首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Long-term Contingency Learning Depends on Contingency Awareness.
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-02-11 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.433
Klaus Rothermund, Lennart Kapinos, Jan De Houwer, James R Schmidt

We examined long-term contingency learning (CL) in a color classification task with two separate sets of non-overlapping color-word contingencies that were employed in alternating blocks of the task ("alternating blocks paradigm"). Analyzing only the first occurrences of the word distractors in each block provides a pure indicator of long-term CL that is free from recency-based episodic retrieval processes. A high-powered (n = 110), pre-registered study revealed evidence for reliable long-term color-word CL. This long-term CL effect depended on contingency awareness, indicating that genuine long-term CL is influenced by propositional knowledge.

{"title":"Long-term Contingency Learning Depends on Contingency Awareness.","authors":"Klaus Rothermund, Lennart Kapinos, Jan De Houwer, James R Schmidt","doi":"10.5334/joc.433","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.433","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We examined long-term contingency learning (CL) in a color classification task with two separate sets of non-overlapping color-word contingencies that were employed in alternating blocks of the task (\"alternating blocks paradigm\"). Analyzing only the first occurrences of the word distractors in each block provides a pure indicator of long-term CL that is free from recency-based episodic retrieval processes. A high-powered (n = 110), pre-registered study revealed evidence for reliable long-term color-word CL. This long-term CL effect depended on contingency awareness, indicating that genuine long-term CL is influenced by propositional knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"23"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-02-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11827565/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143433983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
I am Once Again Asking for Your Attention: A Replication of Feature-Based Attention Modulations of Binding Effects with Picture Stimuli.
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-02-05 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.432
Tarini Singh, Lars-Michael Schöpper, Christian Frings

Action control theories assume that stimulus and response features are integrated or bound into short term episodic traces. A repetition of any of these features results in a retrieval of the entire episodic trace, and can thus facilitate or interfere with future actions. Along with stimuli features, features of the response and any other irrelevant stimuli that are present, are also integrated into such traces and can influence future actions. Using word stimuli, Singh et al. (2018) observed that such so-called binding effects are larger for attended features relative to unattended features. This was the case even for features generally believed to be automatically processed, like valence. Since previous research has shown differences in the processing of word and picture stimuli, it is questionable whether the attentional modulations in the above study would extend to picture stimuli. In order to examine this question, Experiment 1 replicated the design of Singh et al. (2018) but used picture instead of word stimuli. In order to directly compare word and picture stimuli, the data of Singh et al (2018) were re-analysed together with the data of the present study. In Experiment 2, the alternative hypothesis, that the effects were driven by the encoding of stimulus contingencies, was tested. Taken together, the findings of the present study replicate those of Singh et al. (2018), indicating that even with picture stimuli, valence related binding effects are modulated by attention allocation.

{"title":"I am Once Again Asking for Your Attention: A Replication of Feature-Based Attention Modulations of Binding Effects with Picture Stimuli.","authors":"Tarini Singh, Lars-Michael Schöpper, Christian Frings","doi":"10.5334/joc.432","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.432","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Action control theories assume that stimulus and response features are integrated or bound into short term episodic traces. A repetition of any of these features results in a retrieval of the entire episodic trace, and can thus facilitate or interfere with future actions. Along with stimuli features, features of the response and any other irrelevant stimuli that are present, are also integrated into such traces and can influence future actions. Using word stimuli, Singh et al. (2018) observed that such so-called binding effects are larger for attended features relative to unattended features. This was the case even for features generally believed to be automatically processed, like valence. Since previous research has shown differences in the processing of word and picture stimuli, it is questionable whether the attentional modulations in the above study would extend to picture stimuli. In order to examine this question, Experiment 1 replicated the design of Singh et al. (2018) but used picture instead of word stimuli. In order to directly compare word and picture stimuli, the data of Singh et al (2018) were re-analysed together with the data of the present study. In Experiment 2, the alternative hypothesis, that the effects were driven by the encoding of stimulus contingencies, was tested. Taken together, the findings of the present study replicate those of Singh et al. (2018), indicating that even with picture stimuli, valence related binding effects are modulated by attention allocation.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"22"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-02-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11804182/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143383276","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Implicit Learning of Parity and Magnitude Associations with Number Color.
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-28 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.428
Talia L Retter, Christine Schiltz

Associative learning can occur implicitly for stimuli that occur together probabilistically. It is debated whether probabilistic, implicit learning occurs not only at the item level, but also at the category level. Here, we investigated whether associative learning would occur between color and numerical categories, while participants performed a color task. In category-level experiments for each parity and magnitude, high-probability pairings of four numbers with one color were categorically consistent (e.g., the Arabic numerals 2,4,6, and 8 appeared in blue with a high probability, p = .9). Associative learning was measured as higher performance for high-probability vs. low-probability color/number pairings. For both parity and magnitude, performance was significantly better for high- vs. low-probability trials (parity: 3.1% more accurate; magnitude: 1.3% more accurate; 9 ms faster). Category-level learning was also evident in a subsequent color association report task with novel double-digit numbers (parity: 63% accuracy; magnitude: 55%). In control, item-level experiments, in which high-probability pairings were not categorically consistent (e.g., 2,3,6, and 7 appeared in blue with a high probability, p = .9), no significant differences between high- vs. low-probability trials were present. These results are in line with associative learning occurring at the category level, and, further, suggest automatic semantic processing of symbolic numerals in terms of parity and magnitude.

{"title":"Implicit Learning of Parity and Magnitude Associations with Number Color.","authors":"Talia L Retter, Christine Schiltz","doi":"10.5334/joc.428","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.428","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Associative learning can occur implicitly for stimuli that occur together probabilistically. It is debated whether probabilistic, implicit learning occurs not only at the item level, but also at the category level. Here, we investigated whether associative learning would occur between color and numerical categories, while participants performed a color task. In category-level experiments for each parity and magnitude, high-probability pairings of four numbers with one color were categorically consistent (e.g., the Arabic numerals 2,4,6, and 8 appeared in blue with a high probability, p = .9). Associative learning was measured as higher performance for high-probability vs. low-probability color/number pairings. For both parity and magnitude, performance was significantly better for high- vs. low-probability trials (parity: 3.1% more accurate; magnitude: 1.3% more accurate; 9 ms faster). Category-level learning was also evident in a subsequent color association report task with novel double-digit numbers (parity: 63% accuracy; magnitude: 55%). In control, item-level experiments, in which high-probability pairings were not categorically consistent (e.g., 2,3,6, and 7 appeared in blue with a high probability, p = .9), no significant differences between high- vs. low-probability trials were present. These results are in line with associative learning occurring at the category level, and, further, suggest automatic semantic processing of symbolic numerals in terms of parity and magnitude.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"21"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11784500/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143081159","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Readiness for Perception and Action: Towards a More Mechanistic Understanding of Phasic Alertness.
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-21 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.426
Christian H Poth

Human survival requires prompt perception and action to address relevant events in the environment. For this, the brain has evolved a system that uses warning stimuli to elicit phasic alertness, a state readying the brain for upcoming perception and action. Although a wealth of empirical evidence revealed how phasic alertness improves a wide range of perceptual and cognitive processing, it is still unclear by what cognitive mechanisms this is achieved. Here, we identify key problems that have to be solved for this to be possible and delineate concrete ways to achieve this. Specifically, we discover I) how to establish phasic alertness as a cognitive state of readiness for perception and action, II) how it can affect cognition online or offline, III) how it could be triggered internally without a warning, and IV) to what degrees it relied on bottom-up processing, or top-down temporal or stimulus expectations and the current task. As a result, the discussion provides us with a research program yielding the theoretical and empirical basis for mechanistic and computational models of phasic alertness and its neurophysiological underpinnings.

{"title":"Readiness for Perception and Action: Towards a More Mechanistic Understanding of Phasic Alertness.","authors":"Christian H Poth","doi":"10.5334/joc.426","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.426","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human survival requires prompt perception and action to address relevant events in the environment. For this, the brain has evolved a system that uses warning stimuli to elicit phasic alertness, a state readying the brain for upcoming perception and action. Although a wealth of empirical evidence revealed how phasic alertness improves a wide range of perceptual and cognitive processing, it is still unclear by what cognitive mechanisms this is achieved. Here, we identify key problems that have to be solved for this to be possible and delineate concrete ways to achieve this. Specifically, we discover I) how to establish phasic alertness as a cognitive state of readiness for perception and action, II) how it can affect cognition online or offline, III) how it could be triggered internally without a warning, and IV) to what degrees it relied on bottom-up processing, or top-down temporal or stimulus expectations and the current task. As a result, the discussion provides us with a research program yielding the theoretical and empirical basis for mechanistic and computational models of phasic alertness and its neurophysiological underpinnings.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"19"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11759528/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143048045","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring Inhibitory Control Processes in Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM): A Single Case Study.
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-21 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.421
Jessica Talbot, Daniele Gatti, Marta Boccalari, Michela Marchetti, Danilo Mitaritonna, Gianmarco Convertino, Mara Stockner, Giuliana Mazzoni

Individuals who possess a Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM) can remember their own lives in exceptional detail, retrieving specific autobiographical events in response to dates (e.g., 15th April 1995). The phenomenon remains extremely rare, and little is known about why these individuals can remember substantially more than the general population, without being continually flooded by past memories. According to the cognitive inhibition dependency hypothesis, inhibitory processes modulate general autobiographical memory by determining which memories will (and will not) enter one's consciousness. We hypothesised that these control processes are amplified in HSAM, protecting them from being overwhelmed by their abundance of memories. To explore if cognitive inhibition is exceptional in HSAM, a single case with HSAM (DT) and 20 matched controls completed a battery of 6 tasks assessing various aspects of inhibition (e.g., memory, prepotent motor responses). Participants also completed a screening for obsessive compulsive disorder and autism. Results indicate that DT's inhibitory functioning is comparable to that of the typical population, and thus not exceptional. We conclude that inhibition is unlikely to be the best explanation for extraordinary remembering and add to the growing body of literature that HSAM can occur in the absence of clinical symptomatology. Results are discussed in relation to future directions of HSAM research.

{"title":"Exploring Inhibitory Control Processes in Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM): A Single Case Study.","authors":"Jessica Talbot, Daniele Gatti, Marta Boccalari, Michela Marchetti, Danilo Mitaritonna, Gianmarco Convertino, Mara Stockner, Giuliana Mazzoni","doi":"10.5334/joc.421","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.421","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Individuals who possess a Highly Superior Autobiographical Memory (HSAM) can remember their own lives in exceptional detail, retrieving specific autobiographical events in response to dates (e.g., 15<sup>th</sup> April 1995). The phenomenon remains extremely rare, and little is known about why these individuals can remember substantially more than the general population, without being continually flooded by past memories. According to the <i>cognitive inhibition dependency hypothesis</i>, inhibitory processes modulate general autobiographical memory by determining which memories will (and will not) enter one's consciousness. We hypothesised that these control processes are amplified in HSAM, protecting them from being overwhelmed by their abundance of memories. To explore if cognitive inhibition is exceptional in HSAM, a single case with HSAM (DT) and 20 matched controls completed a battery of 6 tasks assessing various aspects of inhibition (e.g., memory, prepotent motor responses). Participants also completed a screening for obsessive compulsive disorder and autism. Results indicate that DT's inhibitory functioning is comparable to that of the typical population, and thus not exceptional. We conclude that inhibition is unlikely to be the best explanation for extraordinary remembering and add to the growing body of literature that HSAM can occur in the absence of clinical symptomatology. Results are discussed in relation to future directions of HSAM research.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"20"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11760751/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143048043","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
After a Hand Was Lent: Sporadically Experiencing Multisensory Interference During the Rubber Hand Illusion Does Not Shield Against Disembodiment. 在借出一只手之后:在橡胶手幻觉期间偶尔经历多感官干扰并不能防止分离。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-17 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.427
Julia Eck, David Dignath, Andreas Kalckert, Roland Pfister

Observations from multisensory body illusions indicate that the body representation can be adapted to changing task demands, e.g., it can be expanded to integrate external objects based on current sensorimotor experience (embodiment). While the mechanisms that promote embodiment have been studied extensively in earlier work, the opposite phenomenon of, removing an embodied entity from the body representation (i.e., disembodiment) has received little attention yet. The current study addressed this phenomenon and drew inspiration from the partial reinforcement extinction effect in instrumental learning which suggests that behavior is more resistant to extinction when reinforcement is delivered irregularly. In analogy to this, we investigated whether experiencing occasional visuo-motor mismatches during the induction phase of the moving rubber hand illusion (intermittent condition) would result in slower disembodiment as compared to a regular induction phase where motor and visual signals always match (continuous condition). However, we did not find an effect of reinforcement schedule on disembodiment. Keeping a recently embodied entity in the body schema, therefore, requires constant updating through correlated perceptual and motor signals.

多感官身体错觉的观察表明,身体表征可以适应不断变化的任务需求,例如,它可以根据当前的感觉运动经验扩展到整合外部物体(体现)。虽然促进具体化的机制在早期的工作中得到了广泛的研究,但从身体表征中移除具体化实体(即脱离具体化)的相反现象却很少得到关注。目前的研究解决了这一现象,并从工具学习中的部分强化消退效应中获得灵感,该效应表明,当强化不规律地传递时,行为更能抵抗消退。与此类似,我们研究了在移动橡胶手错觉的诱导阶段(间歇条件),与运动和视觉信号总是匹配的常规诱导阶段(连续条件)相比,是否会出现偶尔的视觉-运动不匹配,从而导致较慢的分离。然而,我们没有发现强化计划对分离的影响。因此,在身体图式中保持最近具体化的实体需要通过相关的感知和运动信号不断更新。
{"title":"After a Hand Was Lent: Sporadically Experiencing Multisensory Interference During the Rubber Hand Illusion Does Not Shield Against Disembodiment.","authors":"Julia Eck, David Dignath, Andreas Kalckert, Roland Pfister","doi":"10.5334/joc.427","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.427","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Observations from multisensory body illusions indicate that the body representation can be adapted to changing task demands, e.g., it can be expanded to integrate external objects based on current sensorimotor experience (embodiment). While the mechanisms that promote embodiment have been studied extensively in earlier work, the opposite phenomenon of, removing an embodied entity from the body representation (i.e., disembodiment) has received little attention yet. The current study addressed this phenomenon and drew inspiration from the partial reinforcement extinction effect in instrumental learning which suggests that behavior is more resistant to extinction when reinforcement is delivered irregularly. In analogy to this, we investigated whether experiencing occasional visuo-motor mismatches during the induction phase of the moving rubber hand illusion (intermittent condition) would result in slower disembodiment as compared to a regular induction phase where motor and visual signals always match (continuous condition). However, we did not find an effect of reinforcement schedule on disembodiment. Keeping a recently embodied entity in the body schema, therefore, requires constant updating through correlated perceptual and motor signals.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"18"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11740722/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143013061","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
It's Hard to Prepare for Task Novelty: Cueing the Novelty of Upcoming Tasks Does Not Facilitate Task Performance. 很难为任务的新颖性做好准备:提示即将到来的任务的新颖性并不能促进任务的执行。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-15 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.423
Mengqiao Chai, Ana F Palenciano, Ravi Mill, Michael W Cole, Senne Braem

Rapidly learning new tasks, such as using new technology or playing a new game, is ubiquitous in our daily lives. Previous studies suggest that our brain relies on different networks for rapid task learning versus retrieving known tasks from memory, and behavioral studies have shown that novel versus practiced tasks may rely on different task configuration processes. Here, we investigated whether explicitly informing about the novelty of an incoming task would help participants prepare for different task configuration processes, such as pre-adjusting working memory gating functions. We hypothesized that if different task configuration processes can be prepared for, a pre-cue informing about the novelty of the upcoming task should lead to better task performance. Across four experiments, participants were first trained on a subset of tasks, followed by a test session in which pre-cues were provided in some blocks but not others. After comparing task performance between cued and uncued blocks, our results provided no evidence supporting the benefit of cueing for both practiced and novel tasks, suggesting that people cannot prepare for different task configuration processes in the absence of concrete task information.

快速学习新任务,如使用新技术或玩新游戏,在我们的日常生活中无处不在。先前的研究表明,我们的大脑依赖于不同的网络来快速学习任务,而不是从记忆中检索已知任务,行为研究表明,新任务和实践任务可能依赖于不同的任务配置过程。在这里,我们研究了明确告知新任务的新颖性是否会帮助参与者为不同的任务配置过程做好准备,比如预先调整工作记忆门控功能。我们假设,如果可以为不同的任务配置过程做好准备,那么告知即将到来的任务的新颖性的预提示应该会导致更好的任务表现。在四个实验中,参与者首先接受了任务子集的训练,然后进行了一个测试环节,其中在某些块中提供了预先提示,而在其他块中没有。在比较了提示和未提示块的任务表现后,我们的研究结果没有提供证据支持提示对实践任务和新任务都有好处,这表明人们在缺乏具体任务信息的情况下无法为不同的任务配置过程做好准备。
{"title":"It's Hard to Prepare for Task Novelty: Cueing the Novelty of Upcoming Tasks Does Not Facilitate Task Performance.","authors":"Mengqiao Chai, Ana F Palenciano, Ravi Mill, Michael W Cole, Senne Braem","doi":"10.5334/joc.423","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.423","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Rapidly learning new tasks, such as using new technology or playing a new game, is ubiquitous in our daily lives. Previous studies suggest that our brain relies on different networks for rapid task learning versus retrieving known tasks from memory, and behavioral studies have shown that novel versus practiced tasks may rely on different task configuration processes. Here, we investigated whether explicitly informing about the novelty of an incoming task would help participants prepare for different task configuration processes, such as pre-adjusting working memory gating functions. We hypothesized that if different task configuration processes can be prepared for, a pre-cue informing about the novelty of the upcoming task should lead to better task performance. Across four experiments, participants were first trained on a subset of tasks, followed by a test session in which pre-cues were provided in some blocks but not others. After comparing task performance between cued and uncued blocks, our results provided no evidence supporting the benefit of cueing for both practiced and novel tasks, suggesting that people cannot prepare for different task configuration processes in the absence of concrete task information.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"17"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11740709/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143013068","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
As Time Goes By: Event File Decay Does Not Unleash Inhibition of Return. 随着时间的推移:事件文件衰减不会释放对返回的抑制。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-15 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.422
Lars-Michael Schöpper, Christian Frings

Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to a location repetition cost typically observed when signaling the detection of or localizing sequentially presented stimuli repeating or changing their location. In discrimination tasks, however, IOR is often reduced or even absent; here, effects of binding and retrieval are thought to take place. Information is bound into an event file, which upon feature repetition causes retrieval, leading to partial repetition costs. It is assumed that the presence of retrieval-based effects masks the observation of IOR. Yet, some evidence suggests that long intervals between stimuli can lead to IOR in tasks in which usually mostly binding effects are observed. We hypothesized that with an increasing interval between prime response and probe onset (response stimulus interval, RSI), event files will decay and decreasingly mask IOR. In turn, IOR should be strongest at longest intervals. In the current study, participants discriminated the color of stimuli repeating or changing their location. Crucially, we varied the RSI from 500 ms to 3000 ms, trial-wise (Experiment 1) and block-wise (Experiment 2). We observed overall binding effects that were reduced with increasing RSI; these effects were slightly stronger when presented block-wise. IOR was overall absent (Experiment 1) or weak (Experiment 2) and did not emerge with increasing RSI. While event file decay took place, it did not unleash IOR. Rather, these results suggest that retrieval-based effects do not simply mask but overwrite IOR when manually responding. The observations of IOR with long intervals are discussed in the context of overall fast responding.

返回抑制(IOR)是指当信号检测或定位顺序呈现的重复或改变其位置的刺激时,通常观察到的位置重复成本。然而,在辨别任务中,IOR往往减少甚至不存在;在这里,结合和检索的作用被认为是发生的。信息被绑定到一个事件文件中,当特征重复时,会导致检索,从而导致部分重复成本。假设基于检索的效应的存在掩盖了IOR的观察。然而,一些证据表明,刺激之间的长间隔可能导致通常主要观察到绑定效应的任务中的IOR。我们假设,随着启动反应和探测开始之间的间隔(反应刺激间隔,RSI)的增加,事件文件将衰减并逐渐掩盖IOR。反过来,IOR应该在最长的间隔内最强。在目前的研究中,参与者区分重复或改变其位置的刺激的颜色。至关重要的是,我们将RSI从500 ms改变为3000 ms,试验方向(实验1)和块方向(实验2)。我们观察到总体结合效应随着RSI的增加而降低;当以块为方向呈现时,这些效果略强。IOR总体上不存在(实验1)或较弱(实验2),并且不随RSI的增加而出现。虽然发生了事件文件衰减,但它没有释放IOR。相反,这些结果表明,在手动响应时,基于检索的效果不是简单地掩盖而是覆盖IOR。在整体快速响应的背景下,讨论了长间隔IOR的观测结果。
{"title":"As Time Goes By: Event File Decay Does Not Unleash Inhibition of Return.","authors":"Lars-Michael Schöpper, Christian Frings","doi":"10.5334/joc.422","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.422","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Inhibition of return (IOR) refers to a location repetition cost typically observed when signaling the detection of or localizing sequentially presented stimuli repeating or changing their location. In discrimination tasks, however, IOR is often reduced or even absent; here, effects of binding and retrieval are thought to take place. Information is bound into an event file, which upon feature repetition causes retrieval, leading to partial repetition costs. It is assumed that the presence of retrieval-based effects masks the observation of IOR. Yet, some evidence suggests that long intervals between stimuli can lead to IOR in tasks in which usually mostly binding effects are observed. We hypothesized that with an increasing interval between prime response and probe onset (response stimulus interval, RSI), event files will decay and decreasingly mask IOR. In turn, IOR should be strongest at longest intervals. In the current study, participants discriminated the color of stimuli repeating or changing their location. Crucially, we varied the RSI from 500 ms to 3000 ms, trial-wise (Experiment 1) and block-wise (Experiment 2). We observed overall binding effects that were reduced with increasing RSI; these effects were slightly stronger when presented block-wise. IOR was overall absent (Experiment 1) or weak (Experiment 2) and did not emerge with increasing RSI. While event file decay took place, it did not unleash IOR. Rather, these results suggest that retrieval-based effects do not simply mask but overwrite IOR when manually responding. The observations of IOR with long intervals are discussed in the context of overall fast responding.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11740711/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143013064","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Working Memory and Executive Functions: Theoretical Advances. 工作记忆与执行功能:理论进展。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-10 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.424
Sergio Morra, Steven J Howard, Vanessa M Loaiza

This editorial presents a special collection on working memory and executive functions. Six articles are presented and their contributions to current theoretical debates are briefly discussed.

这篇社论介绍了一个关于工作记忆和执行功能的特别收藏。本文介绍了六篇文章,并简要讨论了它们对当前理论争论的贡献。
{"title":"Working Memory and Executive Functions: Theoretical Advances.","authors":"Sergio Morra, Steven J Howard, Vanessa M Loaiza","doi":"10.5334/joc.424","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.424","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This editorial presents a special collection on working memory and executive functions. Six articles are presented and their contributions to current theoretical debates are briefly discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"15"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11720567/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972420","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Role of Morphological Information in Processing Pseudo-words in Italian L2 Learners: It's a Matter of Experience. 形态信息在意大利语二语学习者假词加工中的作用:一个经验问题。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2025-01-07 eCollection Date: 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.420
Simona Amenta, Francesca Foppolo, Linda Badan

The productive use of morphological information is considered one of the possible ways in which speakers of a language understand and learn unknown words. In the present study we investigate if, and how, also adult L2 learners exploit morphological information to process unknown words by analyzing the impact of language proficiency in the processing of novel derivations. Italian L2 learners, divided into three proficiency groups, participated in a lexical decision where pseudo-words could embed existing stems (e.g., sockle), suffixes (e.g., hettable), or both (novel derivations, e.g., quickify). Participants with low proficiency exhibited reduced accuracy and longer reaction times when presented with pseudo-words embedding a stem compared to those embedding a suffix. Conversely, participants with high proficiency demonstrated comparable accuracy in rejecting pseudo-words with real stems or real suffixes but required more time to reject pseudo-words embedding a suffix. In the case of novel derivations, accuracy (i.e., correct rejection) decreased and reaction time increased for all proficiency groups. Our results show that L2 learners exploit morphological information to process novel words. Most importantly, the ability to extract and exploit morphological information is linked to language proficiency.

形态学信息的有效利用被认为是一种语言的使用者理解和学习未知单词的可能方式之一。在本研究中,我们通过分析语言熟练程度对新衍生词加工的影响,探讨成人二语学习者是否以及如何利用形态信息加工未知单词。意大利语第二语言学习者被分成三个熟练程度组,参与了一个词汇决策,其中伪词可以嵌入现有词干(如sockle),后缀(如hettable),或两者兼有(新衍生词,如quickify)。与嵌入词干的假词相比,熟练程度较低的参与者在看到嵌入词干的假词时表现出较低的准确性和较长的反应时间。相反,熟练程度高的参与者在拒绝具有真实词干或真实后缀的假词时表现出相当的准确性,但拒绝包含后缀的假词需要更多的时间。在新衍生的情况下,所有熟练程度组的准确性(即正确拒绝)降低,反应时间增加。我们的研究结果表明,二语学习者利用词形信息来加工新单词。最重要的是,提取和利用词形信息的能力与语言能力有关。
{"title":"The Role of Morphological Information in Processing Pseudo-words in Italian L2 Learners: It's a Matter of Experience.","authors":"Simona Amenta, Francesca Foppolo, Linda Badan","doi":"10.5334/joc.420","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.420","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The productive use of morphological information is considered one of the possible ways in which speakers of a language understand and learn unknown words. In the present study we investigate if, and how, also adult L2 learners exploit morphological information to process unknown words by analyzing the impact of language proficiency in the processing of novel derivations. Italian L2 learners, divided into three proficiency groups, participated in a lexical decision where pseudo-words could embed existing stems (e.g., <i>sock</i>le), suffixes (e.g., hett<i>able</i>), or both (novel derivations, e.g., <i>quickify</i>). Participants with low proficiency exhibited reduced accuracy and longer reaction times when presented with pseudo-words embedding a stem compared to those embedding a suffix. Conversely, participants with high proficiency demonstrated comparable accuracy in rejecting pseudo-words with real stems or real suffixes but required more time to reject pseudo-words embedding a suffix. In the case of novel derivations, accuracy (i.e., correct rejection) decreased and reaction time increased for all proficiency groups. Our results show that L2 learners exploit morphological information to process novel words. Most importantly, the ability to extract and exploit morphological information is linked to language proficiency.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"8 1","pages":"14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11720858/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1