首页 > 最新文献

Laboratory Phonology最新文献

英文 中文
Gestural reduction, lexical frequency, and sound change: A study of post-vocalic /l/ 手势减少、词汇频率和声音变化:后元音/l/的研究
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2014-02-01 DOI: 10.1515/lp-2014-0002
Susan Lin, P. S. Beddor, A. Coetzee
Abstract The magnitude of anterior and dorsal constrictions for laterals in /(C)(C)VlC/ words produced by eight American English speakers was measured using ultrasound imaging. The results replicate previous findings that laterals have weaker anterior constrictions when followed by labial or velar consonants than when followed by alveolar consonants. The main novel finding is that, in words with /VlClabial/ or /VlCvelar/ sequences, this anterior constriction was weaker in high-frequency words (help , milk ) than in low-frequency words ( whelp , ilk ). Although high-frequency words also showed slight reduction of the dorsal constriction, dorsal reduction was stable, small in magnitude, and not correlated with anterior reduction, consistent with alveolar reduction not being simply a consequence of overall weaker lingual constrictions in more frequent words. Acoustic measures for laterals showed that the degree of anterior constriction correlated with the frequency separation between F1 and F2: more reduced alveolar constrictions – especially likely in high-frequency words – were linked with greater formant proximity. These articulatory and acoustic patterns are interpreted as potentially contributing to the initiation and lexical diffusion of historical /l/ lenition. It is proposed that gestural reduction in high-frequency words in which the anterior gesture for laterals must be coordinated with another supralaryngeal constriction serves as a precipitating factor in /l/ vocalization and possibly (although to a lesser extent) /l/ loss.
摘要采用超声成像技术测量了8例美国英语使用者/(C)(C)VlC/ words的前、背侧侧缩程度。结果重复以前的发现,侧边有较弱的前收缩,当跟随唇或腭辅音比当跟随肺泡辅音。主要的新发现是,在含有/VlClabial/或/ vlvelar /序列的单词中,高频单词(help, milk)的前缩比低频单词(whelp, milk)弱。尽管高频词也表现出轻微的背缩减少,但背缩是稳定的,幅度小,与前缩不相关,这与牙槽缩小不只是更频繁词整体较弱舌缩的结果是一致的。侧耳的声学测量表明,前部收缩的程度与F1和F2之间的频率分离有关:肺泡收缩的程度越小——尤其是在高频单词中——与更大的形成峰接近有关。这些发音和声学模式被解释为可能有助于历史/l/发音的开始和词汇扩散。我们提出,高频词的手势减少,其中前侧手势必须与另一个咽上收缩协调,是/l/发声和可能(尽管程度较小)/l/丧失的促成因素。
{"title":"Gestural reduction, lexical frequency, and sound change: A study of post-vocalic /l/","authors":"Susan Lin, P. S. Beddor, A. Coetzee","doi":"10.1515/lp-2014-0002","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0002","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The magnitude of anterior and dorsal constrictions for laterals in /(C)(C)VlC/ words produced by eight American English speakers was measured using ultrasound imaging. The results replicate previous findings that laterals have weaker anterior constrictions when followed by labial or velar consonants than when followed by alveolar consonants. The main novel finding is that, in words with /VlClabial/ or /VlCvelar/ sequences, this anterior constriction was weaker in high-frequency words (help , milk ) than in low-frequency words ( whelp , ilk ). Although high-frequency words also showed slight reduction of the dorsal constriction, dorsal reduction was stable, small in magnitude, and not correlated with anterior reduction, consistent with alveolar reduction not being simply a consequence of overall weaker lingual constrictions in more frequent words. Acoustic measures for laterals showed that the degree of anterior constriction correlated with the frequency separation between F1 and F2: more reduced alveolar constrictions – especially likely in high-frequency words – were linked with greater formant proximity. These articulatory and acoustic patterns are interpreted as potentially contributing to the initiation and lexical diffusion of historical /l/ lenition. It is proposed that gestural reduction in high-frequency words in which the anterior gesture for laterals must be coordinated with another supralaryngeal constriction serves as a precipitating factor in /l/ vocalization and possibly (although to a lesser extent) /l/ loss.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2014-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/lp-2014-0002","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024863","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 43
Cognitive processing as a bridge between phonetic and social models of sound change 认知加工是语音变化和社会模式之间的桥梁
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2014-02-01 DOI: 10.1515/lp-2014-0001
J. Harrington, M. Stevens
The seven papers in this special edition are derived from the 2nd Workshop on Sound Change held at Kloster Seeon, Germany, in May 2012. The purpose of the workshop was to bring together scientists approaching the question of sound change and its relationship to synchronic variation in speech from many different disciplinary perspectives that we believe are necessary for understanding this complex relationship. The publications in this special issue are a reflection of this breadth and cover a wide range of issues, such as the influence on sound change of child speech, dialect contact, social differences, coarticulatory variation, and imitation. The studies draw upon several languages (Mandarin Chinese, English, German, Khmer, Korean, Spanish) and employ diverse experimental techniques for relating synchronic variation and diachronic change, including ultrasound measurements of the tongue (Lin et al.), acoustic and perceptual analyses of multilingual corpora (Beckman et al.), measurements of oral and nasal airflow in combination with the perceptual analysis of aerodynamic variation (Sole), and computational modelling (Kirby). It has been convenient in the literature so far to draw a distinction between the conditions that give rise to sound change as opposed to those that are concerned with its spread through the community (e.g., Ohala 1993). A classic issue within the first of these is phonologization (Hyman 1976), which can often be related synchronically to a change in the way that the multiple features which cue a phonological distinction are parsed in the speech signal. Four papers in this special issue address this issue. In Kirby’s study, phonologization arises when laryngeal features (primarily fundamental frequency) and/or voice onset time take over from a trill in distinguishing pairs such as /kru:, ku:/ in the colloquial Phnom Penh variety of Khmer. The phonetic basis of this change is likely to be a drop in fundamental frequency
本特刊中的七篇论文来自于2012年5月在德国Kloster Seeon举行的第二届声音变化研讨会。研讨会的目的是将科学家聚集在一起,从许多不同的学科角度探讨声音变化及其与言语共时变化的关系,我们认为这对于理解这种复杂的关系是必要的。本期特刊的出版物反映了这一广度,涵盖了儿童言语对声音变化的影响、方言接触、社会差异、协同发音变化、模仿等广泛的问题。这些研究利用了几种语言(汉语普通话、英语、德语、高棉语、韩语、西班牙语),并采用了不同的实验技术来研究共时变化和历时变化,包括舌头的超声波测量(Lin等人)、多语言语料库的声学和感知分析(Beckman等人)、结合空气动力学变化的感知分析的口腔和鼻腔气流测量(Sole)和计算建模(Kirby)。到目前为止,在文献中很容易区分产生健全变化的条件与那些与其在社区中传播有关的条件(例如,Ohala 1993)。第一个问题中的一个经典问题是音位化(Hyman 1976),它通常与语音信号中提示音位区分的多种特征解析方式的变化同步相关。本期特刊的四篇论文讨论了这个问题。在Kirby的研究中,当喉音特征(主要是基本频率)和/或声音开始时间取代颤音时,就会出现语音化现象,例如高棉语金边口语化中的/kru:, ku:/。这种变化的语音基础很可能是基本频率的下降
{"title":"Cognitive processing as a bridge between phonetic and social models of sound change","authors":"J. Harrington, M. Stevens","doi":"10.1515/lp-2014-0001","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0001","url":null,"abstract":"The seven papers in this special edition are derived from the 2nd Workshop on Sound Change held at Kloster Seeon, Germany, in May 2012. The purpose of the workshop was to bring together scientists approaching the question of sound change and its relationship to synchronic variation in speech from many different disciplinary perspectives that we believe are necessary for understanding this complex relationship. The publications in this special issue are a reflection of this breadth and cover a wide range of issues, such as the influence on sound change of child speech, dialect contact, social differences, coarticulatory variation, and imitation. The studies draw upon several languages (Mandarin Chinese, English, German, Khmer, Korean, Spanish) and employ diverse experimental techniques for relating synchronic variation and diachronic change, including ultrasound measurements of the tongue (Lin et al.), acoustic and perceptual analyses of multilingual corpora (Beckman et al.), measurements of oral and nasal airflow in combination with the perceptual analysis of aerodynamic variation (Sole), and computational modelling (Kirby). It has been convenient in the literature so far to draw a distinction between the conditions that give rise to sound change as opposed to those that are concerned with its spread through the community (e.g., Ohala 1993). A classic issue within the first of these is phonologization (Hyman 1976), which can often be related synchronically to a change in the way that the multiple features which cue a phonological distinction are parsed in the speech signal. Four papers in this special issue address this issue. In Kirby’s study, phonologization arises when laryngeal features (primarily fundamental frequency) and/or voice onset time take over from a trill in distinguishing pairs such as /kru:, ku:/ in the colloquial Phnom Penh variety of Khmer. The phonetic basis of this change is likely to be a drop in fundamental frequency","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2014-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/lp-2014-0001","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Pre-aspiration, quantity, and sound change 预吸,数量和声音变化
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/lp-2014-0015
M. Stevens, Ulrich Reubold
Abstract Geminate voiceless stops /pː tː kː / have been recently found to show optional pre-aspiration under certain circumstances in spontaneous and read Italian speech. This paper investigates the impact of pre-aspiration on the perception and production of contrastive quantity, e.g., fato ‘fate’ vs. fatto ‘done’. It tests the hypothesis that synchronic variability involving pre-aspiration, together with concomitant stop closure shortening, may be setting in motion a sound change in Italian ultimately leading to de-gemination, i.e., /pː tː kː / > [hp ht hk] > /p t k/. The proposed sound change would be perceptually driven (Ohala 1981, 1993) and comes about via listener association of pre-aspiration with the preceding vowel rather than the oral closure. The hypothesis is only partially supported by the experimental results. Perception data show that Italians perceive pre-aspirated stops as shorter than plain stops of analogous overall duration. However, production data show that pre-aspiration in Italian does not in fact involve concomitant oral closure shortening.
摘要:最近发现,在自发和阅读意大利语的某些情况下,双元音的无音顿音/p [t] k [k] /表现出选择性的前送音。本文调查了预期望对感知和生产对比数量的影响,例如,“命运”与“完成”。它验证了一种假设,即涉及前误吸的同步性变化,以及伴随的停止闭合缩短,可能会导致意大利语的声音变化,最终导致去发音,即/p / t / k/ > [hp ht hk] > /p t k/。所提出的声音变化是由感知驱动的(Ohala 1981,1993),通过听者将前吸音与前一个元音而不是口闭元音联系起来而产生。实验结果只是部分地支持了这个假设。感知数据显示,意大利人认为在类似的总持续时间下,预吸气停顿比普通停顿要短。然而,生产数据显示,意大利语的预吸实际上并不包括伴随的口腔闭合缩短。
{"title":"Pre-aspiration, quantity, and sound change","authors":"M. Stevens, Ulrich Reubold","doi":"10.1515/lp-2014-0015","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0015","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Geminate voiceless stops /pː tː kː / have been recently found to show optional pre-aspiration under certain circumstances in spontaneous and read Italian speech. This paper investigates the impact of pre-aspiration on the perception and production of contrastive quantity, e.g., fato ‘fate’ vs. fatto ‘done’. It tests the hypothesis that synchronic variability involving pre-aspiration, together with concomitant stop closure shortening, may be setting in motion a sound change in Italian ultimately leading to de-gemination, i.e., /pː tː kː / > [hp ht hk] > /p t k/. The proposed sound change would be perceptually driven (Ohala 1981, 1993) and comes about via listener association of pre-aspiration with the preceding vowel rather than the oral closure. The hypothesis is only partially supported by the experimental results. Perception data show that Italians perceive pre-aspirated stops as shorter than plain stops of analogous overall duration. However, production data show that pre-aspiration in Italian does not in fact involve concomitant oral closure shortening.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/lp-2014-0015","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024929","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 13
Sound change in an urban setting: Category instability of the palatal fricative in Berlin 城市环境中的声音变化:柏林腭摩擦音的类别不稳定性
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/lp-2014-0005
Stefanie Jannedy, Melanie Weirich
Abstract The differential categorization of identical stimuli depending on the presence of a prime is described as a perceptual divergence effect. We examined whether native listeners of the Berlin vernacular of German categorized identical acoustic stimuli differently in the explicit context of the names of two different districts of Berlin, assuming that listeners infer social information and linguistic stereotypes based on the names of these neighborhoods (Kreuzberg vs. Zehlendorf). All listeners categorized natural acoustic stimuli with synthetic fricatives synthesized along a continuum ranging from /ç/ to /ʃ/ as either Fichte /fɪçtə/ (`spruce') or fischte /fɪʃtə/ (1st person sg. `to fish'). This variable was chosen because many young multiethnic speakers of Berlin German pronounce /ç/ as [ʃ] or [ɕ], and this alternation is highly associated with speakers with a migrant background from Kreuzberg. Data were gathered in a forced-choice identification task, and, for a subset of the participants, reaction times (RTs) were also gathered. Results indicate a differential categorization pattern depending on (1) the copresented information, i.e., Kreuzberg, Zehlendorf, or none (control), and (2) the age of the listeners, with older listeners being more affected by the co-presented information. While older listeners categorized significantly more /ʃ/ sounds in the context of Kreuzberg than in the control or Zehlendorf condition, younger listeners rated most /ʃ/ sounds in the control condition (no added information). Results are interpreted in terms of a potential sound change in progress: the loss of the phoneme contrast between /ç/ and /ʃ/.
摘要根据启动物的存在,对相同刺激的差异分类被描述为知觉分歧效应。我们研究了德国柏林方言的本土听众是否在柏林两个不同地区的名称的明确背景下对相同的声音刺激进行了不同的分类,假设听众根据这些社区的名称推断社会信息和语言刻板印象(Kreuzberg vs. Zehlendorf)。所有的听者都将天然的声音刺激与合成摩擦音沿着/ç/到/ h /的连续体进行分类,要么是Fichte /f / t / (' spruce'),要么是fischte /f / t /(第一人称sg。“鱼”)。之所以选择这个变量,是因为许多年轻的多民族柏林德语使用者将/ç/发音为[j]或[j],这种变化与来自克罗伊茨贝格的移民背景高度相关。在一个强制选择识别任务中收集数据,并且,对于一部分参与者,也收集了反应时间(RTs)。结果表明,不同的分类模式取决于(1)所呈现的信息,即Kreuzberg, Zehlendorf,或无(对照);(2)听众的年龄,年龄较大的听众更容易受到共同呈现信息的影响。年龄较大的听者在Kreuzberg的语境中比对照组或Zehlendorf的语境中对/ h /音的分类明显更多,而年轻的听者在对照组(没有添加信息)中对/ h /音的分类最多。结果被解释为在过程中潜在的声音变化:/ç/和/ h /之间音素对比的丧失。
{"title":"Sound change in an urban setting: Category instability of the palatal fricative in Berlin","authors":"Stefanie Jannedy, Melanie Weirich","doi":"10.1515/lp-2014-0005","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0005","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The differential categorization of identical stimuli depending on the presence of a prime is described as a perceptual divergence effect. We examined whether native listeners of the Berlin vernacular of German categorized identical acoustic stimuli differently in the explicit context of the names of two different districts of Berlin, assuming that listeners infer social information and linguistic stereotypes based on the names of these neighborhoods (Kreuzberg vs. Zehlendorf). All listeners categorized natural acoustic stimuli with synthetic fricatives synthesized along a continuum ranging from /ç/ to /ʃ/ as either Fichte /fɪçtə/ (`spruce') or fischte /fɪʃtə/ (1st person sg. `to fish'). This variable was chosen because many young multiethnic speakers of Berlin German pronounce /ç/ as [ʃ] or [ɕ], and this alternation is highly associated with speakers with a migrant background from Kreuzberg. Data were gathered in a forced-choice identification task, and, for a subset of the participants, reaction times (RTs) were also gathered. Results indicate a differential categorization pattern depending on (1) the copresented information, i.e., Kreuzberg, Zehlendorf, or none (control), and (2) the age of the listeners, with older listeners being more affected by the co-presented information. While older listeners categorized significantly more /ʃ/ sounds in the context of Kreuzberg than in the control or Zehlendorf condition, younger listeners rated most /ʃ/ sounds in the control condition (no added information). Results are interpreted in terms of a potential sound change in progress: the loss of the phoneme contrast between /ç/ and /ʃ/.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/lp-2014-0005","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024869","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 39
Timing of German onset and word boundary clusters 德语开始的时间和词边界集群
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2014-01-01 DOI: 10.1515/lp-2014-0014
Jana Brunner, C. Geng, S. Sotiropoulou, A. Gafos
Abstract Previous studies suggest that there are special timing relations in syllable onsets. The consonants are assumed to be timed, on the one hand, with the vocalic nucleus and, on the other hand, with each other. These competing timing relations result in the C-center effect. However, the C-center effect has not consistently been found in languages with complex onsets. Moreover, it has occasionally been found in languages disallowing complex onsets. The present study investigates onset timing in German while discussing alternative explanations (not related to bonding) for the timing patterns observed. Six German speakers were recorded via Electromagnetic Articulography. The corpus contained items with four clusters (/sk/, /kv/, /gl/, and /pl/). The clusters occur in word-initial position, word-medial position, and across a word boundary preceding different vowels. The results suggest that segmental properties (i.e., oral-laryngeal coordination, coarticulatory resistance) determine the observed timing patterns, and specifically the absence or presence of the C-center effect.
摘要以往的研究表明,音节起首存在着特殊的时序关系。辅音被认为是定时的,一方面,与发声核,另一方面,彼此。这些相互竞争的时间关系导致了c中心效应。然而,c中心效应并不总是出现在复杂的语言中。此外,偶尔也会在不允许复杂启动的语言中发现它。本研究调查了德语的发作时间,同时讨论了观察到的时间模式的其他解释(与键合无关)。通过电磁发音记录仪记录了六名讲德语的人。语料库包含四个聚类(/sk/、/kv/、/gl/和/pl/)的项目。这些簇出现在词的起始位置、词的中间位置以及不同元音前面的单词边界。结果表明,节段性(即口喉协调性、协同发声阻力)决定了观察到的时间模式,特别是c中心效应的存在与否。
{"title":"Timing of German onset and word boundary clusters","authors":"Jana Brunner, C. Geng, S. Sotiropoulou, A. Gafos","doi":"10.1515/lp-2014-0014","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0014","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Previous studies suggest that there are special timing relations in syllable onsets. The consonants are assumed to be timed, on the one hand, with the vocalic nucleus and, on the other hand, with each other. These competing timing relations result in the C-center effect. However, the C-center effect has not consistently been found in languages with complex onsets. Moreover, it has occasionally been found in languages disallowing complex onsets. The present study investigates onset timing in German while discussing alternative explanations (not related to bonding) for the timing patterns observed. Six German speakers were recorded via Electromagnetic Articulography. The corpus contained items with four clusters (/sk/, /kv/, /gl/, and /pl/). The clusters occur in word-initial position, word-medial position, and across a word boundary preceding different vowels. The results suggest that segmental properties (i.e., oral-laryngeal coordination, coarticulatory resistance) determine the observed timing patterns, and specifically the absence or presence of the C-center effect.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2014-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/lp-2014-0014","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024928","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 24
Optionality and locality: Evidence from Navajo sibilant harmony 可选性和局部性:来自纳瓦霍人和谐的证据
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2013-10-25 DOI: 10.1515/LP-2013-0011
Kelly Berkson
While many phonological processes are local, consonant harmony is of interest phonologically because it can occur non-locally. Sibilant harmony in Navajo requires that sibilants within a word have matching anteriority specifications. The process is described as being sometimes mandatory and sometimes optional, but neither the statistical nature of the occurrence in optional settings nor the factors contributing to the optionality are fully understood. This paper provides preliminary investigation into these issues using the first person possessive morpheme, which is underlyingly /ʃi-/ but may harmonize to [si-]. Experiment 1, an online grammaticality judgment survey, reveals that the harmonized prefix is dispreferred in all environments. Experiment 2 presents acoustic data from three Navajo speakers: though none harmonize overtly, the spectral mean and lower bound of frication energy of the prefixal fricative are affected by the presence of [+anterior] sibilants in noun stems. The overall implication of these findings is that harmony is not only optional but is dispreferred or wholly absent for some speakers. While multiple factors are investigated, the only one that consistently affects harmony is adjacency of the trigger and target, indicating that, although consonant harmony may indeed be a non-local process, its occurrence is heavily mediated by distance. Kelly Harper Berkson: Indiana University. E-mail: keberkson@gmail.com
虽然许多语音过程是局部的,但辅音和谐在音系学上是有趣的,因为它可以发生在非局部。在纳瓦霍语中,音节的和谐要求一个单词中的音节具有匹配的优先规范。这个过程有时是强制性的,有时是可选的,但无论是可选设置中发生的统计性质,还是促成可选性的因素,都没有得到充分的理解。本文用第一人称所有语素对这些问题进行了初步的探讨,第一人称所有语素基本是/ /,但可能与[si-]谐音。实验1是一项在线语法性判断调查,结果显示在所有环境中,调和前缀都不受欢迎。实验2给出了三个纳瓦霍人的声学数据:虽然没有明显的协调,但前缀摩擦音的频谱平均值和摩擦能量的下限受到名词词干中[+前]音节的影响。这些发现的总体含义是,和谐不仅是可选的,而且对一些说话者来说是不喜欢或完全没有的。虽然研究了多种因素,但始终影响和谐的唯一因素是触发因素和目标的邻接性,这表明,尽管辅音和谐确实可能是一个非局部过程,但其发生在很大程度上受距离的调节。Kelly Harper Berkson:印第安纳大学。电子邮件:keberkson@gmail.com
{"title":"Optionality and locality: Evidence from Navajo sibilant harmony","authors":"Kelly Berkson","doi":"10.1515/LP-2013-0011","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/LP-2013-0011","url":null,"abstract":"While many phonological processes are local, consonant harmony is of interest phonologically because it can occur non-locally. Sibilant harmony in Navajo requires that sibilants within a word have matching anteriority specifications. The process is described as being sometimes mandatory and sometimes optional, but neither the statistical nature of the occurrence in optional settings nor the factors contributing to the optionality are fully understood. This paper provides preliminary investigation into these issues using the first person possessive morpheme, which is underlyingly /ʃi-/ but may harmonize to [si-]. Experiment 1, an online grammaticality judgment survey, reveals that the harmonized prefix is dispreferred in all environments. Experiment 2 presents acoustic data from three Navajo speakers: though none harmonize overtly, the spectral mean and lower bound of frication energy of the prefixal fricative are affected by the presence of [+anterior] sibilants in noun stems. The overall implication of these findings is that harmony is not only optional but is dispreferred or wholly absent for some speakers. While multiple factors are investigated, the only one that consistently affects harmony is adjacency of the trigger and target, indicating that, although consonant harmony may indeed be a non-local process, its occurrence is heavily mediated by distance. Kelly Harper Berkson: Indiana University. E-mail: keberkson@gmail.com","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2013-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/LP-2013-0011","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024791","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Similarity in the generalization of implicitly learned sound patterns 内隐习得声音模式的相似性
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2013-10-25 DOI: 10.1515/LP-2013-0010
Alejandrina Cristia, Jeff Mielke, Robert Daland, S. Peperkamp
It is likely that generalization of implicitly learned sound patterns to novel words and sounds is structured by a similarity metric, but how may this metric best be captured? We report on an experiment where participants were exposed to an artificial phonology, and frequency ratings were used to probe implicit abstraction of onset statistics. Non-words bearing an onset that was pre- sented during initial exposure were subsequently rated most frequent, indicating that participants generalized onset statistics to new non-words. Participants also rated non-words with untrained onsets as somewhat frequent, indicating gener- alization to onsets that had not been used during the exposure phase. While gen- eralization could be accounted for in terms of featural distance, it was insensitive to natural class structure. Generalization to untrained sounds was predicted better by models requiring prior linguistic knowledge (either traditional distinc- tive features or articulatory phonetic information) than by a model based on a linguistically naive measure of acoustic similarity.
很可能内隐学习的声音模式对新单词和声音的概括是由相似性度量来构建的,但是如何才能最好地捕获这个度量呢?我们报告了一项实验,其中参与者暴露于人工音系,并使用频率评级来探测发病统计的隐性抽象。在初次接触时出现的非单词出现频率随后被评为最高,这表明参与者对新的非单词泛化了发病统计。参与者还认为未经训练的非单词出现频率较高,这表明在接触阶段没有使用过的词语出现了泛化。虽然泛化可以用特征距离来解释,但它对自然类结构不敏感。需要先验语言知识(传统的区别特征或发音语音信息)的模型比基于语言上的声学相似性测量的模型更能预测到未经训练的声音的泛化。
{"title":"Similarity in the generalization of implicitly learned sound patterns","authors":"Alejandrina Cristia, Jeff Mielke, Robert Daland, S. Peperkamp","doi":"10.1515/LP-2013-0010","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/LP-2013-0010","url":null,"abstract":"It is likely that generalization of implicitly learned sound patterns to novel words and sounds is structured by a similarity metric, but how may this metric best be captured? We report on an experiment where participants were exposed to an artificial phonology, and frequency ratings were used to probe implicit abstraction of onset statistics. Non-words bearing an onset that was pre- sented during initial exposure were subsequently rated most frequent, indicating that participants generalized onset statistics to new non-words. Participants also rated non-words with untrained onsets as somewhat frequent, indicating gener- alization to onsets that had not been used during the exposure phase. While gen- eralization could be accounted for in terms of featural distance, it was insensitive to natural class structure. Generalization to untrained sounds was predicted better by models requiring prior linguistic knowledge (either traditional distinc- tive features or articulatory phonetic information) than by a model based on a linguistically naive measure of acoustic similarity.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2013-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/LP-2013-0010","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 20
Morphological effects on the darkness of English intervocalic /l/ 形态学对英语间断音/l/暗度的影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2013-10-25 DOI: 10.1515/LP-2013-0015
Sang-Im Lee-Kim, Lisa Davidson, Sangjin Hwang
{"title":"Morphological effects on the darkness of English intervocalic /l/","authors":"Sang-Im Lee-Kim, Lisa Davidson, Sangjin Hwang","doi":"10.1515/LP-2013-0015","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/LP-2013-0015","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2013-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/LP-2013-0015","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024807","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 87
What determines duration-based rhythm measures: text or speaker? 是什么决定了基于持续时间的节奏测量:文本还是说话者?
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2013-10-25 DOI: 10.1515/LP-2013-0012
Anastassia Loukina, B. Rosner, G. Kochanski, E. Keane, Chilin Shih
Differences in rhythm between languages have been often attributed to differences in phonological properties such as syllable structure. This paper uses quantitative analyses to determine whether and how popular duration-based rhythm measures depend on the phonological structure of a language. Native speakers of five languages read a large corpus of comparable texts (approximately 371,000 syllables in total). Phonological properties of each language were speci- fied as 11 variables, computed from the phonetic transcriptions. These variables were compared against published rhythm measures that captured variation in duration of consonantal and vocalic intervals. While the text-based measures dis- criminated well between languages, the values of rhythm measures overlapped substantially, showing that the languages are more alike in acoustic implementa- tion than in their phonological description. Multilevel models demonstrated that the mapping between phonological properties and acoustics is much weaker than previously assumed: linear effects of the phonological variables explained less than a quarter of the total variance in rhythm measures. Instead, speaker was the main source of variation in those measures. Rhythm, in the sense of dura- tional variability, depends far more on individual timing strategies than on the phonological structure of a language.
不同语言之间的节奏差异通常归因于音节结构等音系特性的差异。本文使用定量分析来确定流行的基于持续时间的节奏测量是否以及如何依赖于语言的语音结构。以五种语言为母语的人阅读大量类似文本的语料库(总共约371,000个音节)。每种语言的音韵特性被指定为11个变量,由音标计算得出。将这些变量与已发表的节奏测量进行比较,这些节奏测量捕获了辅音和声乐间隔持续时间的变化。虽然基于文本的度量在不同语言之间有很好的区别,但节奏度量的值有很大的重叠,这表明语言在声学实现上比在语音描述上更相似。多层模型表明,音系属性和声学之间的映射比以前假设的要弱得多:音系变量的线性效应解释了不到四分之一的节奏测量总方差。相反,演讲者是这些措施的主要变化来源。节奏,在持续时间变化的意义上,更多地取决于个人的时间策略,而不是语言的语音结构。
{"title":"What determines duration-based rhythm measures: text or speaker?","authors":"Anastassia Loukina, B. Rosner, G. Kochanski, E. Keane, Chilin Shih","doi":"10.1515/LP-2013-0012","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/LP-2013-0012","url":null,"abstract":"Differences in rhythm between languages have been often attributed to differences in phonological properties such as syllable structure. This paper uses quantitative analyses to determine whether and how popular duration-based rhythm measures depend on the phonological structure of a language. Native speakers of five languages read a large corpus of comparable texts (approximately 371,000 syllables in total). Phonological properties of each language were speci- fied as 11 variables, computed from the phonetic transcriptions. These variables were compared against published rhythm measures that captured variation in duration of consonantal and vocalic intervals. While the text-based measures dis- criminated well between languages, the values of rhythm measures overlapped substantially, showing that the languages are more alike in acoustic implementa- tion than in their phonological description. Multilevel models demonstrated that the mapping between phonological properties and acoustics is much weaker than previously assumed: linear effects of the phonological variables explained less than a quarter of the total variance in rhythm measures. Instead, speaker was the main source of variation in those measures. Rhythm, in the sense of dura- tional variability, depends far more on individual timing strategies than on the phonological structure of a language.","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2013-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/LP-2013-0012","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024793","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
First language suprasegmentally-conditioned syllable length distinctions influence perception and production of second language vowel contrasts 第一语言超分段条件音节长度差异影响第二语言元音对比的感知和产生
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Health Professions Pub Date : 2013-10-25 DOI: 10.1515/LP-2013-0014
L. Krebs-Lazendic, C. Best
{"title":"First language suprasegmentally-conditioned syllable length distinctions influence perception and production of second language vowel contrasts","authors":"L. Krebs-Lazendic, C. Best","doi":"10.1515/LP-2013-0014","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1515/LP-2013-0014","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":45128,"journal":{"name":"Laboratory Phonology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2013-10-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://sci-hub-pdf.com/10.1515/LP-2013-0014","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"67024806","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Laboratory Phonology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1