首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Dissociating the Effects of Visual Similarity for Brand Names and Common Words. 区分品牌名称和普通词汇的视觉相似性效应
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-08-28 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.397
Francisco Rocabado, Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia

Abstractionist models of visual word recognition can easily accommodate the absence of visual similarity effects in misspelled common words (e.g., viotin vs. viocin) during lexical decision tasks. However, these models fail to account for the sizable effects of visual similarity observed in misspelled brand names (e.g., anazon produces longer responses and more errors than atazon). Importantly, this dissociation has only been reported in separate experiments. Thus, a crucial experiment is necessary to simultaneously examine the role of visual similarity with misspelled common words and brand names. In the current experiment, participants performed a lexical decision task using both brand names and common words. Nonword foils were created by replacing visually similar letters (e.g., anazon [baseword: amazon], anarilllo [amarillo, yellow]) or visually dissimilar letters (e.g., atazon, atarillo). Results showed sizeable visual letter similarity effects for misspelled brand names in response times and percent error. Critically, these effects were absent for misspelled common words. The pervasiveness of visual similarity effects for misspelled brand names, even in the presence of common words, challenges purely abstractionist accounts of visual word recognition. Instead, these findings support instance-based and weakly abstractionist theories, suggesting that episodic traces in the mental lexicon may retain perceptual information, particularly when words are repeatedly presented in a similar format.

视觉单词识别的抽象主义模型可以很容易地解释在词汇决策任务中,拼写错误的普通单词(如 viotin 与 viocin)不存在视觉相似性效应。然而,这些模型却无法解释在拼写错误的品牌名称中观察到的显著的视觉相似性效应(例如,anazon 比 atazon 产生更长的反应时间和更多的错误)。重要的是,这种分离只在单独的实验中被报道过。因此,有必要进行一项重要实验,以同时研究视觉相似性在拼写错误的普通词和品牌名称中的作用。在本实验中,受试者同时使用品牌名称和普通词语进行词汇决策任务。通过替换视觉上相似的字母(如 anazon [基词:amazon]、anarilllo [amarillo,黄色])或视觉上不相似的字母(如 atazon、atarillo)来创建非词衬托。结果表明,对于拼写错误的品牌名称,视觉字母相似性在反应时间和错误率上都有相当大的影响。重要的是,拼写错误的普通单词不存在这些效应。即使在有普通单词的情况下,拼写错误的品牌名称也普遍存在视觉相似性效应,这对纯粹抽象主义的视觉单词识别方法提出了挑战。相反,这些发现支持基于实例和弱抽象主义的理论,表明心理词典中的历时性痕迹可能会保留知觉信息,尤其是当单词以类似格式重复出现时。
{"title":"Dissociating the Effects of Visual Similarity for Brand Names and Common Words.","authors":"Francisco Rocabado, Melanie Labusch, Manuel Perea, Jon Andoni Duñabeitia","doi":"10.5334/joc.397","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.397","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Abstractionist models of visual word recognition can easily accommodate the absence of visual similarity effects in misspelled common words (e.g., <i>viotin</i> vs. <i>viocin</i>) during lexical decision tasks. However, these models fail to account for the sizable effects of visual similarity observed in misspelled brand names (e.g., <i>anazon</i> produces longer responses and more errors than <i>atazon</i>). Importantly, this dissociation has only been reported in separate experiments. Thus, a crucial experiment is necessary to simultaneously examine the role of visual similarity with misspelled common words and brand names. In the current experiment, participants performed a lexical decision task using both brand names and common words. Nonword foils were created by replacing visually similar letters (e.g., <i>anazon</i> [baseword: <i>amazon</i>], <i>anarilllo</i> [amarillo, yellow]) or visually dissimilar letters (e.g., <i>atazon, atarillo</i>). Results showed sizeable visual letter similarity effects for misspelled brand names in response times and percent error. Critically, these effects were absent for misspelled common words. The pervasiveness of visual similarity effects for misspelled brand names, even in the presence of common words, challenges purely abstractionist accounts of visual word recognition. Instead, these findings support instance-based and weakly abstractionist theories, suggesting that episodic traces in the mental lexicon may retain perceptual information, particularly when words are repeatedly presented in a similar format.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"67"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363898/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142112798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Letters, Words, Sentences, and Reading. 字母、单词、句子和阅读。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-08-27 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.396
Jonathan Grainger

In this personal, and therefore highly selective, review article I summarize work performed in collaboration with numerous colleagues on how skilled adult readers perform identification tasks and speeded binary decision tasks involving single letters and visually presented words and sentences. The overarching aim is to highlight similarities in the processing performed at three key levels involved in written language comprehension (in languages that use an alphabetic script): letters, words, and sentences. The comparisons are made using behavioral data obtained with: i) speeded (response-limited) binary decision tasks; and ii) the effects of simultaneous surrounding context on letter and word identification using both data-limited (non-speeded) and response-limited procedures. I then propose a general framework that combines the three levels of processing, and that connects core processes at each level with the processing involved in tasks designed to reflect those core processes, and I end by suggesting possible avenues for future research with an aim to extend this general framework.

在这篇个人评论文章中,我总结了与许多同事合作完成的工作,这些工作涉及熟练的成年读者如何完成涉及单个字母和视觉呈现的单词和句子的识别任务和加速二元判定任务。文章的主要目的是突出书面语言理解(使用字母文字的语言)所涉及的三个关键层次(字母、单词和句子)的处理过程的相似性。比较使用的行为数据包括:i) 速度(反应受限)二元决策任务;ii) 同时使用数据受限(非速度)和反应受限程序的周围语境对字母和单词识别的影响。然后,我提出了一个总体框架,该框架结合了三个层次的处理过程,并将每个层次的核心过程与为反映这些核心过程而设计的任务中所涉及的处理过程联系起来。
{"title":"Letters, Words, Sentences, and Reading.","authors":"Jonathan Grainger","doi":"10.5334/joc.396","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.396","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this personal, and therefore highly selective, review article I summarize work performed in collaboration with numerous colleagues on how skilled adult readers perform identification tasks and speeded binary decision tasks involving single letters and visually presented words and sentences. The overarching aim is to highlight similarities in the processing performed at three key levels involved in written language comprehension (in languages that use an alphabetic script): letters, words, and sentences. The comparisons are made using behavioral data obtained with: i) speeded (response-limited) binary decision tasks; and ii) the effects of simultaneous surrounding context on letter and word identification using both data-limited (non-speeded) and response-limited procedures. I then propose a general framework that combines the three levels of processing, and that connects core processes at each level with the processing involved in tasks designed to reflect those core processes, and I end by suggesting possible avenues for future research with an aim to extend this general framework.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"66"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363890/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142112799","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
One Standard for All: Uniform Scale for Comparing Individuals and Groups in Hierarchical Bayesian Evidence Accumulation Modeling. 人人一个标准:在层次贝叶斯证据积累模型中比较个体和群体的统一尺度。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-08-16 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.394
Rotem Berkovich, Nachshon Meiran

In recent years, a growing body of research uses Evidence Accumulation Models (EAMs) to study individual differences and group effects. This endeavor is challenging because fitting EAMs requires constraining one of the EAM parameters to be equal for all participants, which makes a strong and possibly unlikely assumption. Moreover, if this assumption is violated, differences or lack thereof may be wrongly found. To overcome this limitation, in this study, we introduce a new method that was originally suggested by van Maanen & Miletić (2021), which employs Bayesian hierarchical estimation. In this new method, we set the scale at the population level, thereby allowing for individual and group differences, which is realized by de facto fixing a population-level hyper-parameter through its priors. As proof of concept, we ran two successful parameter recovery studies using the Linear Ballistic Accumulation model. The results suggest that the new method can be reliably used to study individual and group differences using EAMs. We further show a case in which the new method reveals the true group differences whereas the classic method wrongly detects differences that are truly absent.

近年来,越来越多的研究使用证据积累模型(EAM)来研究个体差异和群体效应。这项工作极具挑战性,因为拟合 EAMs 需要限制 EAM 的一个参数对所有参与者都是相等的,这就提出了一个强有力的、可能不太可能实现的假设。此外,如果违反了这一假设,可能会错误地发现差异或缺乏差异。为了克服这一局限性,我们在本研究中引入了 van Maanen 和 Miletić(2021 年)最初提出的一种新方法,即贝叶斯分层估计法。在这种新方法中,我们在群体水平上设定尺度,从而允许个体和群体的差异,这是通过事实上固定群体水平超参数的先验来实现的。作为概念验证,我们使用线性弹道累积模型进行了两次成功的参数恢复研究。结果表明,这种新方法可以可靠地用于使用 EAM 研究个体和群体差异。我们进一步展示了一个案例,在该案例中,新方法揭示了真正的群体差异,而传统方法则错误地检测出了真正不存在的差异。
{"title":"One Standard for All: Uniform Scale for Comparing Individuals and Groups in Hierarchical Bayesian Evidence Accumulation Modeling.","authors":"Rotem Berkovich, Nachshon Meiran","doi":"10.5334/joc.394","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.394","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In recent years, a growing body of research uses Evidence Accumulation Models (EAMs) to study individual differences and group effects. This endeavor is challenging because fitting EAMs requires constraining one of the EAM parameters to be equal for all participants, which makes a strong and possibly unlikely assumption. Moreover, if this assumption is violated, differences or lack thereof may be wrongly found. To overcome this limitation, in this study, we introduce a new method that was originally suggested by van Maanen & Miletić (2021), which employs Bayesian hierarchical estimation. In this new method, we set the scale at the population level, thereby allowing for individual and group differences, which is realized by <i>de facto</i> fixing a population-level hyper-parameter through its priors. As proof of concept, we ran two successful parameter recovery studies using the Linear Ballistic Accumulation model. The results suggest that the new method can be reliably used to study individual and group differences using EAMs. We further show a case in which the new method reveals the true group differences whereas the classic method wrongly detects differences that are truly absent.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"65"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11328677/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142000840","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Web-based Open-access Reliable Decision on Synonyms (WORDS) English Vocabulary Test. 基于网络的开放式同义词可靠判定 (WORDS) 英语词汇量测试。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-29 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.391
Po-Heng Chen, Rachael C Hulme, Lena M Blott, Jennifer M Rodd

A quick and reliable test of vocabulary knowledge is a vital component of many studies looking at a range of language processing skills. Recent proliferation of online (web-based) research has generated a growing need for reliable open-access vocabulary tests that can be administered online. This data report presents the newly developed 30-item Web-based Open-access Reliable Decision on Synonyms (WORDS) English Vocabulary Test. In Experiment 1, we tested 109 participants (age range: 18-69 years) on an initial set of 51 potential multiple-choice test items in which participants read a target word (e.g., ubiquitous) and selected a (near-)synonym (e.g., omnipresent) from among three semantically unrelated foils (e.g., interpersonal, catatonic, voluminous). We conducted an item response theory analysis of participants' accuracy data to select an optimal subset of 30 items to include in the final version of the test. In Experiment 2, we verified the reliability of this 30-item version in a different sample (N = 121; 18-79 years); reliability (internal consistency) was good (Cronbach's α = 0.82). We confirmed that, consistent with numerous previous studies, (1) responses were more accurate and quicker for more frequent compared to less frequent words, and (2) older adults showed greater vocabulary knowledge compared to younger adults. The WORDS test takes on average 4 minutes (5 minutes 40 seconds including consent/debrief) to complete. It can be freely accessed via Gorilla Open Materials (https://app.gorilla.sc/openmaterials/694887), allowing ease of use and for useful comparisons between data collected by different researchers.

快速可靠的词汇知识测试是许多语言处理技能研究的重要组成部分。近年来,在线(基于网络)研究日益增多,因此对可在线进行的可靠的开放式词汇测试的需求也越来越大。本数据报告介绍了新开发的 30 个项目的基于网络的同义词可靠决策(WORDS)英语词汇测试。在实验 1 中,我们对 109 名参与者(年龄范围:18-69 岁)进行了测试,测试的初始题目是 51 个潜在的多项选择测试项目,参与者阅读一个目标词(如:ubiquitous),并从三个语义无关的陪衬词(如:interpersonal、catatonic、voluminous)中选择一个(近)同义词(如:omnipresent)。我们对受试者的准确率数据进行了项目反应理论分析,以选出最佳的 30 个项目子集,并将其纳入测试的最终版本。在实验 2 中,我们在不同的样本(样本数 = 121;18-79 岁)中验证了这 30 个项目版本的可靠性;可靠性(内部一致性)良好(克朗巴赫系数 α = 0.82)。我们证实,与之前的大量研究结果一致,(1) 与词频较低的人相比,词频较高的人的回答更准确、更迅速;(2) 与年轻人相比,老年人的词汇知识更丰富。完成 WORDS 测试平均需要 4 分钟(包括同意/简述在内为 5 分 40 秒)。该测试可通过 Gorilla Open Materials (https://app.gorilla.sc/openmaterials/694887) 免费获取,使用方便,并可对不同研究人员收集的数据进行有用的比较。
{"title":"The Web-based Open-access Reliable Decision on Synonyms (WORDS) English Vocabulary Test.","authors":"Po-Heng Chen, Rachael C Hulme, Lena M Blott, Jennifer M Rodd","doi":"10.5334/joc.391","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.391","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A quick and reliable test of vocabulary knowledge is a vital component of many studies looking at a range of language processing skills. Recent proliferation of online (web-based) research has generated a growing need for reliable open-access vocabulary tests that can be administered online. This data report presents the newly developed 30-item Web-based Open-access Reliable Decision on Synonyms (WORDS) English Vocabulary Test. In Experiment 1, we tested 109 participants (age range: 18-69 years) on an initial set of 51 potential multiple-choice test items in which participants read a target word (e.g., <i>ubiquitous</i>) and selected a (near-)synonym (e.g., <i>omnipresent</i>) from among three semantically unrelated foils (e.g., <i>interpersonal, catatonic, voluminous</i>). We conducted an item response theory analysis of participants' accuracy data to select an optimal subset of 30 items to include in the final version of the test. In Experiment 2, we verified the reliability of this 30-item version in a different sample (<i>N</i> = 121; 18-79 years); reliability (internal consistency) was good (Cronbach's α = 0.82). We confirmed that, consistent with numerous previous studies, (1) responses were more accurate and quicker for more frequent compared to less frequent words, and (2) older adults showed greater vocabulary knowledge compared to younger adults. The WORDS test takes on average 4 minutes (5 minutes 40 seconds including consent/debrief) to complete. It can be freely accessed via Gorilla Open Materials (https://app.gorilla.sc/openmaterials/694887), allowing ease of use and for useful comparisons between data collected by different researchers.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"64"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11295906/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141890265","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Subjective Understanding is Reduced by Mechanistic Framing. 主观理解因机制框架而减弱。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-24 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.393
Jeffrey C Zemla, Daniel Corral

People often believe that they have a good understanding of how devices work (e.g., how a ballpoint pen works), despite having poor knowledge of their internal mechanics. We hypothesized that this bias occurs in part because people conflate mechanistic understanding with functional understanding of how devices work (e.g., how to operate a ballpoint pen). In two experiments, we found that increasing the salience of mechanistic information led to lower judgments of understanding for how devices work. In Experiment 1, we did this by showing participants either the internal parts of a device or an external, whole-object view of that same device. Those who saw the internal parts rated their understanding as less than those who saw a whole-object view. In Experiment 2, we removed the pictures and instead tested participants (without feedback) on their mechanistic or functional knowledge using true-or-false questions. Those who were tested on mechanistic knowledge rated their understanding of devices as less than those who were tested on functional knowledge.

尽管人们对设备的内部构造知之甚少,但他们往往认为自己对设备的工作原理(如圆珠笔的工作原理)了如指掌。我们假设,之所以会出现这种偏差,部分原因是人们将机械理解与对设备工作原理(例如,如何操作圆珠笔)的功能理解混为一谈。在两个实验中,我们发现提高机械信息的显著性会降低对设备工作原理的理解判断。在实验 1 中,我们向参与者展示了设备的内部零件或同一设备的外部整体视图。看到内部零件的参与者对其理解程度的评价低于看到整体物体视图的参与者。在实验 2 中,我们去掉了图片,改用真或假问题对参与者的机械或功能知识进行测试(无反馈)。接受机械知识测试的人对装置理解的评价低于接受功能知识测试的人。
{"title":"Subjective Understanding is Reduced by Mechanistic Framing.","authors":"Jeffrey C Zemla, Daniel Corral","doi":"10.5334/joc.393","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.393","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often believe that they have a good understanding of how devices work (e.g., how a ballpoint pen works), despite having poor knowledge of their internal mechanics. We hypothesized that this bias occurs in part because people conflate mechanistic understanding with functional understanding of how devices work (e.g., how to operate a ballpoint pen). In two experiments, we found that increasing the salience of mechanistic information led to lower judgments of understanding for how devices work. In Experiment 1, we did this by showing participants either the internal parts of a device or an external, whole-object view of that same device. Those who saw the internal parts rated their understanding as less than those who saw a whole-object view. In Experiment 2, we removed the pictures and instead tested participants (without feedback) on their mechanistic or functional knowledge using true-or-false questions. Those who were tested on mechanistic knowledge rated their understanding of devices as less than those who were tested on functional knowledge.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"63"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11276545/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789326","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evidence Against Effects of Cultural Group and Prior Knowledge on Feature Binding in Working Memory. 证据表明,文化群体和先验知识对工作记忆中的特征绑定没有影响。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-23 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.390
Hiu Wah Cheung, Nicolas Geeraert, Vanessa M Loaiza

Feature binding is the process of integrating features, such as colour and shape, into object representations. A persistent question in the literature concerning whether feature binding is an automatic or resource-demanding process may depend on unitisation, that is, whether the to-be-bound information is intrinsic (belonging to) or extrinsic (contextual). Given extensive evidence showing that Easterners may process information more holistically than Westerners, such cultural differences may be useful to understand the fundamental processes of feature binding in visual working memory (WM). Accordingly, we recruited British and Chinese participants to complete a visual WM task wherein to-be-remembered colours were integrated within (i.e., intrinsic binding) or as backgrounds (i.e., extrinsic binding) of to-be-remembered shapes (Experiments 1 and 2). Experiment 2 further investigated the role of prior knowledge in long-term memory to facilitate feature binding in WM. During retrieval, participants decided among three probes: a target, a lure (i.e., recombination of the presented features), and a new colour/shape. Hierarchical Bayesian multinomial processing tree models were fit to the data to estimate parameters representing binding and item memory. The current results suggest that intrinsic and extrinsic binding memory are similar between the two cultural groups, with no prior knowledge benefits for either intrinsic or extrinsic binding for either cultural group. This result conflicts with the Analytic and Holistic framework and suggests that there are no cultural differences or prior knowledge benefits in feature binding.

特征绑定是将颜色和形状等特征整合到对象表征中的过程。文献中一直存在的一个问题是,特征绑定是一个自动过程还是一个需要资源的过程,这可能取决于单元化,即要绑定的信息是内在的(属于)还是外在的(上下文)。有大量证据表明,东方人在处理信息时可能比西方人更注重整体性,因此这种文化差异可能有助于理解视觉工作记忆(WM)中特征绑定的基本过程。因此,我们招募了英国和中国的参与者来完成一项视觉工作记忆任务,在这项任务中,待记忆的颜色被整合到待记忆形状的内部(即内在结合)或背景中(即外在结合)(实验 1 和 2)。实验 2 进一步研究了长时记忆中的先验知识对促进 WM 中特征结合的作用。在检索过程中,参与者在三个探针中做出决定:一个目标、一个诱饵(即呈现特征的重组)和一个新的颜色/形状。分层贝叶斯多项式加工树模型被拟合到数据中,以估计代表绑定和项目记忆的参数。目前的结果表明,两个文化群体之间的内在和外在绑定记忆相似,无论是内在绑定还是外在绑定,对任何一个文化群体都没有先验知识的益处。这一结果与分析和整体框架相冲突,表明在特征绑定方面不存在文化差异或先验知识优势。
{"title":"Evidence Against Effects of Cultural Group and Prior Knowledge on Feature Binding in Working Memory.","authors":"Hiu Wah Cheung, Nicolas Geeraert, Vanessa M Loaiza","doi":"10.5334/joc.390","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.390","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Feature binding is the process of integrating features, such as colour and shape, into object representations. A persistent question in the literature concerning whether feature binding is an automatic or resource-demanding process may depend on unitisation, that is, whether the to-be-bound information is intrinsic (belonging to) or extrinsic (contextual). Given extensive evidence showing that Easterners may process information more holistically than Westerners, such cultural differences may be useful to understand the fundamental processes of feature binding in visual working memory (WM). Accordingly, we recruited British and Chinese participants to complete a visual WM task wherein to-be-remembered colours were integrated within (i.e., intrinsic binding) or as backgrounds (i.e., extrinsic binding) of to-be-remembered shapes (Experiments 1 and 2). Experiment 2 further investigated the role of prior knowledge in long-term memory to facilitate feature binding in WM. During retrieval, participants decided among three probes: a target, a lure (i.e., recombination of the presented features), and a new colour/shape. Hierarchical Bayesian multinomial processing tree models were fit to the data to estimate parameters representing binding and item memory. The current results suggest that intrinsic and extrinsic binding memory are similar between the two cultural groups, with no prior knowledge benefits for either intrinsic or extrinsic binding for either cultural group. This result conflicts with the Analytic and Holistic framework and suggests that there are no cultural differences or prior knowledge benefits in feature binding.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"62"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11276497/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789325","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Breathing Life Into Meta-Analytic Methods. 为 Meta 分析方法注入活力。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-22 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.389
David Allbritton, Pablo Gómez, Bernhard Angele, Martin Vasilev, Manuel Perea

Meta-analyses have become indispensable in the behavioral sciences, combining and summarizing data from multiple studies. While they offer many advantages (e.g., increased power, higher generality, and resolving conflicting findings), they currently only provide a snapshot at a given point. In active research areas, frequent meta-analytic updates are necessary to incorporate new evidence. We propose guidelines for live, dynamic meta-analyses and introduce an accessible tool using the R environment. Our app, powered by the Shiny package, enables the meta-analyst to integrate evidence interactively as an update of an existing meta-analysis or from scratch (i.e., a new meta-analysis). By embracing dynamic meta-analyses and leveraging modern tools, researchers can ensure up-to-date meta-analyses in their respective fields.

元分析已成为行为科学领域不可或缺的工具,它综合并概括了多项研究的数据。虽然元分析具有很多优势(例如,提高分析能力、增强通用性以及解决相互矛盾的研究结果),但目前元分析只能提供特定时间点的快照。在活跃的研究领域,有必要经常更新荟萃分析以纳入新的证据。我们提出了实时动态荟萃分析的指导原则,并介绍了一种使用 R 环境的易用工具。我们的应用程序由 Shiny 软件包提供支持,使元分析师能够以交互方式整合证据,既可以更新现有的元分析,也可以从头开始(即新的元分析)。通过采用动态荟萃分析和利用现代工具,研究人员可以确保各自领域的荟萃分析都是最新的。
{"title":"Breathing Life Into Meta-Analytic Methods.","authors":"David Allbritton, Pablo Gómez, Bernhard Angele, Martin Vasilev, Manuel Perea","doi":"10.5334/joc.389","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.389","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Meta-analyses have become indispensable in the behavioral sciences, combining and summarizing data from multiple studies. While they offer many advantages (e.g., increased power, higher generality, and resolving conflicting findings), they currently only provide a snapshot at a given point. In active research areas, frequent meta-analytic updates are necessary to incorporate new evidence. We propose guidelines for live, dynamic meta-analyses and introduce an accessible tool using the R environment. Our app, powered by the Shiny package, enables the meta-analyst to integrate evidence interactively as an update of an existing meta-analysis or from scratch (i.e., a new meta-analysis). By embracing dynamic meta-analyses and leveraging modern tools, researchers can ensure up-to-date meta-analyses in their respective fields.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"61"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11276543/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141789313","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relation Between Subjective and Objective Measures of Visual Awareness: Current Evidence, Attempt of a Synthesis and Future Research Directions. 视觉意识的主观测量与客观测量之间的关系:当前证据、综合尝试和未来研究方向。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-18 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.381
Markus Kiefer, Thomas Kammer

Within the realm of consciousness research, different methods of measuring the content of visual awareness are used: On the one hand, subjective measures require a report of sensory experiences related to a stimulus. On the other hand, objective measures rely on the observer's performance to accurately detect or discriminate the stimulus. The most appropriate measure of awareness is currently debated. To contribute to this debate, we review findings on the relation between subjective and objective measures of awareness. Although subjective measures sometimes lag behind objective measures, a substantial number of studies demonstrates a convergence of measures. Based on the reviewed studies, we identify five aspects relevant for achieving a convergence of measures. Future research could then identify and empirically test the boundary conditions, under which a convergence or divergence of subjective and measures of awareness is observed.

在意识研究领域,使用了不同的方法来测量视觉意识的内容:一方面,主观测量需要报告与刺激相关的感觉体验。另一方面,客观测量依赖于观察者准确检测或辨别刺激物的表现。目前,关于意识的最合适测量方法还存在争议。为了对这一争论有所贡献,我们回顾了有关意识的主观测量和客观测量之间关系的研究结果。尽管主观测量有时会落后于客观测量,但大量研究表明测量结果趋于一致。根据所回顾的研究,我们确定了与实现衡量标准趋同相关的五个方面。未来的研究可以确定并通过实证检验边界条件,在这些边界条件下,主观测量和意识测量可以趋同或相异。
{"title":"The Relation Between Subjective and Objective Measures of Visual Awareness: Current Evidence, Attempt of a Synthesis and Future Research Directions.","authors":"Markus Kiefer, Thomas Kammer","doi":"10.5334/joc.381","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.381","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Within the realm of consciousness research, different methods of measuring the content of visual awareness are used: On the one hand, subjective measures require a report of sensory experiences related to a stimulus. On the other hand, objective measures rely on the observer's performance to accurately detect or discriminate the stimulus. The most appropriate measure of awareness is currently debated. To contribute to this debate, we review findings on the relation between subjective and objective measures of awareness. Although subjective measures sometimes lag behind objective measures, a substantial number of studies demonstrates a convergence of measures. Based on the reviewed studies, we identify five aspects relevant for achieving a convergence of measures. Future research could then identify and empirically test the boundary conditions, under which a convergence or divergence of subjective and measures of awareness is observed.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"59"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11259121/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735200","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Risk and Protective Factors for Executive Function in Vulnerable South African Preschool-Age Children. 南非学龄前弱势儿童执行功能的风险和保护因素。
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-18 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.377
Caylee J Cook, Steven J Howard, Hleliwe Makaula, Rebecca Merkley, Mbulelo Mshudulu, Nosibusiso Tshetu, Gaia Scerif, Catherine E Draper

Executive function (EF) theory and research continues to under-represent the contexts in which the majority of the world's children reside, despite their potential to support, refute, or refine our current understandings. The current study sought to contribute to our understanding of EF in low-income settings in South Africa by investigating longitudinal associations of context-specific risk and protective factors for EF development in three- to five-year-old children who had limited access to ECCE services before the age of five. Child-caregiver dyads (N = 171) participated in two rounds of data collection (approximately seven months apart) during which child EF was assessed using the Early Years Toolbox; context-specific risk and protective factors were assessed through a caregiver questionnaire. Hierarchical linear regressions revealed that after controlling for age, attending ECCE services at time 2 (β = 0.30, p < 0.001), and diversity of caregivers at time 1 (β = 0.14, p = 0.041) were the only factors positively associated with EF at time 2. Other factors commonly associated with EF such as caregiver education, and household income were not significant, while resources in the home were significantly associated with EF (β = -0.18, p = 0.007) but in the opposite direction to what was expected. These results add to accumulating evidence that predictors of EF established in Minority World contexts may not be consistent across contexts, emphasising the need to broaden the EF evidence base. For instance, future studies could incorporate qualitative and ethnographic methods to better capture the cultural and contextual nuances relating to EF, to better inform our statistical and theoretical models.

尽管执行功能(EF)理论和研究具有支持、反驳或完善我们现有认识的潜力,但它们对世界上大多数儿童所处环境的代表性仍然不足。目前的研究试图通过调查特定环境下的风险和保护因素与南非低收入环境中英孚发展的纵向联系,来帮助我们了解五岁前接受幼儿保育和教育服务机会有限的三至五岁儿童的英孚发展情况。儿童-照顾者二人组(N = 171)参加了两轮数据收集(相隔约七个月),在此期间,使用幼儿工具箱对儿童的幼儿期发展进行了评估;通过照顾者问卷对特定环境的风险和保护因素进行了评估。层次线性回归结果显示,在控制年龄后,第 2 时间段参加幼儿保育和教育服务(β = 0.30,p < 0.001)和第 1 时间段照顾者的多样性(β = 0.14,p = 0.041)是唯一与第 2 时间段幼儿教育能力呈正相关的因素。其他通常与 EF 相关的因素,如照顾者的教育程度和家庭收入并不显著,而家庭资源与 EF 显著相关(β = -0.18,p = 0.007),但与预期方向相反。这些结果补充了不断积累的证据,即在少数世界背景下建立的预测幼儿成长的因素在不同背景下可能并不一致,这强调了扩大幼儿成长证据基础的必要性。例如,未来的研究可以纳入定性和人种学方法,以更好地捕捉与EF相关的文化和环境细微差别,从而更好地为我们的统计和理论模型提供信息。
{"title":"Risk and Protective Factors for Executive Function in Vulnerable South African Preschool-Age Children.","authors":"Caylee J Cook, Steven J Howard, Hleliwe Makaula, Rebecca Merkley, Mbulelo Mshudulu, Nosibusiso Tshetu, Gaia Scerif, Catherine E Draper","doi":"10.5334/joc.377","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.377","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Executive function (EF) theory and research continues to under-represent the contexts in which the majority of the world's children reside, despite their potential to support, refute, or refine our current understandings. The current study sought to contribute to our understanding of EF in low-income settings in South Africa by investigating longitudinal associations of context-specific risk and protective factors for EF development in three- to five-year-old children who had limited access to ECCE services before the age of five. Child-caregiver dyads (N = 171) participated in two rounds of data collection (approximately seven months apart) during which child EF was assessed using the Early Years Toolbox; context-specific risk and protective factors were assessed through a caregiver questionnaire. Hierarchical linear regressions revealed that after controlling for age, attending ECCE services at time 2 (<i>β = 0.30, p < 0.001</i>), and diversity of caregivers at time 1 (<i>β = 0.14, p = 0.041</i>) were the only factors positively associated with EF at time 2. Other factors commonly associated with EF such as caregiver education, and household income were not significant, while resources in the home were significantly associated with EF (<i>β = -0.18, p = 0.007</i>) but in the opposite direction to what was expected. These results add to accumulating evidence that predictors of EF established in Minority World contexts may not be consistent across contexts, emphasising the need to broaden the EF evidence base. For instance, future studies could incorporate qualitative and ethnographic methods to better capture the cultural and contextual nuances relating to EF, to better inform our statistical and theoretical models.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"58"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11259110/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Modelling Working Memory Capacity: Is the Magical Number Four, Seven, or Does it Depend on What You Are Counting? 工作记忆能力建模:神奇数字是 "四"、"七",还是取决于你在数什么?
Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-07-18 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.5334/joc.387
Sergio Morra, Paola Patella, Lorenzo Muscella

Limited attentional capacity is essential to working memory. How its limit should be assessed is a debated issue. Five experiments compare Cowan's 4-units and Pascual-Leone's 7-units models of limited working memory capacity, with presentation time and attention to operative schemes as potential explanations of this discrepancy. Experiments 1a-1c used the Compound Stimuli Visual Information (CSVI) task, with long versus brief presentation. Capacity was estimated with the Bose-Einstein model, assuming a different number of attending acts in each condition. Participants' k estimates in both conditions were highly correlated and the means were not different, indicating that the same capacity is assessed in both conditions. Experiments 2 and 3 used the 5000-msec CSVI, and the Visual Array Task (VAT) in two conditions (5000- vs. 120-msec presentation). Capacity in the VAT was estimated with Morey's Bayesian method. Participants' k estimates in both VAT conditions were correlated, but the mean was higher with long presentation, suggesting that the long condition benefits from recoding or chunking. The k estimate in the CSVI correlated with the short VAT and (to a lesser degree in Exp.2) with the long VAT. The mean estimate of k in the CSVI was one unit more than in the short VAT. We conclude that the CSVI and the short VAT tap the same capacity, one unit of which in the short VAT is allocated to an operative scheme; we discuss how Cowan's and Pascual-Leone's views on limited capacity can be reconciled.

有限的注意能力对工作记忆至关重要。如何评估其极限是一个有争议的问题。五项实验比较了考恩的 4 个单位和帕斯卡尔-莱昂的 7 个单位工作记忆能力有限模型,并将呈现时间和对操作方案的注意作为这一差异的潜在解释。实验 1a-1c 采用的是复合刺激视觉信息(CSVI)任务,呈现时间有长有短。容量是通过玻色-爱因斯坦模型估算的,假设在每种条件下都有不同数量的注意行为。参与者在两种条件下的 k 估计值高度相关,且平均值没有差异,这表明在两种条件下评估的容量是相同的。实验 2 和 3 使用了 5000 毫秒的 CSVI 和视觉阵列任务(VAT)两种条件(5000 毫秒和 120 毫秒)。VAT 中的容量是用莫雷贝叶斯方法估算的。参与者在两种 VAT 条件下的 k 估计值是相关的,但长时间呈现时的平均值更高,这表明长时间呈现条件下的 k 估计值得益于重新编码或分块。CSVI 中的 k 估计值与短 VAT 相关,与长 VAT 的相关程度较低(在 Exp.2 中)。CSVI 中 k 的平均估计值比短 VAT 中多一个单位。我们的结论是,CSVI 和短增值税挖掘了相同的容量,其中短增值税中的一个单位分配给了一个操作方案;我们讨论了如何协调 Cowan 和 Pascual-Leone 关于有限容量的观点。
{"title":"Modelling Working Memory Capacity: Is the Magical Number Four, Seven, or Does it Depend on What You Are Counting?","authors":"Sergio Morra, Paola Patella, Lorenzo Muscella","doi":"10.5334/joc.387","DOIUrl":"10.5334/joc.387","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Limited attentional capacity is essential to working memory. How its limit should be assessed is a debated issue. Five experiments compare Cowan's 4-units and Pascual-Leone's 7-units models of limited working memory capacity, with presentation time and attention to operative schemes as potential explanations of this discrepancy. Experiments 1a-1c used the Compound Stimuli Visual Information (CSVI) task, with long versus brief presentation. Capacity was estimated with the Bose-Einstein model, assuming a different number of attending acts in each condition. Participants' <i>k</i> estimates in both conditions were highly correlated and the means were not different, indicating that the same capacity is assessed in both conditions. Experiments 2 and 3 used the 5000-msec CSVI, and the Visual Array Task (VAT) in two conditions (5000- vs. 120-msec presentation). Capacity in the VAT was estimated with Morey's Bayesian method. Participants' <i>k</i> estimates in both VAT conditions were correlated, but the mean was higher with long presentation, suggesting that the long condition benefits from recoding or chunking. The <i>k</i> estimate in the CSVI correlated with the short VAT and (to a lesser degree in Exp.2) with the long VAT. The mean estimate of <i>k</i> in the CSVI was one unit more than in the short VAT. We conclude that the CSVI and the short VAT tap the same capacity, one unit of which in the short VAT is allocated to an operative scheme; we discuss how Cowan's and Pascual-Leone's views on limited capacity can be reconciled.</p>","PeriodicalId":32728,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Cognition","volume":"7 1","pages":"60"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11259112/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735197","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1