation of a spatial-numerical association of response codes effect. The second one is based on a social cognition experiment for the implicit evaluation of racial attitudes. A fully commented R script for reproducing the analyses illustrated in the examples is available in the online supplemental materials. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bayes factors for logistic (mixed-effect) models. logistic(混合效应)模型的贝叶斯因子。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000714
Catriona Silvey, Zoltan Dienes, Elizabeth Wonnacott

In psychology, we often want to know whether or not an effect exists. The traditional way of answering this question is to use frequentist statistics. However, a significance test against a null hypothesis of no effect cannot distinguish between two states of affairs: evidence of absence of an effect and the absence of evidence for or against an effect. Bayes factors can make this distinction; however, uptake of Bayes factors in psychology has so far been low for two reasons. First, they require researchers to specify the range of effect sizes their theory predicts. Researchers are often unsure about how to do this, leading to the use of inappropriate default values which may give misleading results. Second, many implementations of Bayes factors have a substantial technical learning curve. We present a case study and simulations demonstrating a simple method for generating a range of plausible effect sizes, that is, a model of Hypothesis 1, for treatment effects where there is a binary-dependent variable. We illustrate this using mainly the estimates from frequentist logistic mixed-effects models (because of their widespread adoption) but also using Bayesian model comparison with Bayesian hierarchical models (which have increased flexibility). Bayes factors calculated using these estimates provide intuitively reasonable results across a range of real effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在心理学中,我们经常想知道某种效应是否存在。回答这个问题的传统方法是使用频数统计。然而,针对 "无效应 "零假设的显著性检验无法区分两种情况:没有效应的证据和没有支持或反对效应的证据。贝叶斯因子可以做出这种区分;然而,由于两个原因,贝叶斯因子在心理学中的应用至今还很低。首先,贝叶斯因子要求研究人员明确指出其理论所预测的效应大小范围。研究人员往往不知道如何做到这一点,从而导致使用不恰当的默认值,这可能会产生误导性结果。其次,贝叶斯因子的许多实现方法都有很大的技术学习曲线。我们介绍了一个案例研究和模拟实验,展示了一种简单的方法来生成一系列可信的效应大小,即假设 1 模型,用于二元变量依赖的治疗效果。我们主要使用频数逻辑混合效应模型的估计值(因为它们被广泛采用)来说明这一点,但也使用贝叶斯模型与贝叶斯层次模型(具有更大的灵活性)进行比较。使用这些估计值计算出的贝叶斯系数在实际效应大小范围内提供了直观合理的结果。(PsycInfo 数据库记录 (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Bayes factors for logistic (mixed-effect) models.","authors":"Catriona Silvey, Zoltan Dienes, Elizabeth Wonnacott","doi":"10.1037/met0000714","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000714","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In psychology, we often want to know whether or not an effect exists. The traditional way of answering this question is to use frequentist statistics. However, a significance test against a null hypothesis of no effect cannot distinguish between two states of affairs: evidence of absence of an effect and the absence of evidence for or against an effect. Bayes factors can make this distinction; however, uptake of Bayes factors in psychology has so far been low for two reasons. First, they require researchers to specify the range of effect sizes their theory predicts. Researchers are often unsure about how to do this, leading to the use of inappropriate default values which may give misleading results. Second, many implementations of Bayes factors have a substantial technical learning curve. We present a case study and simulations demonstrating a simple method for generating a range of plausible effect sizes, that is, a model of Hypothesis 1, for treatment effects where there is a binary-dependent variable. We illustrate this using mainly the estimates from frequentist logistic mixed-effects models (because of their widespread adoption) but also using Bayesian model comparison with Bayesian hierarchical models (which have increased flexibility). Bayes factors calculated using these estimates provide intuitively reasonable results across a range of real effect sizes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819021","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing bipolarity. 测试两极性。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000707
Kimberly A Barchard, James M Carroll, Shawn Reynolds, James A Russell

Many psychological dimensions seem bipolar (e.g., happy-sad, optimism-pessimism, and introversion-extraversion). However, seeming opposites frequently do not act the way researchers predict real opposites would: having correlations near -1, loading on the same factor, and having relations with external variables that are equal in magnitude and opposite in sign. We argue these predictions are often incorrect because the bipolar model has been misspecified or specified too narrowly. We therefore explicitly define a general bipolar model for ideal error-free data and then extend this model to empirical data influenced by random and systematic measurement error. Our model shows the predictions above are correct only under restrictive circumstances that are unlikely to apply in practice. Moreover, if a bipolar dimension is divided into two so that researchers can test bipolarity, our model shows that the correlation between the two can be far from -1; thus, strategies based upon Pearson product-moment correlations and their factor analyses do not test if variables are opposites. Moreover, the two parts need not be mutually exclusive; thus, measures of co-occurrence do not test if variables are opposites. We offer alternative strategies for testing if variables are opposites, strategies based upon censored data analysis. Our model and findings have implications not just for testing bipolarity, but also for associated theory and measurement, and they expose potential artifacts in correlational and dimensional analyses involving any type of negative relations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

许多心理维度似乎是两极的(例如,快乐-悲伤,乐观-悲观,内向-外向)。然而,表面上的对立往往不会像研究人员预测的那样发挥作用:相关性接近-1,加载相同的因素,与外部变量的关系大小相等,符号相反。我们认为这些预测往往是不正确的,因为双极模型被错误地指定或指定得太窄。因此,我们明确定义了理想无误差数据的一般双极模型,然后将该模型扩展到受随机和系统测量误差影响的经验数据。我们的模型表明,上述预测只有在不太可能应用于实践的限制性条件下才是正确的。此外,如果将双相维度分为两个,以便研究人员可以测试双极性,我们的模型显示两者之间的相关性可以远离-1;因此,基于皮尔逊积矩相关性及其因素分析的策略不能测试变量是否相反。此外,这两个部分不一定是相互排斥的;因此,共现的度量不能检验变量是否相反。我们提供了测试变量是否相反的替代策略,基于审查数据分析的策略。我们的模型和发现不仅对测试双极性有影响,而且对相关的理论和测量也有影响,并且它们揭示了涉及任何类型的负相关的相关和维度分析中的潜在伪影。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Testing bipolarity.","authors":"Kimberly A Barchard, James M Carroll, Shawn Reynolds, James A Russell","doi":"10.1037/met0000707","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000707","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many psychological dimensions seem bipolar (e.g., happy-sad, optimism-pessimism, and introversion-extraversion). However, seeming opposites frequently do not act the way researchers predict real opposites would: having correlations near -1, loading on the same factor, and having relations with external variables that are equal in magnitude and opposite in sign. We argue these predictions are often incorrect because the bipolar model has been misspecified or specified too narrowly. We therefore explicitly define a general bipolar model for ideal error-free data and then extend this model to empirical data influenced by random and systematic measurement error. Our model shows the predictions above are correct only under restrictive circumstances that are unlikely to apply in practice. Moreover, if a bipolar dimension is divided into two so that researchers can test bipolarity, our model shows that the correlation between the two can be far from -1; thus, strategies based upon Pearson product-moment correlations and their factor analyses do not test if variables are opposites. Moreover, the two parts need not be mutually exclusive; thus, measures of co-occurrence do not test if variables are opposites. We offer alternative strategies for testing if variables are opposites, strategies based upon censored data analysis. Our model and findings have implications not just for testing bipolarity, but also for associated theory and measurement, and they expose potential artifacts in correlational and dimensional analyses involving any type of negative relations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819092","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of a quadratic term in estimating the average treatment effect from longitudinal randomized controlled trials with missing data. 二次项在估计缺少数据的纵向随机对照试验的平均治疗效果中的作用。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000709
Manshu Yang, Lijuan Wang, Scott E Maxwell

Longitudinal randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been commonly used in psychological studies to evaluate the effectiveness of treatment or intervention strategies. Outcomes in longitudinal RCTs may follow either straight-line or curvilinear change trajectories over time, and missing data are almost inevitable in such trials. The current study aims to investigate (a) whether the estimate of average treatment effect (ATE) would be biased if a straight-line growth (SLG) model is fit to longitudinal RCT data with quadratic growth and missing completely at random (MCAR) or missing at random (MAR) data, and (b) whether adding a quadratic term to an SLG model would improve the ATE estimation and inference. Four models were compared via a simulation study, including the SLG model, the quadratic growth model with arm-invariant and fixed quadratic effect (QG-AIF), the quadratic growth model with arm-specific and fixed quadratic effects (QG-ASF), and the quadratic growth model with arm-specific and random quadratic effects (QG-ASR). Results suggest that fitting an SLG model to quadratic growth data often yielded severe biases in ATE estimates, even if data were MCAR or MAR. Given four or more waves of longitudinal data, the QG-ASR model outperformed the other methods; for three-wave data, the QG-ASR model was not applicable and the QG-ASF model performed well. Applications of different models are also illustrated using an empirical data example. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

纵向随机对照试验(RCT)通常用于心理研究,以评估治疗或干预策略的有效性。纵向随机对照试验的结果随着时间的推移可能呈现直线或曲线变化轨迹,而数据缺失在这类试验中几乎是不可避免的。本研究旨在探讨:(a) 如果将直线增长(SLG)模型拟合到具有二次增长和完全随机缺失(MCAR)或随机缺失(MAR)数据的纵向 RCT 数据中,平均治疗效果(ATE)的估计值是否会出现偏差;(b) 在 SLG 模型中加入二次项是否会改善 ATE 的估计和推断。通过模拟研究对四种模型进行了比较,包括 SLG 模型、具有臂不变量和固定二次方效应的二次方生长模型(QG-AIF)、具有臂特异性和固定二次方效应的二次方生长模型(QG-ASF)以及具有臂特异性和随机二次方效应的二次方生长模型(QG-ASR)。结果表明,即使数据是 MCAR 或 MAR,将 SLG 模型拟合到二次方生长数据中往往会导致 ATE 估计值出现严重偏差。对于四波或更多波的纵向数据,QG-ASR 模型的表现优于其他方法;对于三波数据,QG-ASR 模型不适用,而 QG-ASF 模型表现良好。此外,还通过一个经验数据实例说明了不同模型的应用。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The role of a quadratic term in estimating the average treatment effect from longitudinal randomized controlled trials with missing data.","authors":"Manshu Yang, Lijuan Wang, Scott E Maxwell","doi":"10.1037/met0000709","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000709","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Longitudinal randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been commonly used in psychological studies to evaluate the effectiveness of treatment or intervention strategies. Outcomes in longitudinal RCTs may follow either straight-line or curvilinear change trajectories over time, and missing data are almost inevitable in such trials. The current study aims to investigate (a) whether the estimate of average treatment effect (ATE) would be biased if a straight-line growth (SLG) model is fit to longitudinal RCT data with quadratic growth and missing completely at random (MCAR) or missing at random (MAR) data, and (b) whether adding a quadratic term to an SLG model would improve the ATE estimation and inference. Four models were compared via a simulation study, including the SLG model, the quadratic growth model with arm-invariant and fixed quadratic effect (QG-AIF), the quadratic growth model with arm-specific and fixed quadratic effects (QG-ASF), and the quadratic growth model with arm-specific and random quadratic effects (QG-ASR). Results suggest that fitting an SLG model to quadratic growth data often yielded severe biases in ATE estimates, even if data were MCAR or MAR. Given four or more waves of longitudinal data, the QG-ASR model outperformed the other methods; for three-wave data, the QG-ASR model was not applicable and the QG-ASF model performed well. Applications of different models are also illustrated using an empirical data example. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819093","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Power analysis to detect misfit in SEMs with many items: Resolving unrecognized problems, relating old and new approaches, and "matching" power analysis approach to data analysis approach. 功率分析:解决未被识别的问题,将新旧方法联系起来,并将功率分析方法与数据分析方法“匹配”。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000684
Amy Liang, Sonya K Sterba

It is unappreciated that there are four different approaches to power analysis for detecting misspecification by testing overall fit of structural equation models (SEMs) and, moreover, that common approaches can yield radically diverging results for SEMs with many items (high p). Here we newly relate these four approaches. Analytical power analysis methods using theoretical null and theoretical alternative distributions (Approach 1) have a long history, are widespread, and are often contrasted with "the" Monte Carlo method-which is an oversimplification. Actually, three Monte Carlo methods can be distinguished; all use an empirical alternative distribution but differ regarding whether the null distribution is theoretical (Approach 2), empirical (Approach 3), or-as we newly propose and demonstrate the need for-adjusted empirical (Approach 4). Because these four approaches can yield radically diverging power results under high p (as demonstrated here), researchers need to "match" their a priori SEM power analysis approach to their later SEM data analysis approach for testing overall fit, once data are collected. Disturbingly, the most common power analysis approach for a global test-of-fit is mismatched with the most common data analysis approach for a global test-of-fit in SEM. Because of this mismatch, researchers' anticipated versus actual/obtained power can differ substantially. We explain how/why to "match" across power-analysis and data-analysis phases of a study and provide software to facilitate doing so. As extensions, we explain how to relate and implement all four approaches to power analysis (a) for testing overall fit using χ² versus root-mean-square error of approximation and (b) for testing overall fit versus testing a target parameter/effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

通过测试结构方程模型(SEM)的总体拟合度来检测规范失当的幂次分析有四种不同的方法,而且,对于具有许多项目(高 p)的 SEM,普通方法可能会产生截然不同的结果,这一点尚未得到重视。在此,我们新近将这四种方法联系起来。使用理论空分布和理论备择分布的分析幂分析方法(方法 1)历史悠久、应用广泛,经常与蒙特卡罗方法形成鲜明对比--这未免过于简单化。实际上,有三种蒙特卡罗方法可以区分;它们都使用经验替代分布,但在空分布是理论分布(方法 2)、经验分布(方法 3),还是我们新提出并证明需要的调整经验分布(方法 4)方面存在差异。由于这四种方法在高 p 条件下会产生截然不同的功率结果(如本文所示),因此研究人员需要在收集数据后,将其先验的 SEM 功率分析方法与后来用于测试总体拟合度的 SEM 数据分析方法 "匹配 "起来。令人不安的是,全局拟合度测试中最常见的功率分析方法与 SEM 中全局拟合度测试中最常见的数据分析方法并不匹配。由于这种不匹配,研究人员的预期功率与实际功率/获得功率会有很大差异。我们解释了如何/为什么要在研究的功率分析和数据分析阶段之间进行 "匹配",并提供了便于这样做的软件。作为扩展,我们解释了如何联系和实施所有四种功率分析方法:(a) 使用 χ² 与均方根近似误差测试总体拟合度;(b) 测试总体拟合度与测试目标参数/效应。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Power analysis to detect misfit in SEMs with many items: Resolving unrecognized problems, relating old and new approaches, and \"matching\" power analysis approach to data analysis approach.","authors":"Amy Liang, Sonya K Sterba","doi":"10.1037/met0000684","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000684","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>It is unappreciated that there are four different approaches to power analysis for detecting misspecification by testing overall fit of structural equation models (SEMs) and, moreover, that common approaches can yield radically diverging results for SEMs with many items (high <i>p</i>). Here we newly relate these four approaches. Analytical power analysis methods using theoretical null and theoretical alternative distributions (Approach 1) have a long history, are widespread, and are often contrasted with \"the\" Monte Carlo method-which is an oversimplification. Actually, three Monte Carlo methods can be distinguished; all use an empirical alternative distribution but differ regarding whether the null distribution is theoretical (Approach 2), empirical (Approach 3), or-as we newly propose and demonstrate the need for-adjusted empirical (Approach 4). Because these four approaches can yield radically diverging power results under high <i>p</i> (as demonstrated here), researchers need to \"match\" their a priori SEM power analysis approach to their later SEM data analysis approach for testing overall fit, once data are collected. Disturbingly, the most common power analysis approach for a global test-of-fit is mismatched with the most common data analysis approach for a global test-of-fit in SEM. Because of this mismatch, researchers' anticipated versus actual/obtained power can differ substantially. We explain how/why to \"match\" across power-analysis and data-analysis phases of a study and provide software to facilitate doing so. As extensions, we explain how to relate and implement all four approaches to power analysis (a) for testing overall fit using χ² versus root-mean-square error of approximation and (b) for testing overall fit versus testing a target parameter/effect. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819027","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Building a simpler moderated nonlinear factor analysis model with Markov Chain Monte Carlo estimation. 利用马尔可夫链蒙特卡洛估计建立更简单的调节非线性因素分析模型。
IF 7.6 1区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2024-12-12 DOI: 10.1037/met0000712
Craig K Enders, Juan Diego Vera, Brian T Keller, Agatha Lenartowicz, Sandra K Loo

Moderated nonlinear factor analysis (MNLFA) has emerged as an important and flexible data analysis tool, particularly in integrative data analysis setting and psychometric studies of measurement invariance and differential item functioning. Substantive applications abound in the literature and span a broad range of disciplines. MNLFA unifies item response theory, multiple group, and multiple indicator multiple cause modeling traditions, and it extends these frameworks by conceptualizing latent variable heterogeneity as a source of differential item functioning. The purpose of this article was to illustrate a flexible Markov chain Monte Carlo-based approach to MNLFA that offers statistical and practical enhancements to likelihood-based estimation while remaining plug and play with established analytic practices. Among other things, these enhancements include (a) missing data handling functionality for incomplete moderators, (b) multiply imputed factor score estimates that integrate into existing multiple imputation inferential methods, (c) support for common data types, including normal/continuous, nonnormal/continuous, binary, ordinal, multicategorical nominal, count, and two-part constructions for floor and ceiling effects, (d) novel residual diagnostics for identifying potential sources of differential item function, (e) manifest-by-latent variable interaction effects that replace complex moderation function constraints, and (f) integration with familiar regression modeling strategies, including graphical diagnostics. A real data analysis example using the Blimp software application illustrates these features. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

有调节非线性因子分析(MNLFA)已成为一种重要而灵活的数据分析工具,特别是在综合数据分析设置和测量不变性和微分项目功能的心理测量学研究中。实质性的应用在文献中比比皆是,并跨越了广泛的学科范围。MNLFA统一了项目反应理论、多群体和多指标多原因建模传统,并通过将潜在变量异质性概念化为差异项目功能的来源来扩展这些框架。本文的目的是说明一种灵活的基于马尔可夫链蒙特卡罗的MNLFA方法,该方法为基于似然的估计提供了统计和实用的增强,同时保留了即插即用的已建立的分析实践。除此之外,这些增强包括(a)不完整调节器的数据处理功能缺失,(b)集成到现有的多输入推理方法中的多重输入因子得分估计,(c)支持常见数据类型,包括正常/连续,非正常/连续,二进制,有序,多分类标称,计数,以及地板和天花板效应的两部分结构。(d)用于识别差分项函数潜在来源的新型剩余诊断,(e)取代复杂调节函数约束的显性潜变量交互效应,以及(f)与熟悉的回归建模策略的集成,包括图形诊断。一个使用Blimp软件应用程序的真实数据分析示例说明了这些特性。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Building a simpler moderated nonlinear factor analysis model with Markov Chain Monte Carlo estimation.","authors":"Craig K Enders, Juan Diego Vera, Brian T Keller, Agatha Lenartowicz, Sandra K Loo","doi":"10.1037/met0000712","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000712","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Moderated nonlinear factor analysis (MNLFA) has emerged as an important and flexible data analysis tool, particularly in integrative data analysis setting and psychometric studies of measurement invariance and differential item functioning. Substantive applications abound in the literature and span a broad range of disciplines. MNLFA unifies item response theory, multiple group, and multiple indicator multiple cause modeling traditions, and it extends these frameworks by conceptualizing latent variable heterogeneity as a source of differential item functioning. The purpose of this article was to illustrate a flexible Markov chain Monte Carlo-based approach to MNLFA that offers statistical and practical enhancements to likelihood-based estimation while remaining plug and play with established analytic practices. Among other things, these enhancements include (a) missing data handling functionality for incomplete moderators, (b) multiply imputed factor score estimates that integrate into existing multiple imputation inferential methods, (c) support for common data types, including normal/continuous, nonnormal/continuous, binary, ordinal, multicategorical nominal, count, and two-part constructions for floor and ceiling effects, (d) novel residual diagnostics for identifying potential sources of differential item function, (e) manifest-by-latent variable interaction effects that replace complex moderation function constraints, and (f) integration with familiar regression modeling strategies, including graphical diagnostics. A real data analysis example using the Blimp software application illustrates these features. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":20782,"journal":{"name":"Psychological methods","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":7.6,"publicationDate":"2024-12-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142819023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychological methods
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1