首页 > 最新文献

Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews最新文献

英文 中文
Attending to Others’ Visual Attention 注意他人的视觉注意力
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_7
James R. Anderson, Yuko Hattori
{"title":"Attending to Others’ Visual Attention","authors":"James R. Anderson, Yuko Hattori","doi":"10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_7","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"90922232","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Factors Affecting Facial Recognition in Capuchin Monkeys 影响卷尾猴面部识别的因素
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2021-01-01 DOI: 10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_5
C. Hiramatsu
{"title":"Factors Affecting Facial Recognition in Capuchin Monkeys","authors":"C. Hiramatsu","doi":"10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-16-2028-7_5","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2021-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74402513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
What can we learn about navigation from associative learning? 我们能从联想学习中学到关于导航的什么?
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001
A. McGregor
The many cue types that animals are able to use for both longand short-scale navigation have been studied extensively, and a well-researched literature has developed into the strategies that have evolved to exploit information provided by these different cues. Less well understood are questions of how animals select different cues to learn about, and the conditions for learning based on these cues. These queries have tended to concern psychologists interested in the extent to which the principles of associative learning apply to spatial learning. The question is of interest because the predictions of associative learning theories are often at odds with spatial learning theories, which instead tend to emphasize the special types of representation and learning process necessary for navigation. Here I examine spatial learning from an associative perspective, starting with the question of what kinds of associations are formed in associative learning and how these may fit within our knowledge of spatial learning. I then examine the conditions of learning, including the effects of prior experience on spatial learning in terms of both latent inhibition and perceptual learning, changes to the attention paid to spatial stimuli as a result of their predictive history, and the extent to which redundancy — when multiple cues predict same outcome — affects learning. The effects are illustrated mostly with examples from the associative learning literature, which is often with rodents or pigeons. But where possible, I have demonstrated similar effects in more diverse species and have tried to indicate the general learning effects that associative learning theories predict.
动物能够用于长尺度和短尺度导航的许多线索类型已经得到了广泛的研究,并且研究充分的文献已经发展到利用这些不同线索提供的信息的策略。动物是如何选择不同的线索来学习的,以及基于这些线索的学习条件,这些问题还不太清楚。这些问题往往引起心理学家对联想学习原理在多大程度上适用于空间学习的兴趣。这个问题很有趣,因为联想学习理论的预测往往与空间学习理论不一致,而空间学习理论倾向于强调导航所必需的特殊类型的表征和学习过程。在这里,我从联想的角度来研究空间学习,从联想学习中形成什么样的联想以及这些联想如何与我们的空间学习知识相适应的问题开始。然后,我研究了学习的条件,包括先前的经验对空间学习的影响,包括潜在抑制和知觉学习,由于预测历史而对空间刺激的关注的变化,以及冗余的程度-当多个线索预测相同的结果时-影响学习。这种影响主要是用联想学习文献中的例子来说明的,这些文献通常是关于啮齿动物或鸽子的。但在可能的情况下,我已经在更多不同的物种中证明了类似的效果,并试图指出联想学习理论所预测的一般学习效果。
{"title":"What can we learn about navigation from associative learning?","authors":"A. McGregor","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001","url":null,"abstract":"The many cue types that animals are able to use for both longand short-scale navigation have been studied extensively, and a well-researched literature has developed into the strategies that have evolved to exploit information provided by these different cues. Less well understood are questions of how animals select different cues to learn about, and the conditions for learning based on these cues. These queries have tended to concern psychologists interested in the extent to which the principles of associative learning apply to spatial learning. The question is of interest because the predictions of associative learning theories are often at odds with spatial learning theories, which instead tend to emphasize the special types of representation and learning process necessary for navigation. Here I examine spatial learning from an associative perspective, starting with the question of what kinds of associations are formed in associative learning and how these may fit within our knowledge of spatial learning. I then examine the conditions of learning, including the effects of prior experience on spatial learning in terms of both latent inhibition and perceptual learning, changes to the attention paid to spatial stimuli as a result of their predictive history, and the extent to which redundancy — when multiple cues predict same outcome — affects learning. The effects are illustrated mostly with examples from the associative learning literature, which is often with rodents or pigeons. But where possible, I have demonstrated similar effects in more diverse species and have tried to indicate the general learning effects that associative learning theories predict.","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235207","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
What Suboptimal Choice Tells Us About the Control of Behavior 次优选择告诉我们的行为控制
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001e
A. McGregor
{"title":"What Suboptimal Choice Tells Us About the Control of Behavior","authors":"A. McGregor","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001e","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150001e","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235220","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Functional performance of the visual system in dogs and humans: A comparative perspective 狗和人类视觉系统的功能表现:比较视角
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150002
A. Barber, D. Mills, F. Montealegre-Z, Victoria F. Ratcliffe, K. Guo, A. Wilkinson
Dogs and humans often work together, and dogs have been selectively bred for traits enabling efficient co-operation with humans, such as tameness (Miklósi, 2008), sensitivity to human cues (Bräuer, Kaminski, Riedel, Call, & Tomasello, 2006; Cooper et al., 2003; Virányi, Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2004), and specialized skills employed by specific types of working dog (Lord, Schneider, & Coppinger, 2016; McKinley & Sambrook, 2000; Miklósi, 2008). As a result, dogs are integral to society and have both important value (e.g., companion, A. L. A. Barber School of Life Sciences and School of Psychology University of Lincoln D. S. Mills and F. Montealegre-Z School of Life Sciences University of Lincoln
狗和人类经常一起工作,狗被选择性地培育出能够与人类有效合作的特征,例如温顺(Miklósi, 2008),对人类暗示的敏感性(Bräuer, Kaminski, Riedel, Call, & Tomasello, 2006;Cooper et al., 2003;Virányi, Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2004),以及特定类型工作犬所使用的专业技能(Lord, Schneider, & Coppinger, 2016;McKinley & Sambrook, 2000;Miklosi, 2008)。因此,狗是社会不可或缺的一部分,具有重要的价值(例如,伴侣,林肯大学a . L. a . Barber生命科学学院和心理学院,林肯大学D. S. Mills和F. montealegrei - z生命科学学院)
{"title":"Functional performance of the visual system in dogs and humans: A comparative perspective","authors":"A. Barber, D. Mills, F. Montealegre-Z, Victoria F. Ratcliffe, K. Guo, A. Wilkinson","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150002","url":null,"abstract":"Dogs and humans often work together, and dogs have been selectively bred for traits enabling efficient co-operation with humans, such as tameness (Miklósi, 2008), sensitivity to human cues (Bräuer, Kaminski, Riedel, Call, & Tomasello, 2006; Cooper et al., 2003; Virányi, Topál, Gácsi, Miklósi, & Csányi, 2004), and specialized skills employed by specific types of working dog (Lord, Schneider, & Coppinger, 2016; McKinley & Sambrook, 2000; Miklósi, 2008). As a result, dogs are integral to society and have both important value (e.g., companion, A. L. A. Barber School of Life Sciences and School of Psychology University of Lincoln D. S. Mills and F. Montealegre-Z School of Life Sciences University of Lincoln","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235279","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Avian olfaction: a review of the recent literature 鸟类嗅觉:近期文献综述
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150005
V. Abankwah, D. Deeming, T. W. Pike
Birds can smell. Despite early assumptions that birds had little or no sense of smell (Audubon, 1826; Hill, 1905; Stager, 1964), there is now abundant evidence that they are endowed with all the anatomical and neurobiological components necessary for a functional olfactory sense (Balthazart & Taziaux, 2009; Caro, Balthazart, & Bonadonna, 2015), and morphologically the olfactory systems of birds closely resembles those of amphibians, reptiles, and mammals (Kare & Mason, 1986; Wenzel, 1987). Most birds have paired external nares through which they breathe air, internal nasal cavities that contain olfactory epithelium, functional olfactory receptors, and a neuronal connection to the olfactory bulb in the forebrain (Jones & Roper, 1997). Moreover, they have been shown to use odor cues for tasks as diverse as foraging (e.g., Healy & Guilford, 1990; Nevitt, Loosekoot, & Weimerskirch, 2008; Potier, Duriez, Celerier, Liegeois, & Bonadonna, 2019), recognizing eggs (Leclaire, Bourret, & Bonadonna, 2017), selecting nest material (e.g., Gwinner, 2013), and avoiding predation (e.g., Amo, Galvan, Tomás, & Sanz, 2008), as well as in social contexts such as species (e.g., Krause et al., 2014), kin (e.g., Bonadonna & Sanz-Aguilar, 2012; Coffin, Watters, & Mateo, 2011; Krause et al., 2012), and mate recognition (e.g., Bonadonna & Nevitt, 2004). However, we argue that even today there is a tendency for researchers to underappreciate the possible role olfaction plays in birds’ everyday lives. Compelling evidence shows that vision and, to a lesser extent, hearing are the primary avian senses (Martin, 2017). By contrast, few bird species smell noticeably to humans (although there are notable exceptions, such as Crested auklets [Aethia cristatella]; Hagelin, Jones, & Rasmussen, 2003), and they do not typically engage in any overt olfactory behavior, such as sniffing. However, this should not be taken to mean that olfaction is of limited importance to them. To put birds’ sense of smell in perspective, it is useful to provide a comparison with humans. In humans, as in birds, olfaction is commonly considered to be the least acute sense; despite this, it has been estimated that humans with intact olfactory systems can detect (Amoore, 1977) and discriminate (Bushdid, Magnasco, Vosshall, & Keller, 2014) virtually
鸟有嗅觉。尽管早期的假设认为鸟类很少或没有嗅觉(Audubon, 1826;希尔,1905;Stager, 1964),现在有大量证据表明它们具有功能性嗅觉所需的所有解剖学和神经生物学成分(Balthazart & Taziaux, 2009;Caro, Balthazart, & Bonadonna, 2015),从形态学上看,鸟类的嗅觉系统与两栖动物、爬行动物和哺乳动物的嗅觉系统非常相似(Kare & Mason, 1986;文策尔,1987)。大多数鸟类有成对的外鼻孔,通过它们呼吸空气,内鼻腔含有嗅觉上皮,功能性嗅觉受体,以及与前脑嗅球连接的神经元(Jones & Roper, 1997)。此外,研究还表明,它们会在觅食等多种任务中使用气味线索(例如,Healy & Guilford, 1990;Nevitt, Loosekoot, & Weimerskirch, 2008;Potier, Duriez, Celerier, Liegeois, & Bonadonna, 2019),识别蛋(Leclaire, Bourret, & Bonadonna, 2017),选择筑巢材料(例如Gwinner, 2013),避免捕食(例如Amo, Galvan, Tomás, & Sanz, 2008),以及在社会背景下,如物种(例如Krause等人,2014),亲属(例如Bonadonna和Sanz- aguilar, 2012;科芬,沃特斯,&马特奥,2011;Krause et al., 2012)和配偶识别(例如,Bonadonna & Nevitt, 2004)。然而,我们认为,即使在今天,研究人员也倾向于低估嗅觉在鸟类日常生活中可能发挥的作用。令人信服的证据表明,视觉和听觉(在较小程度上)是鸟类的主要感官(Martin, 2017)。相比之下,很少有鸟类的气味对人类来说是明显的(尽管也有明显的例外,比如冠毛小海雀;Hagelin, Jones, & Rasmussen, 2003),他们通常不参与任何明显的嗅觉行为,比如嗅。然而,这并不意味着嗅觉对它们的重要性有限。为了正确地看待鸟类的嗅觉,将其与人类进行比较是有用的。人类和鸟类一样,嗅觉通常被认为是最不敏锐的感觉;尽管如此,据估计,具有完整嗅觉系统的人类几乎可以检测(Amoore, 1977)和区分(Bushdid, Magnasco, Vosshall, & Keller, 2014)
{"title":"Avian olfaction: a review of the recent literature","authors":"V. Abankwah, D. Deeming, T. W. Pike","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150005","url":null,"abstract":"Birds can smell. Despite early assumptions that birds had little or no sense of smell (Audubon, 1826; Hill, 1905; Stager, 1964), there is now abundant evidence that they are endowed with all the anatomical and neurobiological components necessary for a functional olfactory sense (Balthazart & Taziaux, 2009; Caro, Balthazart, & Bonadonna, 2015), and morphologically the olfactory systems of birds closely resembles those of amphibians, reptiles, and mammals (Kare & Mason, 1986; Wenzel, 1987). Most birds have paired external nares through which they breathe air, internal nasal cavities that contain olfactory epithelium, functional olfactory receptors, and a neuronal connection to the olfactory bulb in the forebrain (Jones & Roper, 1997). Moreover, they have been shown to use odor cues for tasks as diverse as foraging (e.g., Healy & Guilford, 1990; Nevitt, Loosekoot, & Weimerskirch, 2008; Potier, Duriez, Celerier, Liegeois, & Bonadonna, 2019), recognizing eggs (Leclaire, Bourret, & Bonadonna, 2017), selecting nest material (e.g., Gwinner, 2013), and avoiding predation (e.g., Amo, Galvan, Tomás, & Sanz, 2008), as well as in social contexts such as species (e.g., Krause et al., 2014), kin (e.g., Bonadonna & Sanz-Aguilar, 2012; Coffin, Watters, & Mateo, 2011; Krause et al., 2012), and mate recognition (e.g., Bonadonna & Nevitt, 2004). However, we argue that even today there is a tendency for researchers to underappreciate the possible role olfaction plays in birds’ everyday lives. Compelling evidence shows that vision and, to a lesser extent, hearing are the primary avian senses (Martin, 2017). By contrast, few bird species smell noticeably to humans (although there are notable exceptions, such as Crested auklets [Aethia cristatella]; Hagelin, Jones, & Rasmussen, 2003), and they do not typically engage in any overt olfactory behavior, such as sniffing. However, this should not be taken to mean that olfaction is of limited importance to them. To put birds’ sense of smell in perspective, it is useful to provide a comparison with humans. In humans, as in birds, olfaction is commonly considered to be the least acute sense; despite this, it has been estimated that humans with intact olfactory systems can detect (Amoore, 1977) and discriminate (Bushdid, Magnasco, Vosshall, & Keller, 2014) virtually","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235848","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is the susceptibility to visual illusions related to the relative brain size? Insights from small-brained species 对视觉错觉的易感性与大脑的相对大小有关吗?来自小脑袋物种的见解
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003
Alessandra Pecunioso, Maria Santacà, M. E. M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo
The study of visual illusions represents a useful tool in different research fields. Visual illusions are commonly used to establish the perceptual mechanisms underlying our perception of static and dynamic events (for a review, see Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1997). Visual illusions have become a valid tool in clinical psychology as a noninvasive screening for detecting schizophrenic and other psychopathological traits (e.g., Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016; Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014; Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008). Last, they represent a powerful set of stimuli to compare the vision of human and non human animals. With respect to this issue, the past two decades have been characterized by a growing number of studies finding that apes, old-world monkeys, and new-world monkeys are susceptible to many visual illusions. Nonhuman primates perceive size illusions (e.g., chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Parrish & Beran, 2014; capuchin monkeys, Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015; Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007), depth illusions (baboons, Papio papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002), orientation illusions (rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta: Agrillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b), numerosity illusions (rhesus monkeys: Beran, 2006), and motion illusions (rhesus monkeys: Agrillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015).
视错觉的研究在不同的研究领域是一个有用的工具。视觉错觉通常被用来建立我们对静态和动态事件感知的感知机制(回顾,见Eagleman, 2001;格里高利,1997)。视错觉已经成为临床心理学中一种有效的工具,作为检测精神分裂症和其他精神病理特征的非侵入性筛查(例如,Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016;Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014;Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008)。最后,它们代表了一组强大的刺激来比较人类和非人类动物的视觉。关于这个问题,过去二十年的特点是越来越多的研究发现猿、旧世界的猴子和新世界的猴子都容易产生许多视觉错觉。非人类灵长类动物感知大小错觉(例如黑猩猩,泛类人猿:Parrish & Beran, 2014;卷尾猴,Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015;Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007),深度错觉(狒狒,Papio Papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002),方向错觉(恒河猴,Macaca mulatta: agillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b),数量错觉(恒河猴:Beran, 2006)和运动错觉(恒河猴:agillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015)。
{"title":"Is the susceptibility to visual illusions related to the relative brain size? Insights from small-brained species","authors":"Alessandra Pecunioso, Maria Santacà, M. E. M. Petrazzini, C. Agrillo","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150003","url":null,"abstract":"The study of visual illusions represents a useful tool in different research fields. Visual illusions are commonly used to establish the perceptual mechanisms underlying our perception of static and dynamic events (for a review, see Eagleman, 2001; Gregory, 1997). Visual illusions have become a valid tool in clinical psychology as a noninvasive screening for detecting schizophrenic and other psychopathological traits (e.g., Gori, Molteni, & Facoetti, 2016; Notredame, Pins, Deneve, & Jardri, 2014; Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Simon, Suganuma, & Tavares, 2008). Last, they represent a powerful set of stimuli to compare the vision of human and non human animals. With respect to this issue, the past two decades have been characterized by a growing number of studies finding that apes, old-world monkeys, and new-world monkeys are susceptible to many visual illusions. Nonhuman primates perceive size illusions (e.g., chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes: Parrish & Beran, 2014; capuchin monkeys, Sapajus apella: Parrish, Brosnan, & Beran, 2015; Suganuma, Pessoa, Monge-Fuentes, Castro, & Tavares, 2007), depth illusions (baboons, Papio papio: Barbet & Fagot, 2002), orientation illusions (rhesus monkeys, Macaca mulatta: Agrillo, Parrish, & Beran, 2014b), numerosity illusions (rhesus monkeys: Beran, 2006), and motion illusions (rhesus monkeys: Agrillo, Gori, & Beran, 2015).","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235346","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Teaching animal learning and cognition: adapting to the online environment 动物学习与认知教学:适应网络环境
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150008
V. Kuhlmeier, Tara A. Karasewich, M. Olmstead
The number of online courses offered by institutions of higher education has been increasing sizably in the 21st century. As we write this article in 2020, though, the prevalence of online courses is taking an unexpected upturn as the global COVID-19 pandemic has led to a sudden transition of many in-person courses to remote, online delivery. The specific goal of this article is to share, in a timely manner, our experiences and insights from teaching an online course on animal learning and cognition for the last 7 years. A broader goal is to provide a resource that not only benefits instructors in the present circumstances but also supports course development, review, and redesign-for both on-campus and online curricula-into the future. To these ends, we discuss course organization, learning outcomes, activities, assessments, and considerations such as accessibility and academic integrity. We end with a "call for community" of instructors who share teaching resources, and we hope that this article, and its associated supplemental materials, may serve to support this endeavor. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)
21世纪,高等教育机构提供的在线课程数量一直在大幅增加。然而,就在我们在2020年写这篇文章的时候,随着全球COVID-19大流行导致许多面对面课程突然转变为远程在线授课,在线课程的普及正在出现意想不到的好转。这篇文章的具体目的是及时分享我们在过去7年里教授动物学习和认知在线课程的经验和见解。一个更广泛的目标是提供一种资源,不仅使教师在目前的情况下受益,而且还支持课程开发、审查和重新设计——无论是校园课程还是在线课程——面向未来。为此,我们讨论了课程组织、学习成果、活动、评估以及诸如可访问性和学术诚信等考虑因素。最后,我们以“呼吁社区”的形式号召教师共享教学资源,我们希望本文及其相关的补充材料可以为这一努力提供支持。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2021 APA,版权所有)
{"title":"Teaching animal learning and cognition: adapting to the online environment","authors":"V. Kuhlmeier, Tara A. Karasewich, M. Olmstead","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150008","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150008","url":null,"abstract":"The number of online courses offered by institutions of higher education has been increasing sizably in the 21st century. As we write this article in 2020, though, the prevalence of online courses is taking an unexpected upturn as the global COVID-19 pandemic has led to a sudden transition of many in-person courses to remote, online delivery. The specific goal of this article is to share, in a timely manner, our experiences and insights from teaching an online course on animal learning and cognition for the last 7 years. A broader goal is to provide a resource that not only benefits instructors in the present circumstances but also supports course development, review, and redesign-for both on-campus and online curricula-into the future. To these ends, we discuss course organization, learning outcomes, activities, assessments, and considerations such as accessibility and academic integrity. We end with a \"call for community\" of instructors who share teaching resources, and we hope that this article, and its associated supplemental materials, may serve to support this endeavor. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved)","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The importance of sensory perception in an elephant's cognitive world 感官知觉在大象认知世界中的重要性
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/ccbr.2020.150006
S. L. Jacobson, Joshua M. Plotnik
The three extant species of the family Elephantidae — Asian (Elephas maximus), African savanna (Loxodonta africana), and African forest (L. cyclotis) elephants — have long trajectories of independent evolution. The African species diverged from the Asian species more than 6 million years ago (Rogaev et al., 2006), and the two African species diverged from each other more than 1.9 million years ago (Rohland et al., 2010). Although these species inhabit somewhat different ecosystems, they are all generalist feeders and consume a wide variety of grasses and browse depending on resource availability (Hatt & Clauss, 2006). All species are social, with the main social unit centering on adult female relatives and their offspring (Buss & Smith, 1966; McKay, 1973). The oldest female leads the group consistently in African savanna elephants (Douglas-Hamilton & Douglas-Hamilton,
象科现存的三个物种——亚洲象(大象)、非洲草原象(非洲象)和非洲森林象(L. cyclotis)——都有很长的独立进化轨迹。非洲物种在600多万年前从亚洲物种中分化出来(Rogaev et al., 2006),两个非洲物种在190多万年前相互分化(Rohland et al., 2010)。虽然这些物种居住在不同的生态系统中,但它们都是多面手喂食者,消耗各种各样的草,并根据资源的可用性进行浏览(Hatt & Clauss, 2006)。所有物种都是社会性的,主要的社会单位以成年雌性亲属及其后代为中心(Buss & Smith, 1966;麦凯,1973)。在非洲大草原象群中,最年长的雌性一直领导着这个群体(道格拉斯-汉密尔顿和道格拉斯-汉密尔顿,
{"title":"The importance of sensory perception in an elephant's cognitive world","authors":"S. L. Jacobson, Joshua M. Plotnik","doi":"10.3819/ccbr.2020.150006","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/ccbr.2020.150006","url":null,"abstract":"The three extant species of the family Elephantidae — Asian (Elephas maximus), African savanna (Loxodonta africana), and African forest (L. cyclotis) elephants — have long trajectories of independent evolution. The African species diverged from the Asian species more than 6 million years ago (Rogaev et al., 2006), and the two African species diverged from each other more than 1.9 million years ago (Rohland et al., 2010). Although these species inhabit somewhat different ecosystems, they are all generalist feeders and consume a wide variety of grasses and browse depending on resource availability (Hatt & Clauss, 2006). All species are social, with the main social unit centering on adult female relatives and their offspring (Buss & Smith, 1966; McKay, 1973). The oldest female leads the group consistently in African savanna elephants (Douglas-Hamilton & Douglas-Hamilton,","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235930","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Crocodilians are promising intermediate model organisms for comparative perception research 鳄鱼是比较知觉研究中很有前途的中间模式生物
Q2 Agricultural and Biological Sciences Pub Date : 2020-01-01 DOI: 10.3819/CCBR.2020.150004E
S. Reber
Crocodilians are the closest living relatives of birds and share many ecological challenges with mammalian apex predators. They evolved perception pathways that share similarities with both taxa, birds and mammals. Due to their position in the tree of life, crocodilians therefore represent a promising intermediate model for comparative research. In this review, the different modalities of perception in crocodilians are discussed: vision, audition, olfaction, gustation, sense of touch, and (the potential for) magnetoreception. The anatomy and physiology of the sensory organs are briefly described, and behavioral studies on perception summarized. Throughout the review, the similarities and differences between crocodilians and other vertebrate taxa are addressed. Overall, crocodilian sensory organs seem to have evolved for a terrestrial environment, as their eyes are adapted for vision in air, their hearing resembles that of birds, and they do not seem to use olfaction under water. A clear exception are the integumentary sensory organs, which allow them to perceive minute water movements. While crocodilian sensory organs have received quite some attention, there are relatively few behavioral studies on perception. Future research on the perceptual capacities of crocodilians will provide insight into the evolutionary origins of perception in all amniotes. (Less)
鳄鱼是鸟类的近亲,与哺乳动物共同面临许多生态挑战。它们进化出的感知途径与类群、鸟类和哺乳动物都有相似之处。由于它们在生命之树上的位置,鳄鱼因此代表了一个有希望的比较研究的中间模型。在这篇综述中,我们讨论了鳄鱼的不同感知方式:视觉、听觉、嗅觉、味觉、触觉和(潜在的)磁感受。简要介绍了感觉器官的解剖学和生理学,并对知觉的行为学研究进行了综述。在整个综述中,鳄鱼和其他脊椎动物分类群之间的异同进行了讨论。总的来说,鳄鱼的感觉器官似乎是为了适应陆地环境而进化的,因为它们的眼睛适应了在空气中的视觉,它们的听觉与鸟类相似,而且它们似乎不使用水下嗅觉。一个明显的例外是外皮感觉器官,它使它们能够感知微小的水运动。虽然鳄鱼的感觉器官受到了相当多的关注,但对感知的行为研究相对较少。未来对鳄鱼感知能力的研究将为所有羊膜动物感知的进化起源提供见解。(少)
{"title":"Crocodilians are promising intermediate model organisms for comparative perception research","authors":"S. Reber","doi":"10.3819/CCBR.2020.150004E","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3819/CCBR.2020.150004E","url":null,"abstract":"Crocodilians are the closest living relatives of birds and share many ecological challenges with mammalian apex predators. They evolved perception pathways that share similarities with both taxa, birds and mammals. Due to their position in the tree of life, crocodilians therefore represent a promising intermediate model for comparative research. In this review, the different modalities of perception in crocodilians are discussed: vision, audition, olfaction, gustation, sense of touch, and (the potential for) magnetoreception. The anatomy and physiology of the sensory organs are briefly described, and behavioral studies on perception summarized. Throughout the review, the similarities and differences between crocodilians and other vertebrate taxa are addressed. Overall, crocodilian sensory organs seem to have evolved for a terrestrial environment, as their eyes are adapted for vision in air, their hearing resembles that of birds, and they do not seem to use olfaction under water. A clear exception are the integumentary sensory organs, which allow them to perceive minute water movements. While crocodilian sensory organs have received quite some attention, there are relatively few behavioral studies on perception. Future research on the perceptual capacities of crocodilians will provide insight into the evolutionary origins of perception in all amniotes. (Less)","PeriodicalId":44593,"journal":{"name":"Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"70235416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":0,"RegionCategory":"","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
期刊
Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1