首页 > 最新文献

Neurobiology of Language最新文献

英文 中文
Connectivity of fronto-temporal regions in syntactic structure building during speaking and listening 口语和听力过程中句法结构构建过程中的颞前区连通性
IF 3.6 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00154
Laura Giglio, D. Sharoh, M. Ostarek, Peter Hagoort
The neural infrastructure for sentence production and comprehension has been found to be mostly shared. The same regions are engaged during speaking and listening, with some differences in how strongly they activate depending on modality. In this study, we investigated how modality affects the connectivity between regions previously found to be involved in syntactic processing across modalities. We determined how constituent size and modality affected the connectivity of the pars triangularis of the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and of the left posterior temporal lobe (LPTL) with the pars opercularis of the LIFG, the anterior temporal lobe (LATL) and the rest of the brain. We found that constituent size reliably increased the connectivity across these frontal and temporal ROIs. Connectivity between the two LIFG regions and the LPTL was enhanced as a function of constituent size in both modalities, and it was upregulated in production possibly because of linearization and motor planning in the frontal cortex. The connectivity of both ROIs with the LATL was lower and only enhanced for larger constituent sizes, suggesting a contributing role of the LATL in sentence processing in both modalities. These results thus show that the connectivity among fronto-temporal regions is upregulated for syntactic structure building in both sentence production and comprehension, providing further evidence for accounts of shared neural resources for sentence-level processing across modalities.
研究发现,句子生成和理解的神经基础结构大部分是共享的。在口语和听力过程中,同样的区域会被激活,但根据模态的不同,它们的激活程度也会有所不同。在本研究中,我们研究了模式如何影响之前发现的跨模式句法处理区域之间的连接。我们确定了成分大小和模式如何影响左侧额叶下回(LIFG)三角旁和左侧颞叶后部(LPTL)与左侧额叶下回三角旁、颞叶前部(LATL)和大脑其他部分的连接。我们发现,成分大小可靠地增加了这些额叶和颞叶 ROI 之间的连接性。在两种模式中,两个 LIFG 区域和 LPTL 之间的连通性都随着成分大小的变化而增强,而且在制作过程中,可能由于额叶皮层的线性化和运动规划,连通性也会增强。这两个 ROI 与 LATL 的连接性较低,只有在成分大小较大时才会增强,这表明 LATL 在两种模态的句子处理中都起到了促进作用。这些结果表明,在句子生成和理解的句法结构构建过程中,前颞区之间的连通性都会被上调,这为跨模态句子级处理的神经资源共享提供了进一步的证据。
{"title":"Connectivity of fronto-temporal regions in syntactic structure building during speaking and listening","authors":"Laura Giglio, D. Sharoh, M. Ostarek, Peter Hagoort","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00154","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00154","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The neural infrastructure for sentence production and comprehension has been found to be mostly shared. The same regions are engaged during speaking and listening, with some differences in how strongly they activate depending on modality. In this study, we investigated how modality affects the connectivity between regions previously found to be involved in syntactic processing across modalities. We determined how constituent size and modality affected the connectivity of the pars triangularis of the left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG) and of the left posterior temporal lobe (LPTL) with the pars opercularis of the LIFG, the anterior temporal lobe (LATL) and the rest of the brain. We found that constituent size reliably increased the connectivity across these frontal and temporal ROIs. Connectivity between the two LIFG regions and the LPTL was enhanced as a function of constituent size in both modalities, and it was upregulated in production possibly because of linearization and motor planning in the frontal cortex. The connectivity of both ROIs with the LATL was lower and only enhanced for larger constituent sizes, suggesting a contributing role of the LATL in sentence processing in both modalities. These results thus show that the connectivity among fronto-temporal regions is upregulated for syntactic structure building in both sentence production and comprehension, providing further evidence for accounts of shared neural resources for sentence-level processing across modalities.","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141824988","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Small but mighty: Ten myths and misunderstandings about the cerebellum 小而强大关于小脑的十大神话和误解
IF 3.6 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-07-03 DOI: 10.1162/nol_e_00152
J. Fiez, Catherine J. Stoodley
This special issue of Neurobiology of Language focuses on the role of the cerebellum in spoken and written language comprehension and production. The volume brings together behavioral and neural evidence bearing upon this question using an array of methods. As editors, we are excited by the collective impact of this work, which includes recent findings from many of the leading researchers who study the cerebellum and language. We also find ourselves pondering the term “special” as a reflection of the widespread tendency of brain researchers to comfortably relegate the cerebellum to a minor role in cognition. As a result, our 21st-century understanding of the cognitive neuroscience of the cerebellum is not yet consistently recognized by the field, leading to an under-appreciation of the cerebellar contributions to language beyond its role in the coordination of articulation. Here we offer a “top ten” list aimed at countering some of the myths and misunderstandings that keep it out of the limelight.
本期《语言神经生物学》特刊重点关注小脑在口语和书面语言理解与生成中的作用。本期特刊通过一系列方法汇集了与这一问题相关的行为和神经证据。作为编辑,我们对这项工作的集体影响感到兴奋,其中包括许多研究小脑和语言的顶尖研究人员的最新研究成果。我们也发现自己在思考 "特殊 "这个词,它反映了大脑研究人员普遍存在的一种倾向,即认为小脑在认知中的作用微不足道。因此,我们在 21 世纪对小脑认知神经科学的理解尚未得到该领域的一致认可,导致人们对小脑在协调发音之外对语言的贡献认识不足。在此,我们列出了 "十大 "清单,旨在反驳一些让小脑远离人们视线的神话和误解。
{"title":"Small but mighty: Ten myths and misunderstandings about the cerebellum","authors":"J. Fiez, Catherine J. Stoodley","doi":"10.1162/nol_e_00152","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_e_00152","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This special issue of Neurobiology of Language focuses on the role of the cerebellum in spoken and written language comprehension and production. The volume brings together behavioral and neural evidence bearing upon this question using an array of methods. As editors, we are excited by the collective impact of this work, which includes recent findings from many of the leading researchers who study the cerebellum and language. We also find ourselves pondering the term “special” as a reflection of the widespread tendency of brain researchers to comfortably relegate the cerebellum to a minor role in cognition. As a result, our 21st-century understanding of the cognitive neuroscience of the cerebellum is not yet consistently recognized by the field, leading to an under-appreciation of the cerebellar contributions to language beyond its role in the coordination of articulation. Here we offer a “top ten” list aimed at countering some of the myths and misunderstandings that keep it out of the limelight.","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2024-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141683560","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can the mismatch negativity really be elicited by abstract linguistic contrasts? 抽象的语言对比真的能引起错配否定吗?
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00147
Stephen Politzer-Ahles, B. Jap
The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an ERP component that reflects pre-attentive change detection in the brain. As an electrophysiological index of processing that responds to differences in incoming consecutive stimuli, the MMN can be elicited through, for example, the presentation of two different categories of sounds in an oddball paradigm where sounds from the "standard" category occur frequently and sounds from the "deviant" category occur rarely. The specificity of what can elicit the MMN is yet to be fully defined. Here we test whether the MMN can be generated by an abstract linguistic contrast with no reliable acoustic cue. Previous studies have shown that the way in which an acoustic cue is used to elicit MMN is influenced by linguistic knowledge, but have not shown that a non-acoustic, abstract linguistic contrast can itself elicit MMN. In this study, we test the strongest interpretation of the claim that the MMN can be generated through a purely linguistic contrast, by contrasting tenses in ablauting irregular English verbs (where there is no reliable acoustic cue for tense). We find that this contrast elicits a negativity, as do other linguistic contrasts previously shown to elicit MMN. The findings provide evidence that the MMN is indeed sensitive to purely abstract linguistic categories.
错配负性(MMN)是一种ERP成分,它反映了大脑中的前注意变化检测。作为对连续输入刺激的差异做出反应的处理过程的电生理指标,MMN 可以通过以下方式激发:例如,在一个怪人范例中呈现两种不同类别的声音,其中 "标准 "类别的声音经常出现,而 "偏差 "类别的声音很少出现。目前还没有完全确定哪些因素可以诱发MMN。在这里,我们测试了抽象的语言对比是否能产生 MMN,而这种对比并没有可靠的声音线索。以往的研究表明,使用声音线索来诱发 MMN 的方式会受到语言知识的影响,但并没有表明非声音的抽象语言对比本身可以诱发 MMN。在本研究中,我们通过对比不规则英语动词的时态(没有可靠的时态声学线索),检验了对 "MMN 可以通过纯粹的语言对比产生 "这一说法的最有力解释。我们发现,这种对比会引起一种否定性,就像之前被证明能引起 MMN 的其他语言对比一样。研究结果证明,MMN 的确对纯粹抽象的语言类别很敏感。
{"title":"Can the mismatch negativity really be elicited by abstract linguistic contrasts?","authors":"Stephen Politzer-Ahles, B. Jap","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00147","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00147","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an ERP component that reflects pre-attentive change detection in the brain. As an electrophysiological index of processing that responds to differences in incoming consecutive stimuli, the MMN can be elicited through, for example, the presentation of two different categories of sounds in an oddball paradigm where sounds from the \"standard\" category occur frequently and sounds from the \"deviant\" category occur rarely. The specificity of what can elicit the MMN is yet to be fully defined. Here we test whether the MMN can be generated by an abstract linguistic contrast with no reliable acoustic cue. Previous studies have shown that the way in which an acoustic cue is used to elicit MMN is influenced by linguistic knowledge, but have not shown that a non-acoustic, abstract linguistic contrast can itself elicit MMN. In this study, we test the strongest interpretation of the claim that the MMN can be generated through a purely linguistic contrast, by contrasting tenses in ablauting irregular English verbs (where there is no reliable acoustic cue for tense). We find that this contrast elicits a negativity, as do other linguistic contrasts previously shown to elicit MMN. The findings provide evidence that the MMN is indeed sensitive to purely abstract linguistic categories.","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141362893","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Subword representations successfully decode brain responses to morphologically complex written words 子词表征成功解码大脑对形态复杂的书面词语的反应
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00149
Tero Hakala, Tiina Lindh-Knuutila, Annika Hultén, Minna Lehtonen, R. Salmelin
This study extends the idea of decoding word-evoked brain activations using a corpus-semantic vector space to multimorphemic words in the agglutinative Finnish language. The corpus-semantic models are trained on word segments, and decoding is carried out with word vectors that are composed of these segments. We tested several alternative vector-space models using different segmentations: no segmentation (whole word), linguistic morphemes, statistical morphemes, random segmentation, and character-level 1-, 2- and 3-grams, and paired them with recorded MEG responses to multimorphemic words in a visual word recognition task. For all variants, the decoding accuracy exceeded the standard word-label permutation-based significance thresholds at 350--500 ms after stimulus onset. However, the critical segment-label permutation test revealed that only those segmentations that were morphologically aware reached significance in the brain decoding task. The results suggest that both whole-word forms and morphemes are represented in the brain and show that neural decoding using corpus-semantic word representations derived from compositional subword segments is applicable also for multimorphemic word forms. This is especially relevant for languages with complex morphology, because a large proportion of word forms are rare and it can be difficult to find statistically reliable surface representations for them in any large corpus.
这项研究将利用语料-语义向量空间对单词诱发的大脑激活进行解码的想法扩展到了多词素芬兰语中。语料库-语义模型在词段上进行训练,解码则通过由这些词段组成的词向量进行。我们使用不同的词段测试了几种可供选择的向量空间模型:无词段(整词)、语言词素、统计词素、随机词段以及字符级 1-、2-和 3-词素,并将它们与视觉单词识别任务中记录的多词素单词的 MEG 反应配对。对于所有变体,在刺激开始后350-500毫秒时,解码准确率都超过了基于词标签排列的标准显著性阈值。然而,临界词段标签排列测试表明,只有那些具有形态意识的词段才能在大脑解码任务中达到显著性。这些结果表明,全词形式和词素都能在大脑中得到表征,并表明使用由组成性子词片段衍生的语料库-语义词表征进行神经解码也适用于多词素词形式。这对于形态复杂的语言尤为重要,因为很大一部分词形是罕见的,很难在任何大型语料库中找到统计上可靠的表面表征。
{"title":"Subword representations successfully decode brain responses to morphologically complex written words","authors":"Tero Hakala, Tiina Lindh-Knuutila, Annika Hultén, Minna Lehtonen, R. Salmelin","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00149","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00149","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This study extends the idea of decoding word-evoked brain activations using a corpus-semantic vector space to multimorphemic words in the agglutinative Finnish language. The corpus-semantic models are trained on word segments, and decoding is carried out with word vectors that are composed of these segments. We tested several alternative vector-space models using different segmentations: no segmentation (whole word), linguistic morphemes, statistical morphemes, random segmentation, and character-level 1-, 2- and 3-grams, and paired them with recorded MEG responses to multimorphemic words in a visual word recognition task. For all variants, the decoding accuracy exceeded the standard word-label permutation-based significance thresholds at 350--500 ms after stimulus onset. However, the critical segment-label permutation test revealed that only those segmentations that were morphologically aware reached significance in the brain decoding task. The results suggest that both whole-word forms and morphemes are represented in the brain and show that neural decoding using corpus-semantic word representations derived from compositional subword segments is applicable also for multimorphemic word forms. This is especially relevant for languages with complex morphology, because a large proportion of word forms are rare and it can be difficult to find statistically reliable surface representations for them in any large corpus.","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141363774","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dissociating cerebellar regions involved in formulating and articulating words and sentences 分离小脑中参与构词和造句的区域
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-10 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00148
Oiwi Parker Jones, Sharon Geva, S. Prejawa, T. Hope, M. Oberhuber, Mohamed L. Seghier, David W. Green, Cathy J. Price
This fMRI study of healthy volunteers investigated which parts of the cerebellum are involved in formulating and articulating sentences using three sentence-based tasks: (i) a sentence production task that involved describing simple events in pictures (e.g. “The goat is eating the hat”); (ii) An auditory sentence repetition task involving the same sentence articulation but not sentence formulation, and (iii) An auditory sentence-topicture matching task that involved the same pictorial events and no overt articulation. Activation for each of these tasks was compared to the equivalent word processing tasks: noun production (object naming), verb production (naming the verb in pictorial events), auditory noun repetition, and auditory noun-to-picture matching. Auditory and visual semantic association tasks were also included, in the same within-subjects design, to control for visual and auditory working memory and semantic processing. Three distinct cerebellar regions were activated by sentence production compared to noun and verb production. First, we associate activation in bilateral cerebellum lobule VIIb with sequencing words into sentences, as well as phonemes into words because it increased for sentence production compared to all other conditions, including sentence repetition and sentence-to-picture matching; and was also activated by word production compared to word matching. Second, we associate a paravermal part of right cerebellar lobule VIIIb with overt motor execution of speech, because activation was higher during (i) production and repetition of sentences compared to the corresponding noun conditions, and (ii) noun and verb production compared to all matching tasks; with no activation relative to fixation during any silent (non-speaking) matching task. Third, we associate activation within right cerebellar Crus II with covert articulatory activity because it activated for (i) all speech production more than matching tasks, and (ii) sentences compared to nouns during silent (non-speaking) matching as well as sentence production and sentence repetition. As all three regions were activated during word production tasks, our study serendipitously segregated, for the first time, three distinct functional roles for the cerebellum in generic speech production; and demonstrates how sentence production enhanced the demands on these three cerebellar speech production regions.
这项针对健康志愿者的 fMRI 研究通过三项以句子为基础的任务调查了小脑的哪些部分参与了句子的表述和衔接:(i) 一项涉及描述图片中简单事件(如 "山羊在吃帽子")的造句任务;(ii) 一项涉及相同句子衔接但不涉及句子表述的听觉句子重复任务;以及 (iii) 一项涉及相同图片事件但不涉及明显衔接的听觉句子-图片匹配任务。每项任务的激活情况都与相应的文字处理任务进行了比较:名词制作(对象命名)、动词制作(在图像事件中命名动词)、听觉名词重复和听觉名词与图像匹配。听觉和视觉语义联想任务也包括在内,采用相同的被试内设计,以控制视觉和听觉工作记忆和语义处理。与名词和动词造句相比,造句激活了三个不同的小脑区域。首先,我们将双侧小脑第VIIb小叶的激活与单词到句子的排序以及音素到单词的排序联系起来,因为与所有其他条件(包括句子重复和句子到图片的匹配)相比,句子生成会增加小脑第VIIb小叶的激活;与单词匹配相比,单词生成也会激活小脑第VIIb小叶。其次,我们将右侧小脑第 VIIIb 小叶的边缘部分与言语的公开运动执行联系起来,因为(i) 与相应的名词条件相比,在造句和复述句子时,以及(ii) 与所有配对任务相比,在造名词和动词时,激活程度更高;而在任何无声(不说话)配对任务中,相对于固定点没有激活。第三,我们将右侧小脑第二簇的激活与隐蔽发音活动联系起来,因为它在以下情况下激活:(i) 在所有语音生成任务中比在匹配任务中激活更多,(ii) 在无声(不说话)匹配以及句子生成和句子重复过程中,句子比名词激活更多。由于在造词任务中这三个区域都被激活,我们的研究首次偶然分离出了小脑在一般言语生成中的三种不同功能作用;并证明了句子生成如何提高了对这三个小脑言语生成区域的要求。
{"title":"Dissociating cerebellar regions involved in formulating and articulating words and sentences","authors":"Oiwi Parker Jones, Sharon Geva, S. Prejawa, T. Hope, M. Oberhuber, Mohamed L. Seghier, David W. Green, Cathy J. Price","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00148","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00148","url":null,"abstract":"\u0000 This fMRI study of healthy volunteers investigated which parts of the cerebellum are involved in formulating and articulating sentences using three sentence-based tasks: (i) a sentence production task that involved describing simple events in pictures (e.g. “The goat is eating the hat”); (ii) An auditory sentence repetition task involving the same sentence articulation but not sentence formulation, and (iii) An auditory sentence-topicture matching task that involved the same pictorial events and no overt articulation. Activation for each of these tasks was compared to the equivalent word processing tasks: noun production (object naming), verb production (naming the verb in pictorial events), auditory noun repetition, and auditory noun-to-picture matching. Auditory and visual semantic association tasks were also included, in the same within-subjects design, to control for visual and auditory working memory and semantic processing.\u0000 Three distinct cerebellar regions were activated by sentence production compared to noun and verb production. First, we associate activation in bilateral cerebellum lobule VIIb with sequencing words into sentences, as well as phonemes into words because it increased for sentence production compared to all other conditions, including sentence repetition and sentence-to-picture matching; and was also activated by word production compared to word matching. Second, we associate a paravermal part of right cerebellar lobule VIIIb with overt motor execution of speech, because activation was higher during (i) production and repetition of sentences compared to the corresponding noun conditions, and (ii) noun and verb production compared to all matching tasks; with no activation relative to fixation during any silent (non-speaking) matching task. Third, we associate activation within right cerebellar Crus II with covert articulatory activity because it activated for (i) all speech production more than matching tasks, and (ii) sentences compared to nouns during silent (non-speaking) matching as well as sentence production and sentence repetition.\u0000 As all three regions were activated during word production tasks, our study serendipitously segregated, for the first time, three distinct functional roles for the cerebellum in generic speech production; and demonstrates how sentence production enhanced the demands on these three cerebellar speech production regions.","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141363625","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Tracking Components of Bilingual Language Control in Speech Production: An fMRI Study Using Functional Localizers. 跟踪语音生成中的双语语言控制成分:使用功能定位器的 fMRI 研究
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-03 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00128
Agata Wolna, Jakub Szewczyk, Michele Diaz, Aleksandra Domagalik, Marcin Szwed, Zofia Wodniecka

When bilingual speakers switch back to speaking in their native language (L1) after having used their second language (L2), they often experience difficulty in retrieving words in their L1. This phenomenon is referred to as the L2 after-effect. We used the L2 after-effect as a lens to explore the neural bases of bilingual language control mechanisms. Our goal was twofold: first, to explore whether bilingual language control draws on domain-general or language-specific mechanisms; second, to investigate the precise mechanism(s) that drive the L2 after-effect. We used a precision fMRI approach based on functional localizers to measure the extent to which the brain activity that reflects the L2 after-effect overlaps with the language network (Fedorenko et al., 2010) and the domain-general multiple demand network (Duncan, 2010), as well as three task-specific networks that tap into interference resolution, lexical retrieval, and articulation. Forty-two Polish-English bilinguals participated in the study. Our results show that the L2 after-effect reflects increased engagement of domain-general but not language-specific resources. Furthermore, contrary to previously proposed interpretations, we did not find evidence that the effect reflects increased difficulty related to lexical access, articulation, and the resolution of lexical interference. We propose that difficulty of speech production in the picture naming paradigm-manifested as the L2 after-effect-reflects interference at a nonlinguistic level of task schemas or a general increase of cognitive control engagement during speech production in L1 after L2.

当二语使用者在使用第二语言(L2)后转回母语(L1)说话时,他们往往会在检索母语单词时遇到困难。这种现象被称为 L2 后效应。我们以 L2 后效应为视角,探索二语语言控制机制的神经基础。我们的目标有两个:第一,探索双语语言控制是利用领域通用机制还是语言特定机制;第二,研究驱动 L2 后效应的精确机制。我们使用基于功能定位器的精确 fMRI 方法来测量反映 L2 后效应的大脑活动与语言网络(Fedorenko 等人,2010 年)和领域通用多重需求网络(Duncan,2010 年)以及三个特定任务网络(分别用于干扰解决、词汇检索和发音)的重叠程度。42 名波兰语-英语双语者参与了研究。我们的研究结果表明,L2 后效应反映了对一般领域而非特定语言资源的更多参与。此外,与之前提出的解释相反,我们没有发现证据表明后效应反映了与词汇获取、发音和解决词汇干扰有关的难度增加。我们认为,图片命名范式中的语音生成困难--表现为 L2 后效应--反映了任务图式非语言层面的干扰或 L1 后 L2 语音生成过程中认知控制参与的普遍增加。
{"title":"Tracking Components of Bilingual Language Control in Speech Production: An fMRI Study Using Functional Localizers.","authors":"Agata Wolna, Jakub Szewczyk, Michele Diaz, Aleksandra Domagalik, Marcin Szwed, Zofia Wodniecka","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00128","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00128","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When bilingual speakers switch back to speaking in their native language (L1) after having used their second language (L2), they often experience difficulty in retrieving words in their L1. This phenomenon is referred to as the <i>L2 after-effect</i>. We used the L2 after-effect as a lens to explore the neural bases of bilingual language control mechanisms. Our goal was twofold: first, to explore whether bilingual language control draws on domain-general or language-specific mechanisms; second, to investigate the precise mechanism(s) that drive the L2 after-effect. We used a precision fMRI approach based on functional localizers to measure the extent to which the brain activity that reflects the L2 after-effect overlaps with the language network (Fedorenko et al., 2010) and the domain-general multiple demand network (Duncan, 2010), as well as three task-specific networks that tap into interference resolution, lexical retrieval, and articulation. Forty-two Polish-English bilinguals participated in the study. Our results show that the L2 after-effect reflects increased engagement of domain-general but not language-specific resources. Furthermore, contrary to previously proposed interpretations, we did not find evidence that the effect reflects increased difficulty related to lexical access, articulation, and the resolution of lexical interference. We propose that difficulty of speech production in the picture naming paradigm-manifested as the L2 after-effect-reflects interference at a nonlinguistic level of task schemas or a general increase of cognitive control engagement during speech production in L1 after L2.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11093400/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141238331","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Approaches to Measuring Language Lateralisation: An Exploratory Study Comparing Two fMRI Methods and Functional Transcranial Doppler Ultrasound. 测量语言侧化的方法:比较两种 fMRI 方法和功能性经颅多普勒超声的探索性研究。
IF 3.6 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-03 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00136
Dorothy V M Bishop, Zoe V J Woodhead, Kate E Watkins

In this exploratory study we compare and contrast two methods for deriving a laterality index (LI) from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data: the weighted bootstrapped mean from the LI Toolbox (toolbox method), and a novel method that uses subtraction of activations from homologous regions in left and right hemispheres to give an array of difference scores (mirror method). Data came from 31 individuals who had been selected to include a high proportion of people with atypical laterality when tested with functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound (fTCD). On two tasks, word generation and semantic matching, the mirror method generally gave better agreement with fTCD laterality than the toolbox method, both for individual regions of interest, and for a large region corresponding to the middle cerebral artery. LI estimates from this method had much smaller confidence intervals (CIs) than those from the toolbox method; with the mirror method, most participants were reliably lateralised to left or right, whereas with the toolbox method, a higher proportion were categorised as bilateral (i.e., the CI for the LI spanned zero). Reasons for discrepancies between fMRI methods are discussed: one issue is that the toolbox method averages the LI across a wide range of thresholds. Furthermore, examination of task-related t-statistic maps from the two hemispheres showed that language lateralisation is evident in regions characterised by deactivation, and so key information may be lost by ignoring voxel activations below zero, as is done with conventional estimates of the LI.

在这项探索性研究中,我们比较并对比了从功能磁共振成像(fMRI)数据中得出侧位指数(LI)的两种方法:LI 工具箱中的加权自引导平均值(工具箱法)和一种新方法,后者使用减去左右半球同源区域的激活来给出一系列差异分数(镜像法)。数据来自 31 人,这些人在接受功能性经颅多普勒超声(fTCD)测试时被选中,其中有很大一部分人的侧位不典型。在单词生成和语义匹配这两项任务中,镜像法与 fTCD 侧位的一致性普遍优于工具箱法,无论是在单个相关区域,还是在与大脑中动脉相对应的一个大区域。这种方法得出的侧位估计值的置信区间(CI)比工具箱方法小得多;使用镜像方法,大多数参与者都能可靠地侧位到左侧或右侧,而使用工具箱方法,被归类为双侧的比例较高(即侧位估计值的置信区间为零)。本文讨论了 fMRI 方法之间存在差异的原因:其中一个问题是,工具箱方法是在广泛的阈值范围内平均 LI。此外,对来自两个半球的任务相关 t 统计图的研究表明,语言的侧向性在以失活为特征的区域非常明显,因此如果像传统的 LI 估算方法那样忽略低于零的体素激活,可能会丢失关键信息。
{"title":"Approaches to Measuring Language Lateralisation: An Exploratory Study Comparing Two fMRI Methods and Functional Transcranial Doppler Ultrasound.","authors":"Dorothy V M Bishop, Zoe V J Woodhead, Kate E Watkins","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00136","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00136","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this exploratory study we compare and contrast two methods for deriving a laterality index (LI) from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data: the weighted bootstrapped mean from the LI Toolbox (toolbox method), and a novel method that uses subtraction of activations from homologous regions in left and right hemispheres to give an array of difference scores (mirror method). Data came from 31 individuals who had been selected to include a high proportion of people with atypical laterality when tested with functional transcranial Doppler ultrasound (fTCD). On two tasks, word generation and semantic matching, the mirror method generally gave better agreement with fTCD laterality than the toolbox method, both for individual regions of interest, and for a large region corresponding to the middle cerebral artery. LI estimates from this method had much smaller confidence intervals (CIs) than those from the toolbox method; with the mirror method, most participants were reliably lateralised to left or right, whereas with the toolbox method, a higher proportion were categorised as bilateral (i.e., the CI for the LI spanned zero). Reasons for discrepancies between fMRI methods are discussed: one issue is that the toolbox method averages the LI across a wide range of thresholds. Furthermore, examination of task-related <i>t</i>-statistic maps from the two hemispheres showed that language lateralisation is evident in regions characterised by deactivation, and so key information may be lost by ignoring voxel activations below zero, as is done with conventional estimates of the LI.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11192441/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443465","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differences in Cortical Surface Area in Developmental Language Disorder. 发育性语言障碍的皮质表面积差异。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-03 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00127
Nilgoun Bahar, Gabriel J Cler, Saloni Krishnan, Salomi S Asaridou, Harriet J Smith, Hanna E Willis, Máiréad P Healy, Kate E Watkins

Approximately 7% of children have developmental language disorder (DLD), a neurodevelopmental condition associated with persistent language learning difficulties without a known cause. Our understanding of the neurobiological basis of DLD is limited. Here, we used FreeSurfer to investigate cortical surface area and thickness in a large cohort of 156 children and adolescents aged 10-16 years with a range of language abilities, including 54 with DLD, 28 with a history of speech-language difficulties who did not meet criteria for DLD, and 74 age-matched controls with typical language development (TD). We also examined cortical asymmetries in DLD using an automated surface-based technique. Relative to the TD group, those with DLD showed smaller surface area bilaterally in the inferior frontal gyrus extending to the anterior insula, in the posterior temporal and ventral occipito-temporal cortex, and in portions of the anterior cingulate and superior frontal cortex. Analysis of the whole cohort using a language proficiency factor revealed that language ability correlated positively with surface area in similar regions. There were no differences in cortical thickness, nor in asymmetry of these cortical metrics between TD and DLD. This study highlights the importance of distinguishing between surface area and cortical thickness in investigating the brain basis of neurodevelopmental disorders and suggests the development of cortical surface area to be of importance to DLD. Future longitudinal studies are required to understand the developmental trajectory of these cortical differences in DLD and how they relate to language maturation.

约有 7% 的儿童患有发育性语言障碍 (DLD),这是一种神经发育疾病,与不明原因的持续性语言学习困难有关。我们对 DLD 的神经生物学基础了解有限。在这里,我们使用 FreeSurfer 对 156 名年龄在 10-16 岁之间、具有不同语言能力的儿童和青少年的皮层表面积和厚度进行了研究,其中包括 54 名发育性语言障碍患者、28 名有语言障碍史但不符合发育性语言障碍标准的患者,以及 74 名年龄匹配的典型语言发育(TD)对照者。我们还使用一种基于表面的自动技术检测了 DLD 的皮层不对称性。与 TD 组相比,DLD 患者的双侧额叶下回延伸至岛叶前部、颞叶后部和枕颞叶腹侧皮层以及扣带回前部和额叶上部皮层的部分表面积较小。使用语言能力因子对整个群体进行分析后发现,语言能力与类似区域的表面积呈正相关。TD和DLD的皮质厚度以及这些皮质指标的不对称性均无差异。这项研究强调了在研究神经发育障碍的大脑基础时区分表面积和皮质厚度的重要性,并表明皮质表面积的发展对 DLD 很重要。未来需要进行纵向研究,以了解 DLD 中这些皮质差异的发展轨迹以及它们与语言成熟的关系。
{"title":"Differences in Cortical Surface Area in Developmental Language Disorder.","authors":"Nilgoun Bahar, Gabriel J Cler, Saloni Krishnan, Salomi S Asaridou, Harriet J Smith, Hanna E Willis, Máiréad P Healy, Kate E Watkins","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00127","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00127","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Approximately 7% of children have developmental language disorder (DLD), a neurodevelopmental condition associated with persistent language learning difficulties without a known cause. Our understanding of the neurobiological basis of DLD is limited. Here, we used FreeSurfer to investigate cortical surface area and thickness in a large cohort of 156 children and adolescents aged 10-16 years with a range of language abilities, including 54 with DLD, 28 with a history of speech-language difficulties who did not meet criteria for DLD, and 74 age-matched controls with typical language development (TD). We also examined cortical asymmetries in DLD using an automated surface-based technique. Relative to the TD group, those with DLD showed smaller surface area bilaterally in the inferior frontal gyrus extending to the anterior insula, in the posterior temporal and ventral occipito-temporal cortex, and in portions of the anterior cingulate and superior frontal cortex. Analysis of the whole cohort using a language proficiency factor revealed that language ability correlated positively with surface area in similar regions. There were no differences in cortical thickness, nor in asymmetry of these cortical metrics between TD and DLD. This study highlights the importance of distinguishing between surface area and cortical thickness in investigating the brain basis of neurodevelopmental disorders and suggests the development of cortical surface area to be of importance to DLD. Future longitudinal studies are required to understand the developmental trajectory of these cortical differences in DLD and how they relate to language maturation.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11093399/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141238330","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reactive Inhibitory Control Precedes Overt Stuttering Events. 反应性抑制控制先于明显的口吃事件
IF 3.6 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2024-06-03 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00138
Joan Orpella, Graham Flick, M Florencia Assaneo, Ravi Shroff, Liina Pylkkänen, David Poeppel, Eric S Jackson

Research points to neurofunctional differences underlying fluent speech between stutterers and non-stutterers. Considerably less work has focused on processes that underlie stuttered vs. fluent speech. Additionally, most of this research has focused on speech motor processes despite contributions from cognitive processes prior to the onset of stuttered speech. We used MEG to test the hypothesis that reactive inhibitory control is triggered prior to stuttered speech. Twenty-nine stutterers completed a delayed-response task that featured a cue (prior to a go cue) signaling the imminent requirement to produce a word that was either stuttered or fluent. Consistent with our hypothesis, we observed increased beta power likely emanating from the right pre-supplementary motor area (R-preSMA)-an area implicated in reactive inhibitory control-in response to the cue preceding stuttered vs. fluent productions. Beta power differences between stuttered and fluent trials correlated with stuttering severity and participants' percentage of trials stuttered increased exponentially with beta power in the R-preSMA. Trial-by-trial beta power modulations in the R-preSMA following the cue predicted whether a trial would be stuttered or fluent. Stuttered trials were also associated with delayed speech onset suggesting an overall slowing or freezing of the speech motor system that may be a consequence of inhibitory control. Post-hoc analyses revealed that independently generated anticipated words were associated with greater beta power and more stuttering than researcher-assisted anticipated words, pointing to a relationship between self-perceived likelihood of stuttering (i.e., anticipation) and inhibitory control. This work offers a neurocognitive account of stuttering by characterizing cognitive processes that precede overt stuttering events.

研究表明,口吃者与非口吃者流利说话的神经功能差异。而关于口吃与流利说话的基础过程的研究则少得多。此外,尽管口吃言语发生前的认知过程也有贡献,但大部分研究都集中在言语运动过程上。我们使用 MEG 测试了在口吃言语发生前触发反应性抑制控制的假设。29 名口吃者完成了一项延迟反应任务,该任务的特点是提示(在 "开始 "提示之前)即将要求他们说出一个结巴或流利的单词。与我们的假设一致的是,我们观察到,在结巴与流利发音之前,来自右侧前辅助运动区(R-preSMA)--一个与反应性抑制控制有关的区域--的β功率增加了。口吃试验和流利试验之间的β功率差异与口吃严重程度相关,参与者口吃试验的百分比随R-preSMA中β功率的增加而呈指数增长。在提示之后,R-preSMA 中的逐次试验贝塔功率调节可预测试验是口吃还是流利。口吃试验还与延迟开始说话有关,这表明言语运动系统的整体减慢或冻结可能是抑制控制的结果。事后分析表明,与研究人员辅助的预期词相比,独立产生的预期词与更大的β功率和更多的口吃有关,这表明自我感知的口吃可能性(即预期)与抑制控制之间存在关系。这项研究通过描述明显口吃事件发生前的认知过程,提供了口吃的神经认知解释。
{"title":"Reactive Inhibitory Control Precedes Overt Stuttering Events.","authors":"Joan Orpella, Graham Flick, M Florencia Assaneo, Ravi Shroff, Liina Pylkkänen, David Poeppel, Eric S Jackson","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00138","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00138","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research points to neurofunctional differences underlying fluent speech between stutterers and non-stutterers. Considerably less work has focused on processes that underlie stuttered vs. fluent speech. Additionally, most of this research has focused on speech motor processes despite contributions from cognitive processes prior to the onset of stuttered speech. We used MEG to test the hypothesis that reactive inhibitory control is triggered prior to stuttered speech. Twenty-nine stutterers completed a delayed-response task that featured a cue (prior to a go cue) signaling the imminent requirement to produce a word that was either stuttered or fluent. Consistent with our hypothesis, we observed increased beta power likely emanating from the right pre-supplementary motor area (R-preSMA)-an area implicated in reactive inhibitory control-in response to the cue preceding stuttered vs. fluent productions. Beta power differences between stuttered and fluent trials correlated with stuttering severity and participants' percentage of trials stuttered increased exponentially with beta power in the R-preSMA. Trial-by-trial beta power modulations in the R-preSMA following the cue predicted whether a trial would be stuttered or fluent. Stuttered trials were also associated with delayed speech onset suggesting an overall slowing or freezing of the speech motor system that may be a consequence of inhibitory control. Post-hoc analyses revealed that independently generated anticipated words were associated with greater beta power and more stuttering than researcher-assisted anticipated words, pointing to a relationship between self-perceived likelihood of stuttering (i.e., anticipation) and inhibitory control. This work offers a neurocognitive account of stuttering by characterizing cognitive processes that precede overt stuttering events.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11192511/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443467","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pars Opercularis Underlies Efferent Predictions and Successful Auditory Feedback Processing in Speech: Evidence From Left-Hemisphere Stroke. 听小骨旁支持言语中的传出预测和成功的听觉反馈处理:左半球中风的证据
IF 3.6 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2024-06-03 eCollection Date: 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00139
Sara D Beach, Ding-Lan Tang, Swathi Kiran, Caroline A Niziolek

Hearing one's own speech allows for acoustic self-monitoring in real time. Left-hemisphere motor planning regions are thought to give rise to efferent predictions that can be compared to true feedback in sensory cortices, resulting in neural suppression commensurate with the degree of overlap between predicted and actual sensations. Sensory prediction errors thus serve as a possible mechanism of detection of deviant speech sounds, which can then feed back into corrective action, allowing for online control of speech acoustics. The goal of this study was to assess the integrity of this detection-correction circuit in persons with aphasia (PWA) whose left-hemisphere lesions may limit their ability to control variability in speech output. We recorded magnetoencephalography (MEG) while 15 PWA and age-matched controls spoke monosyllabic words and listened to playback of their utterances. From this, we measured speaking-induced suppression of the M100 neural response and related it to lesion profiles and speech behavior. Both speaking-induced suppression and cortical sensitivity to deviance were preserved at the group level in PWA. PWA with more spared tissue in pars opercularis had greater left-hemisphere neural suppression and greater behavioral correction of acoustically deviant pronunciations, whereas sparing of superior temporal gyrus was not related to neural suppression or acoustic behavior. In turn, PWA who made greater corrections had fewer overt speech errors in the MEG task. Thus, the motor planning regions that generate the efferent prediction are integral to performing corrections when that prediction is violated.

听自己说话可以实时进行声音自我监测。左半球的运动规划区域被认为会产生传出预测,这些预测可与感觉皮层中的真实反馈进行比较,从而导致与预测和实际感觉之间的重叠程度相称的神经抑制。因此,感觉预测误差可作为检测偏差语音的一种可能机制,然后反馈到纠正行动中,从而实现对语音声学的在线控制。本研究的目的是评估失语症患者(PWA)这种检测-纠正回路的完整性,因为他们的左半球病变可能会限制他们控制语音输出变异的能力。我们记录了 15 名 PWA 和年龄匹配的对照组患者在说单音节词时的脑磁图 (MEG),并聆听了他们的语音回放。由此,我们测量了说话引起的 M100 神经反应抑制,并将其与病变特征和言语行为联系起来。在 PWA 中,说话引起的抑制和大脑皮层对偏差的敏感性在群体水平上都得到了保留。肌旁组织受损较多的 PWA 具有更强的左半球神经抑制能力,对声音偏差发音的行为纠正能力也更强,而颞上回的受损则与神经抑制或声音行为无关。反过来,在 MEG 任务中,做出更多纠正的 PWA 出现的明显语音错误更少。因此,产生传出预测的运动规划区域是在预测被违反时进行纠正的不可或缺的部分。
{"title":"Pars Opercularis Underlies Efferent Predictions and Successful Auditory Feedback Processing in Speech: Evidence From Left-Hemisphere Stroke.","authors":"Sara D Beach, Ding-Lan Tang, Swathi Kiran, Caroline A Niziolek","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00139","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00139","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Hearing one's own speech allows for acoustic self-monitoring in real time. Left-hemisphere motor planning regions are thought to give rise to efferent predictions that can be compared to true feedback in sensory cortices, resulting in neural suppression commensurate with the degree of overlap between predicted and actual sensations. Sensory prediction errors thus serve as a possible mechanism of detection of deviant speech sounds, which can then feed back into corrective action, allowing for online control of speech acoustics. The goal of this study was to assess the integrity of this detection-correction circuit in persons with aphasia (PWA) whose left-hemisphere lesions may limit their ability to control variability in speech output. We recorded magnetoencephalography (MEG) while 15 PWA and age-matched controls spoke monosyllabic words and listened to playback of their utterances. From this, we measured speaking-induced suppression of the M100 neural response and related it to lesion profiles and speech behavior. Both speaking-induced suppression and cortical sensitivity to deviance were preserved at the group level in PWA. PWA with more spared tissue in pars opercularis had greater left-hemisphere neural suppression and greater behavioral correction of acoustically deviant pronunciations, whereas sparing of superior temporal gyrus was not related to neural suppression or acoustic behavior. In turn, PWA who made greater corrections had fewer overt speech errors in the MEG task. Thus, the motor planning regions that generate the efferent prediction are integral to performing corrections when that prediction is violated.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11192514/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141443466","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Neurobiology of Language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1