首页 > 最新文献

CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION最新文献

英文 中文
Kitsch and Perception: Towards a New ‘Aesthetic from Below’ 媚俗与感知:走向新的“自下而上的美学”
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-03-06 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-00001091
Stefan Ortlieb, C. Carbon
Although kitsch is one of the most important concepts of twentieth-century art theory, it has gone widely unnoticed by empirical aesthetics. In this article we make a case that the study of kitsch is of considerable heuristic value for both empirical aesthetics and art perception. As a descriptive term, kitsch appears like a perfect example of hedonic fluency. In fact, the frequently invoked opposition of kitsch and art reflects two types of aesthetic experience that can be reliably distinguished in terms of processing dynamics: a disfluent one that promises new insights but requires cognitive elaboration (art), and a fluent one that consists of an immediate, unreflective emotional response but leaves us with what we already know (kitsch). Yet as a derogatory word, kitsch draws our attention to a general disregard for effortless emotional gratification in modern Western aesthetics that can be traced back to eighteenth-century Rationalism. Despite all efforts of Pop Art to embrace kitsch and to question normative values in art, current models of aesthetic liking—including fluency-based ones—still adhere to an elitist notion of Modern art that privileges style over content and thereby excludes what is essential not only for popular taste and Postmodern art but also for premodern artistic production: emotionally rich content. Revisiting Fechner’s (Vorschule der Aesthetik, 1876) criticism of highbrow aesthetics we propose a new aesthetic from below (Aesthetik von Unten) that goes beyond processing characteristics by taking content- and context-related associations into account.
虽然媚俗是二十世纪艺术理论中最重要的概念之一,但它却被经验主义美学所广泛忽视。在本文中,我们提出了一个案例,即媚俗的研究对于经验美学和艺术感知都具有相当大的启发式价值。作为一个描述性术语,媚俗似乎是享乐流畅的完美例子。事实上,经常被提及的媚俗和艺术的对立反映了两种类型的审美体验,它们可以根据处理动态可靠地区分开来:一种是不流畅的,它承诺新的见解,但需要认知上的阐述(艺术),一种是流畅的,它由直接的、不反思的情感反应组成,但留给我们的是我们已经知道的(媚俗)。然而,作为一个贬义词,媚俗引起了我们对现代西方美学中对毫不费力的情感满足的普遍漠视,这种审美可以追溯到18世纪的理性主义。尽管波普艺术努力拥抱媚俗,质疑艺术的规范价值,但目前的审美喜好模式——包括以流畅为基础的模式——仍然坚持现代艺术的精英主义观念,认为风格高于内容,从而排除了不仅对流行品味和后现代艺术,而且对前现代艺术生产至关重要的东西:情感丰富的内容。回顾费希纳(《美学》,1876)对高雅美学的批评,我们提出了一种从下而上的新美学,它通过考虑内容和上下文相关的关联而超越了加工特征。
{"title":"Kitsch and Perception: Towards a New ‘Aesthetic from Below’","authors":"Stefan Ortlieb, C. Carbon","doi":"10.1163/22134913-00001091","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-00001091","url":null,"abstract":"Although kitsch is one of the most important concepts of twentieth-century art theory, it has gone widely unnoticed by empirical aesthetics. In this article we make a case that the study of kitsch is of considerable heuristic value for both empirical aesthetics and art perception. As a descriptive term, kitsch appears like a perfect example of hedonic fluency. In fact, the frequently invoked opposition of kitsch and art reflects two types of aesthetic experience that can be reliably distinguished in terms of processing dynamics: a disfluent one that promises new insights but requires cognitive elaboration (art), and a fluent one that consists of an immediate, unreflective emotional response but leaves us with what we already know (kitsch). Yet as a derogatory word, kitsch draws our attention to a general disregard for effortless emotional gratification in modern Western aesthetics that can be traced back to eighteenth-century Rationalism. Despite all efforts of Pop Art to embrace kitsch and to question normative values in art, current models of aesthetic liking—including fluency-based ones—still adhere to an elitist notion of Modern art that privileges style over content and thereby excludes what is essential not only for popular taste and Postmodern art but also for premodern artistic production: emotionally rich content. Revisiting Fechner’s (Vorschule der Aesthetik, 1876) criticism of highbrow aesthetics we propose a new aesthetic from below (Aesthetik von Unten) that goes beyond processing characteristics by taking content- and context-related associations into account.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88527042","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Narrative Sequence Position of Close-Ups Influences Cognitive and Affective Processing and Facilitates Theory of Mind 特写叙事序列位置影响认知和情感加工,促进心理理论
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-03-06 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-20191095
K. Bálint, B. Rooney
Close-up shots have been shown to modulate affective, cognitive and theory-of-mind responding to visual narratives. However, the role of close-up’s narrative-sequence position, that is the relative timing of close-up shots in a visual narrative, is largely unknown. Participants watched one of ten versions of the same animated film, after we inserted a close-up shot (neutral or a sad face) at one of five different time points. Story recall responses of 168 participants were analyzed by the Linguistic Inquiry of Word Count, a computerized content analysis software, and coded manually for theory of mind. The narrative-sequence position of the close-up influenced the level of cognitive processing, affective processing, and theory of mind evident in participant responses where a U-shaped relationship was observed for the close-up position. These findings further our understanding of how close-ups affect narrative processing and are of relevance for studies on formal features in visual narratives.
特写镜头已被证明可以调节对视觉叙事的情感、认知和心理理论反应。然而,特写镜头的叙事序列位置的作用,即特写镜头在视觉叙事中的相对时间,在很大程度上是未知的。在我们在五个不同的时间点插入特写镜头(中性或悲伤的脸)之后,参与者观看了同一部动画电影的十个版本中的一个。采用计算机化内容分析软件“词数语言调查”对168名被试的故事回忆进行分析,并进行人工编码。特写镜头的叙事顺序位置影响认知加工、情感加工和心理理论的水平,这在参与者的反应中很明显,其中特写镜头位置观察到u形关系。这些发现进一步加深了我们对特写镜头如何影响叙事加工的理解,并对视觉叙事的形式特征研究具有重要意义。
{"title":"Narrative Sequence Position of Close-Ups Influences Cognitive and Affective Processing and Facilitates Theory of Mind","authors":"K. Bálint, B. Rooney","doi":"10.1163/22134913-20191095","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20191095","url":null,"abstract":"Close-up shots have been shown to modulate affective, cognitive and theory-of-mind responding to visual narratives. However, the role of close-up’s narrative-sequence position, that is the relative timing of close-up shots in a visual narrative, is largely unknown. Participants watched one of ten versions of the same animated film, after we inserted a close-up shot (neutral or a sad face) at one of five different time points. Story recall responses of 168 participants were analyzed by the Linguistic Inquiry of Word Count, a computerized content analysis software, and coded manually for theory of mind. The narrative-sequence position of the close-up influenced the level of cognitive processing, affective processing, and theory of mind evident in participant responses where a U-shaped relationship was observed for the close-up position. These findings further our understanding of how close-ups affect narrative processing and are of relevance for studies on formal features in visual narratives.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85011319","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Aesthetic Stimuli Attract Visual Spatial Attention 审美刺激吸引视觉空间注意
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2019-03-06 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-20191101
B. Rolke, Madeleine Y. Stepper, Verena C. Seibold, Elisabeth Hein
We investigated the relationship between attention and the aesthetic appearance of visual stimuli. In a first study, we evaluated the aesthetic value of pictures depicting chairs by means of a questionnaire. These pictures were categorized as aesthetic, neutral, or non-aesthetic and used in a second study, in which we employed a dot-probe task and a recognition memory task. In the dot-probe task, two pictures of chairs were presented to the left and to the right of fixation, followed by a dot at one of the chair pictures’ positions. Participants decided at which side the dot had been presented. To investigate the relation between the aesthetic value of the chairs and the orientation of attention, we paired either aesthetic or non-aesthetic pictures with neutral pictures. The results showed that participants reacted faster when the dot appeared at the position of the aesthetic chair than when it appeared at the position of the neutral one. Such a ‘congruency’ effect was absent for non-aesthetic chairs. This interactive pattern of results shows that aesthetic stimuli capture attention. In the recognition memory task, in which participants were asked to decide whether a chair had been presented before or not, aesthetic chairs were more accurately and faster recognized than neutral or non-aesthetic ones. Taken together, these results show that aesthetic stimuli entail prioritized cognitive processing. In a final study, we investigated which particular features of the aesthetic stimuli might be important for this effect by correlating the aesthetic evaluation of the pictures with their Gestalt impression.
我们研究了注意与视觉刺激的审美外观之间的关系。在第一项研究中,我们通过问卷调查的方式评估了描绘椅子的图片的美学价值。这些图片被分类为美学、中性或非美学,并在第二个研究中使用,在这个研究中我们采用了点探测任务和识别记忆任务。在点探测任务中,在固定的左侧和右侧分别呈现两张椅子图片,然后在椅子图片的一个位置上放置一个点。参与者决定圆点出现在哪一边。为了研究椅子的审美价值与注意取向之间的关系,我们将审美或非审美图片与中性图片配对。结果表明,当圆点出现在审美椅的位置时,参与者的反应要比出现在中性椅的位置时快。这种“一致性”效应在非美学椅子上是不存在的。这种交互模式的结果表明,审美刺激吸引注意力。在识别记忆任务中,参与者被要求判断一把椅子之前是否出现过,审美椅子比中性或非审美椅子更准确、更快地被识别出来。综上所述,这些结果表明审美刺激需要优先的认知处理。在最后的研究中,我们通过将图片的审美评价与其格式塔印象联系起来,研究了审美刺激的哪些特定特征可能对这种效果很重要。
{"title":"Aesthetic Stimuli Attract Visual Spatial Attention","authors":"B. Rolke, Madeleine Y. Stepper, Verena C. Seibold, Elisabeth Hein","doi":"10.1163/22134913-20191101","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20191101","url":null,"abstract":"We investigated the relationship between attention and the aesthetic appearance of visual stimuli. In a first study, we evaluated the aesthetic value of pictures depicting chairs by means of a questionnaire. These pictures were categorized as aesthetic, neutral, or non-aesthetic and used in a second study, in which we employed a dot-probe task and a recognition memory task. In the dot-probe task, two pictures of chairs were presented to the left and to the right of fixation, followed by a dot at one of the chair pictures’ positions. Participants decided at which side the dot had been presented. To investigate the relation between the aesthetic value of the chairs and the orientation of attention, we paired either aesthetic or non-aesthetic pictures with neutral pictures. The results showed that participants reacted faster when the dot appeared at the position of the aesthetic chair than when it appeared at the position of the neutral one. Such a ‘congruency’ effect was absent for non-aesthetic chairs. This interactive pattern of results shows that aesthetic stimuli capture attention. In the recognition memory task, in which participants were asked to decide whether a chair had been presented before or not, aesthetic chairs were more accurately and faster recognized than neutral or non-aesthetic ones. Taken together, these results show that aesthetic stimuli entail prioritized cognitive processing. In a final study, we investigated which particular features of the aesthetic stimuli might be important for this effect by correlating the aesthetic evaluation of the pictures with their Gestalt impression.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2019-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89900199","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Abstracts from the 6th Visual Science of Art Conference (VSAC) 第六届视觉艺术科学会议(VSAC)摘要
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-11-28 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-00604001
{"title":"Abstracts from the 6th Visual Science of Art Conference (VSAC)","authors":"","doi":"10.1163/22134913-00604001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-00604001","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-11-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"86880027","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Visual Image Statistics in the History of Western Art 西方美术史上的视觉形象统计
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-10-17 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-20181092
G. Mather
The history of Western visual art is traditionally divided into a succession of stylistic movements on the basis of the art-historical provenance and visual qualities of artworks. Little is known about how the visual statistics of Western artworks have changed over time, though this data could inform debate about the transitions between art movements. This longitudinal statistical study shows that two measures of the statistics of Western paintings remained relatively stable for 500 years, and similar to the values found in photographic images depicting the same subjects. Dramatic changes began in the late nineteenth century between the years 1878 and 1891, when the statistics of artworks became steadily more variable, and more frequently departed from values that are typical of representational images. This period can be considered as a major turning point that marks the beginning of the Modern Art movement. Statistically, abstract Modern art is more diverse than the representational art of any period. There is only limited evidence that aesthetic responses to paintings bear any relation to their visual statistics.
传统上,西方视觉艺术史根据艺术史的起源和艺术作品的视觉品质划分为一系列风格运动。西方艺术作品的视觉统计数据是如何随着时间的推移而变化的,人们知之甚少,尽管这些数据可以为有关艺术运动之间过渡的辩论提供信息。这项纵向统计研究表明,西方绘画的两种统计方法在500年内保持相对稳定,与描绘同一主题的摄影图像的值相似。19世纪末1878年至1891年间,艺术作品的统计数据开始发生戏剧性的变化,当时艺术作品的统计数据变得越来越多变,而且越来越频繁地偏离典型的代表性图像的价值。这一时期可以被认为是标志着现代艺术运动开始的一个重要转折点。据统计,抽象的现代艺术比任何时期的具象艺术都更加多样化。只有有限的证据表明,对绘画的审美反应与它们的视觉统计有任何关系。
{"title":"Visual Image Statistics in the History of Western Art","authors":"G. Mather","doi":"10.1163/22134913-20181092","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20181092","url":null,"abstract":"The history of Western visual art is traditionally divided into a succession of stylistic movements on the basis of the art-historical provenance and visual qualities of artworks. Little is known about how the visual statistics of Western artworks have changed over time, though this data could inform debate about the transitions between art movements. This longitudinal statistical study shows that two measures of the statistics of Western paintings remained relatively stable for 500 years, and similar to the values found in photographic images depicting the same subjects. Dramatic changes began in the late nineteenth century between the years 1878 and 1891, when the statistics of artworks became steadily more variable, and more frequently departed from values that are typical of representational images. This period can be considered as a major turning point that marks the beginning of the Modern Art movement. Statistically, abstract Modern art is more diverse than the representational art of any period. There is only limited evidence that aesthetic responses to paintings bear any relation to their visual statistics.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78245822","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Illusory Depth Based on Interactions Between Fluorescent and Conventional Colours: A Case Study on Frank Stella’s Irregular Polygons Paintings 基于荧光与常规色彩相互作用的错觉深度——以Frank Stella的不规则多边形绘画为例
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-10-17 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-20181093
S. D. Winter, Pieter Moors, H. V. Gelder, J. Wagemans
Although Frank Stella intended to create flat, illusion-less Irregular Polygons paintings, it is not uncommon to experience the illusion of colour depth, based on the interaction between their fluorescent and conventional colours. Some critics praised these artworks’ flatness, while others described odd depth experiences that they categorised as a new kind of illusion. In order to provide a correct reading of these works and to reassess their art historical significance, a scientific case study regarding this colour-depth effect imposes itself. This article discusses an experiment in which we determined whether twenty artists, twenty art historians and twenty laypeople experienced fluorescent colours as protruding, receding or flat in combination with conventional colours. We additionally looked at whether they still perceived colour depth when all fluorescent colours were replaced with their conventional variants. All participants observed fifteen designs, which they had to rate according to the perceived depth of each coloured region with a number between −3 (strongly receding) and +3 (strongly protruding). The results revealed that most participants experience fluorescent regions as strongly protruding, unlike all conventional colours, which were rated as much less protruding. When a fluorescent colour was swapped with a conventional variant, all participants experienced significantly less depth. The differences between the subject groups were statistically negligible when looking at the mean depth ratings for both colour types. However, we discovered that artists experienced more contrast effects, as they gave different ratings to different panels (of identical colour and shape) in the same design, depending on their position.
尽管Frank Stella打算创作平面的、无错觉的不规则多边形绘画,但基于荧光色和传统色彩之间的相互作用,体验色彩深度的错觉并不罕见。一些评论家称赞这些艺术品的平面,而另一些人则描述了奇怪的深度体验,他们将其归类为一种新的错觉。为了提供对这些作品的正确解读,并重新评估它们的艺术史意义,对这种颜色深度效应进行了科学的案例研究。本文讨论了一个实验,我们确定了20位艺术家,20位艺术史学家和20位外行人在与传统颜色结合时,荧光颜色是突出的,后退的还是平坦的。此外,我们还观察了当所有的荧光颜色都被传统的变体所取代时,它们是否仍然感知到颜色的深度。所有参与者观察了15种设计,他们必须根据每个彩色区域的感知深度,用−3(强烈后退)和+3(强烈突出)之间的数字对它们进行评分。结果显示,大多数参与者体验到荧光区域是强烈突出的,不像所有传统颜色,它们被评为不那么突出。当荧光颜色与传统颜色交换时,所有参与者的深度都明显减少。当观察两种颜色类型的平均深度评级时,受试者组之间的差异在统计上可以忽略不计。然而,我们发现艺术家体验到更多的对比效果,因为他们对相同设计的不同面板(相同颜色和形状)给出了不同的评分,这取决于他们的位置。
{"title":"Illusory Depth Based on Interactions Between Fluorescent and Conventional Colours: A Case Study on Frank Stella’s Irregular Polygons Paintings","authors":"S. D. Winter, Pieter Moors, H. V. Gelder, J. Wagemans","doi":"10.1163/22134913-20181093","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20181093","url":null,"abstract":"Although Frank Stella intended to create flat, illusion-less Irregular Polygons paintings, it is not uncommon to experience the illusion of colour depth, based on the interaction between their fluorescent and conventional colours. Some critics praised these artworks’ flatness, while others described odd depth experiences that they categorised as a new kind of illusion. In order to provide a correct reading of these works and to reassess their art historical significance, a scientific case study regarding this colour-depth effect imposes itself. This article discusses an experiment in which we determined whether twenty artists, twenty art historians and twenty laypeople experienced fluorescent colours as protruding, receding or flat in combination with conventional colours. We additionally looked at whether they still perceived colour depth when all fluorescent colours were replaced with their conventional variants. All participants observed fifteen designs, which they had to rate according to the perceived depth of each coloured region with a number between −3 (strongly receding) and +3 (strongly protruding). The results revealed that most participants experience fluorescent regions as strongly protruding, unlike all conventional colours, which were rated as much less protruding. When a fluorescent colour was swapped with a conventional variant, all participants experienced significantly less depth. The differences between the subject groups were statistically negligible when looking at the mean depth ratings for both colour types. However, we discovered that artists experienced more contrast effects, as they gave different ratings to different panels (of identical colour and shape) in the same design, depending on their position.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79912858","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Gericault’s Fake-Gallop Horse Judged Speedy but Unrealistic Gericault的假飞奔马被认为是快速但不现实的
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2018-10-17 DOI: 10.1163/22134913-20181094
S. Mastandrea, J. Kennedy
In two experiments, we tested pictures of horse gaits—alt (standing), walk, trot, gallop, and a fake gallop, a pose shown in a well-known Gericault painting. The pose was portrayed frequently in the nineteenth century, its features hotly debated. Fake gallop has legs extended fore and rear, close to parallel to the ground. Experiment 1 sampled real artworks depicting horses and Experiment 2 used silhouettes of horses. In both, reports of amount of movement increased from alt to fake gallop. In Experiment 1 similar results were obtained from novices and equestrians (‘experts’ familiar with horses). The extreme leg extension in fake gallop may suggest high speed, as Arnheim suggested. However, true gallop includes legs curled close together under the body—a ‘running pony’ pose—so both extremes of extension may suggest high speed. In Experiment 2, novices judged fake gallop unrealistic despite giving high movement scores. We suggest its depiction may be metaphoric, meaning a deliberately false item has relevant features of a referent. For the artworks, the amount of movement reported correlated positively but quite modestly with aesthetic appreciation, but for the silhouettes, the correlation was reversed. We suggest expression can be positive for many horse poses.
在两个实验中,我们测试了马匹步态的图片——站立、行走、小跑、疾驰和假疾驰,这是一幅著名的格里柯画作中的一种姿势。这个姿势在19世纪经常被描绘出来,它的特征引起了激烈的争论。假驰的腿前后伸展,接近与地面平行。实验1选取了描绘马的真实艺术品,实验2使用了马的剪影。在这两种情况下,报告的移动量从alt增加到假飞奔。在实验1中,从新手和骑手(熟悉马的“专家”)身上得到了类似的结果。正如阿恩海姆所指出的那样,假跑中腿部的极端伸展可能意味着高速。然而,真正的疾驰包括腿在身体下面紧密地蜷曲在一起——一种“奔跑的小马”的姿势——所以两种极端的伸展都可能意味着高速。在实验2中,尽管新手给出了很高的动作分数,但他们认为假飞奔是不现实的。我们认为它的描述可能是隐喻性的,这意味着一个故意虚假的项目具有指称物的相关特征。对于艺术品来说,运动的数量与审美欣赏呈正相关,但相关性不大,但对于轮廓,相关性相反。我们建议,对于许多马的姿势,表情可以是积极的。
{"title":"Gericault’s Fake-Gallop Horse Judged Speedy but Unrealistic","authors":"S. Mastandrea, J. Kennedy","doi":"10.1163/22134913-20181094","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1163/22134913-20181094","url":null,"abstract":"In two experiments, we tested pictures of horse gaits—alt (standing), walk, trot, gallop, and a fake gallop, a pose shown in a well-known Gericault painting. The pose was portrayed frequently in the nineteenth century, its features hotly debated. Fake gallop has legs extended fore and rear, close to parallel to the ground. Experiment 1 sampled real artworks depicting horses and Experiment 2 used silhouettes of horses. In both, reports of amount of movement increased from alt to fake gallop. In Experiment 1 similar results were obtained from novices and equestrians (‘experts’ familiar with horses). The extreme leg extension in fake gallop may suggest high speed, as Arnheim suggested. However, true gallop includes legs curled close together under the body—a ‘running pony’ pose—so both extremes of extension may suggest high speed. In Experiment 2, novices judged fake gallop unrealistic despite giving high movement scores. We suggest its depiction may be metaphoric, meaning a deliberately false item has relevant features of a referent. For the artworks, the amount of movement reported correlated positively but quite modestly with aesthetic appreciation, but for the silhouettes, the correlation was reversed. We suggest expression can be positive for many horse poses.","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2018-10-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"78297288","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Spirit Guards: A Squad of Ceramic Dogs in South Africa 精神护卫:一队陶瓷狗在南非
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2017-01-01 DOI: 10.1007/978-3-319-56874-4_5
N. Swanepoel
{"title":"Spirit Guards: A Squad of Ceramic Dogs in South Africa","authors":"N. Swanepoel","doi":"10.1007/978-3-319-56874-4_5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-56874-4_5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2017-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83627246","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Remembrance of things past 对过去的回忆
4区 艺术学 Q4 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 1900-01-01 DOI: 10.1016/b978-0-12-460480-3.50011-0
Martin Kerby
{"title":"Remembrance of things past","authors":"Martin Kerby","doi":"10.1016/b978-0-12-460480-3.50011-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1016/b978-0-12-460480-3.50011-0","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":42895,"journal":{"name":"CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"1900-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77984875","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"艺术学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
CERAMICS-ART AND PERCEPTION
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1