首页 > 最新文献

Neurobiology of Language最新文献

英文 中文
Cerebral White Matter Mediation of Age-Related Differences in Picture Naming Across Adulthood. 大脑白质对成年后与年龄有关的图片命名差异的调节作用
IF 3.6 Q1 LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2022-03-30 eCollection Date: 2022-03-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00065
Sara B W Troutman, David J Madden, Michele T Diaz

As people age, one of the most common complaints is difficulty with word retrieval. A wealth of behavioral research confirms such age-related language production deficits, yet the structural neural differences that relate to age-related language production deficits remains an open area of exploration. Therefore, the present study used a large sample of healthy adults across adulthood to investigate how age-related white matter differences in three key left-hemisphere language tracts may contribute to age-related differences in language ability. Specifically, we used diffusion tensor imaging to measure fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD) which are indicators of white matter structure. We then used a series of path models to test whether white matter from the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and the frontal aslant tract (FAT) mediated age-related differences in one form of language production, picture naming. We found that FA, as well as RD from the SLF and FAT mediated the relation between age and picture naming performance, whereas a control tract (corticospinal) was not a mediator. Moreover, differences between mediation of picture naming and a control naming condition suggest that left SLF has a greater role in higher-order aspects of naming, such as semantic and lexical selection whereas left FAT is more sensitive to sensorimotor aspects of fluency or speech motor planning. These results suggest that dorsal white matter contributes to age-related differences in generating speech and may be particularly important in supporting word retrieval across adulthood.

随着年龄的增长,人们最常抱怨的问题之一就是单词检索困难。大量的行为学研究证实了这种与年龄相关的语言生成障碍,然而与年龄相关的语言生成障碍有关的结构性神经差异仍然是一个有待探索的领域。因此,本研究使用了大量成年期健康成人样本,研究三个关键左半球语言束中与年龄相关的白质差异如何导致语言能力的年龄相关性差异。具体来说,我们使用扩散张量成像技术测量了白质结构的指标--分数各向异性(FA)和径向扩散率(RD)。然后,我们使用一系列路径模型来检验来自上纵筋束(SLF)、下纵筋束和额叶斜束(FAT)的白质是否介导了与年龄有关的一种语言生成形式--图片命名--的差异。我们发现,FA以及SLF和FAT的RD介导了年龄与图片命名表现之间的关系,而对照束(皮质脊髓)不是介导因素。此外,图片命名和对照命名条件之间的中介差异表明,左侧SLF在命名的高阶方面(如语义和词汇选择)起着更大的作用,而左侧FAT则对流畅性或言语运动规划的感觉运动方面更敏感。这些结果表明,背侧白质有助于产生语音的年龄相关差异,并可能在支持成年后的单词检索方面发挥特别重要的作用。
{"title":"Cerebral White Matter Mediation of Age-Related Differences in Picture Naming Across Adulthood.","authors":"Sara B W Troutman, David J Madden, Michele T Diaz","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00065","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00065","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As people age, one of the most common complaints is difficulty with word retrieval. A wealth of behavioral research confirms such age-related language production deficits, yet the structural neural differences that relate to age-related language production deficits remains an open area of exploration. Therefore, the present study used a large sample of healthy adults across adulthood to investigate how age-related white matter differences in three key left-hemisphere language tracts may contribute to age-related differences in language ability. Specifically, we used diffusion tensor imaging to measure fractional anisotropy (FA) and radial diffusivity (RD) which are indicators of white matter structure. We then used a series of path models to test whether white matter from the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), the inferior longitudinal fasciculus, and the frontal aslant tract (FAT) mediated age-related differences in one form of language production, picture naming. We found that FA, as well as RD from the SLF and FAT mediated the relation between age and picture naming performance, whereas a control tract (corticospinal) was not a mediator. Moreover, differences between mediation of picture naming and a control naming condition suggest that left SLF has a greater role in higher-order aspects of naming, such as semantic and lexical selection whereas left FAT is more sensitive to sensorimotor aspects of fluency or speech motor planning. These results suggest that dorsal white matter contributes to age-related differences in generating speech and may be particularly important in supporting word retrieval across adulthood.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.6,"publicationDate":"2022-03-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9169883/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10252281","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Precentral Gyrus Contributions to the Early Time-Course of Grapheme-to-Phoneme Conversion. 前中央回对早期词素到音素转换时间过程的贡献
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-02-10 eCollection Date: 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00047
Erik Kaestner, Xiaojing Wu, Daniel Friedman, Patricia Dugan, Orrin Devinsky, Chad Carlson, Werner Doyle, Thomas Thesen, Eric Halgren

As part of silent reading models, visual orthographic information is transduced into an auditory phonological code in a process of grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (GPC). This process is often identified with lateral temporal-parietal regions associated with auditory phoneme encoding. However, the role of articulatory phonemic representations and the precentral gyrus in GPC is ambiguous. Though the precentral gyrus is implicated in many functional MRI studies of reading, it is not clear if the time course of activity in this region is consistent with the precentral gyrus being involved in GPC. We recorded cortical electrophysiology during a bimodal match/mismatch task from eight patients with perisylvian subdural electrodes to examine the time course of neural activity during a task that necessitated GPC. Patients made a match/mismatch decision between a 3-letter string and the following auditory bi-phoneme. We characterized the distribution and timing of evoked broadband high gamma (70-170 Hz) as well as phase-locking between electrodes. The precentral gyrus emerged with a high concentration of broadband high gamma responses to visual and auditory language as well as mismatch effects. The pars opercularis, supramarginal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus were also involved. The precentral gyrus showed strong phase-locking with the caudal fusiform gyrus during letter-string presentation and with surrounding perisylvian cortex during the bimodal visual-auditory comparison period. These findings hint at a role for precentral cortex in transducing visual into auditory codes during silent reading.

作为无声阅读模型的一部分,视觉正字法信息在词素到音素的转换(GPC)过程中被转换成听觉音素编码。这一过程通常与听觉音素编码相关的外侧颞顶叶区域相联系。然而,发音音位表征和前中央回在 GPC 中的作用并不明确。虽然在许多关于阅读的功能性核磁共振成像研究中都涉及到了前额回,但目前还不清楚该区域的活动时间进程是否与前额回参与 GPC 的时间进程一致。我们用硬膜下电极记录了八名患者在完成双模匹配/错配任务时的皮层电生理学过程,以研究他们在完成需要 GPC 的任务时神经活动的时间进程。患者在 3 个字母的字符串和接下来的听觉双音节之间进行匹配/错配判断。我们对诱发宽带高伽马(70-170 Hz)的分布和时间以及电极间的相位锁定进行了描述。前中央回对视觉和听觉语言的宽带高伽马反应以及错配效应非常集中。眼旁、边际上回和颞上回也参与其中。在字母字符串呈现过程中,前中央回与尾部纺锤形回表现出很强的相位锁定,而在视觉-听觉双模态比较期,前中央回与周围的颞叶周围皮层也表现出很强的相位锁定。这些研究结果表明,在默读过程中,前中央皮层在将视觉代码转换为听觉代码方面发挥了作用。
{"title":"The Precentral Gyrus Contributions to the Early Time-Course of Grapheme-to-Phoneme Conversion.","authors":"Erik Kaestner, Xiaojing Wu, Daniel Friedman, Patricia Dugan, Orrin Devinsky, Chad Carlson, Werner Doyle, Thomas Thesen, Eric Halgren","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00047","DOIUrl":"10.1162/nol_a_00047","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>As part of silent reading models, visual orthographic information is transduced into an auditory phonological code in a process of grapheme-to-phoneme conversion (GPC). This process is often identified with lateral temporal-parietal regions associated with auditory phoneme encoding. However, the role of articulatory phonemic representations and the precentral gyrus in GPC is ambiguous. Though the precentral gyrus is implicated in many functional MRI studies of reading, it is not clear if the time course of activity in this region is consistent with the precentral gyrus being involved in GPC. We recorded cortical electrophysiology during a bimodal match/mismatch task from eight patients with perisylvian subdural electrodes to examine the time course of neural activity during a task that necessitated GPC. Patients made a match/mismatch decision between a 3-letter string and the following auditory bi-phoneme. We characterized the distribution and timing of evoked broadband high gamma (70-170 Hz) as well as phase-locking between electrodes. The precentral gyrus emerged with a high concentration of broadband high gamma responses to visual and auditory language as well as mismatch effects. The pars opercularis, supramarginal gyrus, and superior temporal gyrus were also involved. The precentral gyrus showed strong phase-locking with the caudal fusiform gyrus during letter-string presentation and with surrounding perisylvian cortex during the bimodal visual-auditory comparison period. These findings hint at a role for precentral cortex in transducing visual into auditory codes during silent reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-02-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158576/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9875135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A Linear Superposition Model of Envelope and Frequency Following Responses May Help Identify Generators Based on Latency. 包络和频率跟随响应的线性叠加模型可以帮助识别基于延迟的发生器。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00072
Tobias Teichert, G Nike Gnanateja, Srivatsun Sadagopan, Bharath Chandrasekaran

Envelope and frequency-following responses (FFRENV and FFRTFS) are scalp-recorded electrophysiological potentials that closely follow the periodicity of complex sounds such as speech. These signals have been established as important biomarkers in speech and learning disorders. However, despite important advances, it has remained challenging to map altered FFRENV and FFRTFS to altered processing in specific brain regions. Here we explore the utility of a deconvolution approach based on the assumption that FFRENV and FFRTFS reflect the linear superposition of responses that are triggered by the glottal pulse in each cycle of the fundamental frequency (F0 responses). We tested the deconvolution method by applying it to FFRENV and FFRTFS of rhesus monkeys to human speech and click trains with time-varying pitch patterns. Our analyses show that F0ENV responses could be measured with high signal-to-noise ratio and featured several spectro-temporally and topographically distinct components that likely reflect the activation of brainstem (<5 ms; 200-1000 Hz), midbrain (5-15 ms; 100-250 Hz), and cortex (15-35 ms; ~90 Hz). In contrast, F0TFS responses contained only one spectro-temporal component that likely reflected activity in the midbrain. In summary, our results support the notion that the latency of F0 components map meaningfully onto successive processing stages. This opens the possibility that pathologically altered FFRENV or FFRTFS may be linked to altered F0ENV or F0TFS and from there to specific processing stages and ultimately spatially targeted interventions.

包络和频率跟随响应(FFRENV和FFRTFS)是头皮记录的电生理电位,密切关注复杂声音(如语音)的周期性。这些信号已被确定为言语和学习障碍的重要生物标志物。然而,尽管取得了重要进展,但将改变的FFRENV和FFRTFS映射到特定大脑区域改变的加工过程仍然具有挑战性。在这里,我们基于假设FFRENV和FFRTFS反映了由声门脉冲在基频的每个周期(F0响应)触发的响应的线性叠加,探讨了反卷积方法的效用。我们将反卷积方法应用于恒河猴的FFRENV和FFRTFS对人类语音和具有时变音高模式的点击训练进行测试。我们的分析表明,F0ENV反应可以用高信噪比测量,并且具有几个可能反映脑干激活的光谱时间和地形不同的成分(TFS反应只包含一个可能反映中脑活动的光谱时间成分)。总之,我们的研究结果支持这样的观点,即F0组件的延迟有意义地映射到连续的处理阶段。这开启了一种可能性,即病理性改变的FFRENV或FFRTFS可能与改变的F0ENV或F0TFS有关,并由此进入特定的加工阶段,最终实现空间靶向干预。
{"title":"A Linear Superposition Model of Envelope and Frequency Following Responses May Help Identify Generators Based on Latency.","authors":"Tobias Teichert,&nbsp;G Nike Gnanateja,&nbsp;Srivatsun Sadagopan,&nbsp;Bharath Chandrasekaran","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00072","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00072","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Envelope and frequency-following responses (FFR<sub>ENV</sub> and FFR<sub>TFS</sub>) are scalp-recorded electrophysiological potentials that closely follow the periodicity of complex sounds such as speech. These signals have been established as important biomarkers in speech and learning disorders. However, despite important advances, it has remained challenging to map altered FFR<sub>ENV</sub> and FFR<sub>TFS</sub> to altered processing in specific brain regions. Here we explore the utility of a deconvolution approach based on the assumption that FFR<sub>ENV</sub> and FFR<sub>TFS</sub> reflect the linear superposition of responses that are triggered by the glottal pulse in each cycle of the fundamental frequency (F0 responses). We tested the deconvolution method by applying it to FFR<sub>ENV</sub> and FFR<sub>TFS</sub> of rhesus monkeys to human speech and click trains with time-varying pitch patterns. Our analyses show that F0<sub>ENV</sub> responses could be measured with high signal-to-noise ratio and featured several spectro-temporally and topographically distinct components that likely reflect the activation of brainstem (<5 ms; 200-1000 Hz), midbrain (5-15 ms; 100-250 Hz), and cortex (15-35 ms; ~90 Hz). In contrast, F0<sub>TFS</sub> responses contained only one spectro-temporal component that likely reflected activity in the midbrain. In summary, our results support the notion that the latency of F0 components map meaningfully onto successive processing stages. This opens the possibility that pathologically altered FFR<sub>ENV</sub> or FFR<sub>TFS</sub> may be linked to altered F0<sub>ENV</sub> or F0<sub>TFS</sub> and from there to specific processing stages and ultimately spatially targeted interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10003646/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9112292","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Activation in Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Underlies Stuttering Anticipation. 右背外侧前额叶皮层的激活是口吃预期的基础。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00073
Eric S Jackson, Swethasri Dravida, Xian Zhang, J Adam Noah, Vincent Gracco, Joy Hirsch

People who stutter learn to anticipate many of their overt stuttering events. Despite the critical role of anticipation, particularly how responses to anticipation shape stuttering behaviors, the neural bases associated with anticipation are unknown. We used a novel approach to identify anticipated and unanticipated words, which were produced by 22 adult stutterers in a delayed-response task while hemodynamic activity was measured using functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Twenty-two control participants were included such that each individualized set of anticipated and unanticipated words was produced by one stutterer and one control participant. We conducted an analysis on the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (R-DLPFC) based on converging lines of evidence from the stuttering and cognitive control literatures. We also assessed connectivity between the R-DLPFC and right supramarginal gyrus (R-SMG), two key nodes of the frontoparietal network (FPN), to assess the role of cognitive control, and particularly error-likelihood monitoring, in stuttering anticipation. All analyses focused on the five-second anticipation phase preceding the go signal to produce speech. The results indicate that anticipated words are associated with elevated activation in the R-DLPFC, and that compared to non-stutterers, stutterers exhibit greater activity in the R-DLPFC, irrespective of anticipation. Further, anticipated words are associated with reduced connectivity between the R-DLPFC and R-SMG. These findings highlight the potential roles of the R-DLPFC and the greater FPN as a neural substrate of stuttering anticipation. The results also support previous accounts of error-likelihood monitoring and action-stopping in stuttering anticipation. Overall, this work offers numerous directions for future research with clinical implications for targeted neuromodulation.

口吃的人学会预测许多明显的口吃事件。尽管预期的关键作用,特别是对预期的反应如何塑造口吃行为,但与预期相关的神经基础是未知的。我们采用一种新颖的方法来识别22名成年口吃者在延迟反应任务中产生的预期词和非预期词,同时使用功能近红外光谱(fNIRS)测量血液动力学活动。22名对照参与者被包括在内,这样每一组预期和未预期的单词由一名口吃者和一名对照参与者产生。我们对右背外侧前额叶皮层(R-DLPFC)进行了一项基于口吃和认知控制文献证据的分析。我们还评估了R-DLPFC和右侧边缘上回(R-SMG)之间的连通性,这是额顶叶网络(FPN)的两个关键节点,以评估认知控制,特别是错误可能性监测在口吃预期中的作用。所有的分析都集中在go信号产生语音之前的5秒预期阶段。结果表明,预期单词与R-DLPFC的激活升高有关,并且与非口吃者相比,与预期无关,口吃者在R-DLPFC中表现出更大的活性。此外,预期单词与R-DLPFC和R-SMG之间的连通性降低有关。这些发现强调了R-DLPFC和大FPN作为口吃预期的神经基质的潜在作用。研究结果也支持了先前关于错误可能性监测和口吃预期行为停止的说法。总的来说,这项工作为未来的研究提供了许多方向,具有针对性的神经调节的临床意义。
{"title":"Activation in Right Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex Underlies Stuttering Anticipation.","authors":"Eric S Jackson,&nbsp;Swethasri Dravida,&nbsp;Xian Zhang,&nbsp;J Adam Noah,&nbsp;Vincent Gracco,&nbsp;Joy Hirsch","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00073","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00073","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People who stutter learn to anticipate many of their overt stuttering events. Despite the critical role of anticipation, particularly how responses to anticipation shape stuttering behaviors, the neural bases associated with anticipation are unknown. We used a novel approach to identify anticipated and unanticipated words, which were produced by 22 adult stutterers in a delayed-response task while hemodynamic activity was measured using functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Twenty-two control participants were included such that each individualized set of anticipated and unanticipated words was produced by one stutterer and one control participant. We conducted an analysis on the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (R-DLPFC) based on converging lines of evidence from the stuttering and cognitive control literatures. We also assessed connectivity between the R-DLPFC and right supramarginal gyrus (R-SMG), two key nodes of the frontoparietal network (FPN), to assess the role of cognitive control, and particularly error-likelihood monitoring, in stuttering anticipation. All analyses focused on the five-second anticipation phase preceding the go signal to produce speech. The results indicate that anticipated words are associated with elevated activation in the R-DLPFC, and that compared to non-stutterers, stutterers exhibit greater activity in the R-DLPFC, irrespective of anticipation. Further, anticipated words are associated with reduced connectivity between the R-DLPFC and R-SMG. These findings highlight the potential roles of the R-DLPFC and the greater FPN as a neural substrate of stuttering anticipation. The results also support previous accounts of error-likelihood monitoring and action-stopping in stuttering anticipation. Overall, this work offers numerous directions for future research with clinical implications for targeted neuromodulation.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158639/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9705201","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Early Development of Neural Speech Encoding Depends on Age but Not Native Language Status: Evidence From Lexical Tone. 神经语音编码的早期发展取决于年龄而非母语状态:来自词汇语调的证据。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00049
Nikolay Novitskiy, Akshay R Maggu, Ching Man Lai, Peggy H Y Chan, Kay H Y Wong, Hugh Simon Lam, Tak Yeung Leung, Ting Fan Leung, Patrick C M Wong

We investigated the development of early-latency and long-latency brain responses to native and non-native speech to shed light on the neurophysiological underpinnings of perceptual narrowing and early language development. Specifically, we postulated a two-level process to explain the decrease in sensitivity to non-native phonemes toward the end of infancy. Neurons at the earlier stages of the ascending auditory pathway mature rapidly during infancy facilitating the encoding of both native and non-native sounds. This growth enables neurons at the later stages of the auditory pathway to assign phonological status to speech according to the infant's native language environment. To test this hypothesis, we collected early-latency and long-latency neural responses to native and non-native lexical tones from 85 Cantonese-learning children aged between 23 days and 24 months, 16 days. As expected, a broad range of presumably subcortical early-latency neural encoding measures grew rapidly and substantially during the first two years for both native and non-native tones. By contrast, long-latency cortical electrophysiological changes occurred on a much slower scale and showed sensitivity to nativeness at around six months. Our study provided a comprehensive understanding of early language development by revealing the complementary roles of earlier and later stages of speech processing in the developing brain.

我们研究了对母语和非母语语言的早潜伏期和长潜伏期大脑反应的发展,以揭示感知狭窄和早期语言发展的神经生理学基础。具体来说,我们假设了一个两个层次的过程来解释婴儿期结束时对非母语音素的敏感性下降。上升听觉通路早期阶段的神经元在婴儿期迅速成熟,促进了原生和非原生声音的编码。这种生长使听觉通路后期阶段的神经元能够根据婴儿的母语环境来分配语音状态。为了验证这一假设,我们收集了85名年龄在23天至24个月(16天)之间的粤语学习者对母语和非母语词汇声调的早潜伏期和长潜伏期神经反应。正如预期的那样,在头两年里,无论是原生音调还是非原生音调,大范围的可能是皮层下的早潜伏期神经编码测量都迅速而显著地增长。相比之下,长潜伏期皮质电生理变化发生的速度要慢得多,并且在大约6个月时表现出对原生动物的敏感性。我们的研究通过揭示早期和后期语言处理在大脑发育中的互补作用,为早期语言发展提供了全面的理解。
{"title":"Early Development of Neural Speech Encoding Depends on Age but Not Native Language Status: Evidence From Lexical Tone.","authors":"Nikolay Novitskiy,&nbsp;Akshay R Maggu,&nbsp;Ching Man Lai,&nbsp;Peggy H Y Chan,&nbsp;Kay H Y Wong,&nbsp;Hugh Simon Lam,&nbsp;Tak Yeung Leung,&nbsp;Ting Fan Leung,&nbsp;Patrick C M Wong","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00049","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00049","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We investigated the development of early-latency and long-latency brain responses to native and non-native speech to shed light on the neurophysiological underpinnings of perceptual narrowing and early language development. Specifically, we postulated a two-level process to explain the decrease in sensitivity to non-native phonemes toward the end of infancy. Neurons at the earlier stages of the ascending auditory pathway mature rapidly during infancy facilitating the encoding of both native and non-native sounds. This growth enables neurons at the later stages of the auditory pathway to assign phonological status to speech according to the infant's native language environment. To test this hypothesis, we collected early-latency and long-latency neural responses to native and non-native lexical tones from 85 Cantonese-learning children aged between 23 days and 24 months, 16 days. As expected, a broad range of presumably subcortical early-latency neural encoding measures grew rapidly and substantially during the first two years for both native and non-native tones. By contrast, long-latency cortical electrophysiological changes occurred on a much slower scale and showed sensitivity to nativeness at around six months. Our study provided a comprehensive understanding of early language development by revealing the complementary roles of earlier and later stages of speech processing in the developing brain.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10178623/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9875134","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Neural Tracking in Infancy Predicts Language Development in Children With and Without Family History of Autism. 婴儿期神经追踪预测有或无自闭症家族史儿童的语言发展。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00074
Katharina H Menn, Emma K Ward, Ricarda Braukmann, Carlijn van den Boomen, Jan Buitelaar, Sabine Hunnius, Tineke M Snijders

During speech processing, neural activity in non-autistic adults and infants tracks the speech envelope. Recent research in adults indicates that this neural tracking relates to linguistic knowledge and may be reduced in autism. Such reduced tracking, if present already in infancy, could impede language development. In the current study, we focused on children with a family history of autism, who often show a delay in first language acquisition. We investigated whether differences in tracking of sung nursery rhymes during infancy relate to language development and autism symptoms in childhood. We assessed speech-brain coherence at either 10 or 14 months of age in a total of 22 infants with high likelihood of autism due to family history and 19 infants without family history of autism. We analyzed the relationship between speech-brain coherence in these infants and their vocabulary at 24 months as well as autism symptoms at 36 months. Our results showed significant speech-brain coherence in the 10- and 14-month-old infants. We found no evidence for a relationship between speech-brain coherence and later autism symptoms. Importantly, speech-brain coherence in the stressed syllable rate (1-3 Hz) predicted later vocabulary. Follow-up analyses showed evidence for a relationship between tracking and vocabulary only in 10-month-olds but not in 14-month-olds and indicated possible differences between the likelihood groups. Thus, early tracking of sung nursery rhymes is related to language development in childhood.

在言语处理过程中,非自闭症成人和婴儿的神经活动追踪言语包络。最近对成人的研究表明,这种神经跟踪与语言知识有关,在自闭症中可能会减少。这种追踪能力的减少,如果已经出现在婴儿时期,可能会阻碍语言的发展。在目前的研究中,我们关注的是有自闭症家族史的儿童,他们通常在第一语言习得方面表现迟缓。我们调查了婴儿时期唱童谣的跟踪差异是否与儿童时期的语言发展和自闭症症状有关。我们在10个月或14个月大时评估了22名因家族史而有高自闭症可能性的婴儿和19名无自闭症家族史的婴儿的言语-大脑一致性。我们分析了这些婴儿在24个月大时的言语-大脑一致性与其词汇量以及36个月大时的自闭症症状之间的关系。我们的研究结果显示,10个月和14个月大的婴儿具有显著的言语-大脑一致性。我们没有发现言语-大脑一致性与后来的自闭症症状之间存在关系的证据。重要的是,语音-大脑在重读音节频率(1-3赫兹)上的一致性预测了后来的词汇量。后续分析显示,只有在10个月大的婴儿中,追踪和词汇量之间存在联系,而在14个月大的婴儿中则没有,并指出了可能性组之间可能存在的差异。因此,儿歌的早期跟踪与儿童时期的语言发展有关。
{"title":"Neural Tracking in Infancy Predicts Language Development in Children With and Without Family History of Autism.","authors":"Katharina H Menn,&nbsp;Emma K Ward,&nbsp;Ricarda Braukmann,&nbsp;Carlijn van den Boomen,&nbsp;Jan Buitelaar,&nbsp;Sabine Hunnius,&nbsp;Tineke M Snijders","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00074","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00074","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>During speech processing, neural activity in non-autistic adults and infants tracks the speech envelope. Recent research in adults indicates that this neural tracking relates to linguistic knowledge and may be reduced in autism. Such reduced tracking, if present already in infancy, could impede language development. In the current study, we focused on children with a family history of autism, who often show a delay in first language acquisition. We investigated whether differences in tracking of sung nursery rhymes during infancy relate to language development and autism symptoms in childhood. We assessed speech-brain coherence at either 10 or 14 months of age in a total of 22 infants with high likelihood of autism due to family history and 19 infants without family history of autism. We analyzed the relationship between speech-brain coherence in these infants and their vocabulary at 24 months as well as autism symptoms at 36 months. Our results showed significant speech-brain coherence in the 10- and 14-month-old infants. We found no evidence for a relationship between speech-brain coherence and later autism symptoms. Importantly, speech-brain coherence in the stressed syllable rate (1-3 Hz) predicted later vocabulary. Follow-up analyses showed evidence for a relationship between tracking and vocabulary only in 10-month-olds but not in 14-month-olds and indicated possible differences between the likelihood groups. Thus, early tracking of sung nursery rhymes is related to language development in childhood.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158647/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9504377","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Effects of Sleep on Language and Motor Consolidation: Evidence of Domain General and Specific Mechanisms. 睡眠对语言和运动巩固的影响:领域一般和特定机制的证据。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00060
Dafna Ben-Zion, Ella Gabitov, Anat Prior, Tali Bitan

The current study explores the effects of time and sleep on the consolidation of a novel language learning task containing both item-specific knowledge and the extraction of grammatical regularities. We also compare consolidation effects in language and motor sequence learning tasks, to ask whether consolidation mechanisms are domain general. Young adults learned to apply plural inflections to novel words based on morphophonological rules embedded in the input, and learned to type a motor sequence using a keyboard. Participants were randomly assigned into one of two groups, practicing each task during either the morning or evening hours. Both groups were retested 12 and 24 hours post-training. Performance on frequent trained items in the language task stabilized only following sleep, consistent with a hippocampal mechanism for item-specific learning. However, regularity extraction, indicated by generalization to untrained items in the linguistic task, as well as performance on motor sequence learning, improved 24 hours post-training, irrespective of the timing of sleep. This consolidation process is consistent with a frontostriatal skill-learning mechanism, common across the language and motor domains. This conclusion is further reinforced by cross-domain correlations at the individual level between improvement across 24 hours in the motor task and in the low-frequency trained items in the linguistic task, which involve regularity extraction. Taken together, our results at the group and individual levels suggest that some aspects of consolidation are shared across the motor and language domains, and more specifically, between motor sequence learning and grammar learning.

本研究探讨了时间和睡眠对包含特定项目知识和语法规则提取的新语言学习任务巩固的影响。我们还比较了语言和运动序列学习任务中的巩固效应,以了解巩固机制是否具有普遍性。年轻人学会了根据输入中嵌入的词形规则对新单词进行复数屈折,并学会了使用键盘输入一个运动序列。参与者被随机分为两组,在早上或晚上练习每项任务。两组在训练后12小时和24小时再次进行测试。在语言任务中,频繁训练项目的表现只有在睡眠后才稳定下来,这与海马体特定项目学习的机制一致。然而,无论睡眠时间如何,规律性提取(通过对语言任务中未训练项目的概化)以及运动序列学习的表现在训练后24小时均有所改善。这种巩固过程与额纹状体技能学习机制是一致的,在语言和运动领域都很常见。在个体水平上,运动任务和语言任务中涉及规则提取的低频训练项目在24小时内的改善之间的跨域相关性进一步强化了这一结论。综上所述,我们在群体和个人层面上的结果表明,在运动和语言领域,更具体地说,在运动序列学习和语法学习之间,巩固的某些方面是共享的。
{"title":"Effects of Sleep on Language and Motor Consolidation: Evidence of Domain General and Specific Mechanisms.","authors":"Dafna Ben-Zion,&nbsp;Ella Gabitov,&nbsp;Anat Prior,&nbsp;Tali Bitan","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00060","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00060","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The current study explores the effects of time and sleep on the consolidation of a novel language learning task containing both item-specific knowledge and the extraction of grammatical regularities. We also compare consolidation effects in language and motor sequence learning tasks, to ask whether consolidation mechanisms are domain general. Young adults learned to apply plural inflections to novel words based on morphophonological rules embedded in the input, and learned to type a motor sequence using a keyboard. Participants were randomly assigned into one of two groups, practicing each task during either the morning or evening hours. Both groups were retested 12 and 24 hours post-training. Performance on frequent trained items in the language task stabilized only following sleep, consistent with a hippocampal mechanism for item-specific learning. However, regularity extraction, indicated by generalization to untrained items in the linguistic task, as well as performance on motor sequence learning, improved 24 hours post-training, irrespective of the timing of sleep. This consolidation process is consistent with a frontostriatal skill-learning mechanism, common across the language and motor domains. This conclusion is further reinforced by cross-domain correlations at the individual level between improvement across 24 hours in the motor task and in the low-frequency trained items in the linguistic task, which involve regularity extraction. Taken together, our results at the group and individual levels suggest that some aspects of consolidation are shared across the motor and language domains, and more specifically, between motor sequence learning and grammar learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158628/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9504777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Can You Hear What's Coming? Failure to Replicate ERP Evidence for Phonological Prediction. 你能听到将要发生什么吗?语音预测的ERP证据复制失败。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00078
Victoria R Poulton, Mante S Nieuwland

Prediction-based theories of language comprehension assume that listeners predict both the meaning and phonological form of likely upcoming words. In alleged event-related potential (ERP) demonstrations of phonological prediction, prediction-mismatching words elicit a phonological mismatch negativity (PMN), a frontocentral negativity that precedes the centroparietal N400 component. However, classification and replicability of the PMN has proven controversial, with ongoing debate on whether the PMN is a distinct component or merely an early part of the N400. In this electroencephalography (EEG) study, we therefore attempted to replicate the PMN effect and its separability from the N400, using a participant sample size (N = 48) that was more than double that of previous studies. Participants listened to sentences containing either a predictable word or an unpredictable word with/without phonological overlap with the predictable word. Preregistered analyses revealed a widely distributed negative-going ERP in response to unpredictable words in both the early (150-250 ms) and the N400 (300-500 ms) time windows. Bayes factor analysis yielded moderate evidence against a different scalp distribution of the effects in the two time windows. Although our findings do not speak against phonological prediction during sentence comprehension, they do speak against the PMN effect specifically as a marker of phonological prediction mismatch. Instead of an PMN effect, our results demonstrate the early onset of the auditory N400 effect associated with unpredictable words. Our failure to replicate further highlights the risk associated with commonly employed data-contingent analyses (e.g., analyses involving time windows or electrodes that were selected based on visual inspection) and small sample sizes in the cognitive neuroscience of language.

基于预测的语言理解理论假设听者预测可能出现的单词的意义和语音形式。在所谓的事件相关电位(ERP)语音预测的示范中,预测不匹配的单词引发语音不匹配负性(PMN),这是一种先于中央顶叶N400成分的额中央负性。然而,PMN的分类和可复制性已被证明是有争议的,正在进行的辩论是PMN是一个独特的组成部分还是仅仅是N400的早期部分。因此,在这项脑电图(EEG)研究中,我们试图复制PMN效应及其与N400的可分离性,使用的参与者样本量(N = 48)是以前研究的两倍多。参与者听的句子要么包含一个可预测的单词,要么包含一个与可预测的单词有或没有语音重叠的不可预测的单词。预注册分析显示,在早期(150-250 ms)和N400 (300-500 ms)时间窗中,对不可预测单词的反应出现了广泛分布的负向ERP。贝叶斯因子分析在两个时间窗口中对不同头皮分布的影响产生了温和的证据。虽然我们的研究结果并不反对句子理解过程中的语音预测,但它们确实反对PMN效应作为语音预测不匹配的标志。而不是PMN效应,我们的研究结果表明,听觉N400效应与不可预测的单词有关。我们的失败复制进一步强调了语言认知神经科学中常用的数据偶然分析(例如,涉及时间窗口或基于视觉检查选择的电极的分析)和小样本量的风险。
{"title":"Can You Hear What's Coming? Failure to Replicate ERP Evidence for Phonological Prediction.","authors":"Victoria R Poulton,&nbsp;Mante S Nieuwland","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00078","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00078","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prediction-based theories of language comprehension assume that listeners predict both the meaning and phonological form of likely upcoming words. In alleged event-related potential (ERP) demonstrations of phonological prediction, prediction-mismatching words elicit a phonological mismatch negativity (PMN), a frontocentral negativity that precedes the centroparietal N400 component. However, classification and replicability of the PMN has proven controversial, with ongoing debate on whether the PMN is a distinct component or merely an early part of the N400. In this electroencephalography (EEG) study, we therefore attempted to replicate the PMN effect and its separability from the N400, using a participant sample size (<i>N</i> = 48) that was more than double that of previous studies. Participants listened to sentences containing either a predictable word or an unpredictable word with/without phonological overlap with the predictable word. Preregistered analyses revealed a widely distributed negative-going ERP in response to unpredictable words in both the early (150-250 ms) and the N400 (300-500 ms) time windows. Bayes factor analysis yielded moderate evidence against a different scalp distribution of the effects in the two time windows. Although our findings do not speak against phonological prediction during sentence comprehension, they do speak against the PMN effect specifically as a marker of phonological prediction mismatch. Instead of an PMN effect, our results demonstrate the early onset of the auditory N400 effect associated with unpredictable words. Our failure to replicate further highlights the risk associated with commonly employed data-contingent analyses (e.g., analyses involving time windows or electrodes that were selected based on visual inspection) and small sample sizes in the cognitive neuroscience of language.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158594/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9858912","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Event-Related Potential Correlates of Learning to Produce Novel Foreign Phonemes. 学习产生新外来语位的事件相关潜在关联。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00080
Henry Railo, Anni Varjonen, Minna Lehtonen, Pilleriin Sikka

Learning to pronounce a foreign phoneme requires an individual to acquire a motor program that enables the reproduction of the new acoustic target sound. This process is largely based on the use of auditory feedback to detect pronunciation errors to adjust vocalization. While early auditory evoked neural activity underlies automatic detection and adaptation to vocalization errors, little is known about the neural correlates of acquiring novel speech targets. To investigate the neural processes that mediate the learning of foreign phoneme pronunciation, we recorded event-related potentials when participants (N = 19) pronounced native or foreign phonemes. Behavioral results indicated that the participants' pronunciation of the foreign phoneme improved during the experiment. Early auditory responses (N1 and P2 waves, approximately 85-290 ms after the sound onset) revealed no differences between foreign and native phonemes. In contrast, the amplitude of the frontocentrally distributed late slow wave (LSW, 320-440 ms) was modulated by the pronunciation of the foreign phonemes, and the effect changed during the experiment, paralleling the improvement in pronunciation. These results suggest that the LSW may reflect higher-order monitoring processes that signal successful pronunciation and help learn novel phonemes.

学习一个外来音的发音需要一个人获得一个运动程序,使新的声学目标声音的再现。这个过程很大程度上是基于听觉反馈来检测发音错误来调整发声。虽然早期听觉诱发的神经活动是自动检测和适应发声错误的基础,但人们对获取新语音目标的神经关联知之甚少。为了研究介导外来音发音学习的神经过程,我们记录了参与者(N = 19)发音本地或外来音时的事件相关电位。行为实验结果表明,在实验过程中,参与者对外来音的发音有所提高。早期听觉反应(N1和P2波,大约在声音开始后85-290 ms)显示外来语和母语音素之间没有差异。与此相反,外音发音对额中央分布的晚慢波(LSW, 320 ~ 440 ms)振幅的调制作用在实验过程中发生了变化,且与发音的改善同步。这些结果表明,LSW可能反映了高阶的监测过程,这些过程标志着成功的发音,并有助于学习新的音素。
{"title":"Event-Related Potential Correlates of Learning to Produce Novel Foreign Phonemes.","authors":"Henry Railo,&nbsp;Anni Varjonen,&nbsp;Minna Lehtonen,&nbsp;Pilleriin Sikka","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00080","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00080","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Learning to pronounce a foreign phoneme requires an individual to acquire a motor program that enables the reproduction of the new acoustic target sound. This process is largely based on the use of auditory feedback to detect pronunciation errors to adjust vocalization. While early auditory evoked neural activity underlies automatic detection and adaptation to vocalization errors, little is known about the neural correlates of acquiring novel speech targets. To investigate the neural processes that mediate the learning of foreign phoneme pronunciation, we recorded event-related potentials when participants (<i>N</i> = 19) pronounced native or foreign phonemes. Behavioral results indicated that the participants' pronunciation of the foreign phoneme improved during the experiment. Early auditory responses (N1 and P2 waves, approximately 85-290 ms after the sound onset) revealed no differences between foreign and native phonemes. In contrast, the amplitude of the frontocentrally distributed late slow wave (LSW, 320-440 ms) was modulated by the pronunciation of the foreign phonemes, and the effect changed during the experiment, paralleling the improvement in pronunciation. These results suggest that the LSW may reflect higher-order monitoring processes that signal successful pronunciation and help learn novel phonemes.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158638/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9858908","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Conceptual Combination in the LATL With and Without Syntactic Composition. 有和没有句法组合的LATL概念组合。
IF 3.2 Q1 Social Sciences Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1162/nol_a_00048
Alicia Parrish, Liina Pylkkänen

The relationship among syntactic, semantic, and conceptual processes in language comprehension is a central question to the neurobiology of language. Several studies have suggested that conceptual combination in particular can be localized to the left anterior temporal lobe (LATL), while syntactic processes are more often associated with the posterior temporal lobe or inferior frontal gyrus. However, LATL activity can also correlate with syntactic computations, particularly in narrative comprehension. Here we investigated the degree to which LATL conceptual combination is dependent on syntax, specifically asking whether rapid (∼200 ms) magnetoencephalography effects of conceptual combination in the LATL can occur in the absence of licit syntactic phrase closure and in the absence of a semantically plausible output for the composition. We find that such effects do occur: LATL effects of conceptual combination were observed even when there was no syntactic phrase closure or plausible meaning. But syntactic closure did have an additive effect such that LATL signals were the highest for expressions that composed both conceptually and syntactically. Our findings conform to an account in which LATL conceptual composition is influenced by local syntactic composition but is also able to operate without it.

语言理解中的句法、语义和概念过程之间的关系是语言神经生物学的一个核心问题。一些研究表明,概念组合特别可能定位于左前颞叶(LATL),而句法过程通常与后颞叶或额下回有关。然而,LATL活动也与句法计算有关,特别是在叙事理解中。在这里,我们研究了LATL概念组合对语法的依赖程度,特别是询问在没有合法的句法短语关闭和没有语义上合理的输出的情况下,LATL概念组合的快速(~ 200 ms)脑磁图效应是否会发生。我们发现这种效应确实存在:概念组合的LATL效应即使在没有句法短语闭合或似是而非的意义时也被观察到。但是语法闭包确实有一个累加效应,这样LATL信号在概念和语法上都组成的表达式中是最高的。我们的研究结果符合一种说法,即LATL概念组成受到局部句法组成的影响,但也能够在没有它的情况下运作。
{"title":"Conceptual Combination in the LATL With and Without Syntactic Composition.","authors":"Alicia Parrish,&nbsp;Liina Pylkkänen","doi":"10.1162/nol_a_00048","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1162/nol_a_00048","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The relationship among syntactic, semantic, and conceptual processes in language comprehension is a central question to the neurobiology of language. Several studies have suggested that conceptual combination in particular can be localized to the left anterior temporal lobe (LATL), while syntactic processes are more often associated with the posterior temporal lobe or inferior frontal gyrus. However, LATL activity can also correlate with syntactic computations, particularly in narrative comprehension. Here we investigated the degree to which LATL conceptual combination is dependent on syntax, specifically asking whether rapid (∼200 ms) magnetoencephalography effects of conceptual combination in the LATL can occur in the absence of licit syntactic phrase closure and in the absence of a semantically plausible output for the composition. We find that such effects do occur: LATL effects of conceptual combination were observed even when there was no syntactic phrase closure or plausible meaning. But syntactic closure did have an additive effect such that LATL signals were the highest for expressions that composed both conceptually and syntactically. Our findings conform to an account in which LATL conceptual composition is influenced by local syntactic composition but is also able to operate without it.</p>","PeriodicalId":34845,"journal":{"name":"Neurobiology of Language","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.2,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10158584/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9875130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Neurobiology of Language
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1