首页 > 最新文献

Language Learning and Development最新文献

英文 中文
Infants’ Sensitivity to Lexical Tone and Word Stress in Their First Year: A Thai and English Cross-Language Study 一岁婴儿对词汇语调和单词重音的敏感性:泰英跨语言研究
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-08-23 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1954928
M. Kalashnikova, C. Onsuwan, D. Burnham
ABSTRACT Non-tone language infants’ native language recognition is based first on supra-segmental then segmental cues, but this trajectory is unknown for tone-language infants. This study investigated non-tone (English) and tone (Thai) language 6- to 10-month-old infants’ preference for English vs. Thai one-syllable words (containing segmental and tone cues) and two-syllable words (additionally containing stress cues). A preference for their native one-syllable words was observed in each of the two groups of infants, but this was not the case for two-syllable words where Thai-learning infants showed no native-language preference. These findings indicate that as early as six months of age, infants acquiring tone- and non-tone languages identify their native language by relying solely on lexical tone cues, but tone language infants no longer show successful identification of their native language when two pitch-based cues co-occur in the signal.
非声调语言婴儿的母语识别首先是基于超音段线索,然后是音段线索,但这种轨迹对于声调语言婴儿来说是未知的。本研究调查了6 ~ 10个月大婴儿对英语和泰语单音节词(包含音段和声调线索)和双音节词(额外包含重音线索)的偏好。两组婴儿对母语单音节单词的偏好都被观察到,但对于双音节单词则不是这样,学习泰语的婴儿没有表现出母语偏好。这些发现表明,早在6个月大的时候,习得声调语言和非声调语言的婴儿就能仅仅依靠词汇语调线索来识别母语,但当两个基于音调的线索同时出现在信号中时,声调语言婴儿就不再能成功地识别母语。
{"title":"Infants’ Sensitivity to Lexical Tone and Word Stress in Their First Year: A Thai and English Cross-Language Study","authors":"M. Kalashnikova, C. Onsuwan, D. Burnham","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1954928","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1954928","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Non-tone language infants’ native language recognition is based first on supra-segmental then segmental cues, but this trajectory is unknown for tone-language infants. This study investigated non-tone (English) and tone (Thai) language 6- to 10-month-old infants’ preference for English vs. Thai one-syllable words (containing segmental and tone cues) and two-syllable words (additionally containing stress cues). A preference for their native one-syllable words was observed in each of the two groups of infants, but this was not the case for two-syllable words where Thai-learning infants showed no native-language preference. These findings indicate that as early as six months of age, infants acquiring tone- and non-tone languages identify their native language by relying solely on lexical tone cues, but tone language infants no longer show successful identification of their native language when two pitch-based cues co-occur in the signal.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"159 1","pages":"278 - 293"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80069763","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Effect of Explicit Instruction on Implicit and Explicit Linguistic Knowledge in Kindergartners 外显教学对幼儿内隐和外显语言知识的影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-08-04 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1941968
S. Spit, Sible Andringa, J. Rispens, E. Aboh
ABSTRACT Research consistently shows that adults engaged in tutored acquisition benefit from explicit instruction in several linguistic domains. For preschool children, it is often assumed that such explicit instruction does not make a difference. In the present study, we investigated whether explicit instruction affected young learners in acquiring a morpho-syntactic element. A total of 103 Dutch-speaking kindergartners (M = 5;7) received training in a miniature language to learn a meaningful agreement marker. Results from a picture matching task, during which eye movements were recorded, provided no evidence that explicit instruction led to higher accuracy rates, but suggest that it did lead to earlier predictive eye movements. These data seem incompatible with the idea that explicit instruction does not make a difference when kindergartners learn a grammatical element, and tentatively suggest that explicit instruction has a different effect on explicit knowledge than on implicit knowledge in this age group.
研究一致表明,从事辅导习得的成年人受益于几个语言领域的显性教学。对于学龄前儿童,人们通常认为这种明确的指导不会产生影响。在本研究中,我们调查了外显教学是否影响了青少年学习者对一种形态句法元素的习得。共有103名荷兰语幼儿园儿童(M = 5;7)接受了微型语言培训,学习有意义的协议标记。在一项图片匹配任务中,眼球运动被记录下来,结果显示,没有证据表明明确的指示会导致更高的准确率,但表明它确实会导致更早的预测眼球运动。这些数据似乎与外显教学对幼儿园儿童学习语法元素没有影响的观点不一致,并初步表明外显教学对这个年龄段的外显知识和内隐知识的影响不同。
{"title":"The Effect of Explicit Instruction on Implicit and Explicit Linguistic Knowledge in Kindergartners","authors":"S. Spit, Sible Andringa, J. Rispens, E. Aboh","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1941968","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1941968","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Research consistently shows that adults engaged in tutored acquisition benefit from explicit instruction in several linguistic domains. For preschool children, it is often assumed that such explicit instruction does not make a difference. In the present study, we investigated whether explicit instruction affected young learners in acquiring a morpho-syntactic element. A total of 103 Dutch-speaking kindergartners (M = 5;7) received training in a miniature language to learn a meaningful agreement marker. Results from a picture matching task, during which eye movements were recorded, provided no evidence that explicit instruction led to higher accuracy rates, but suggest that it did lead to earlier predictive eye movements. These data seem incompatible with the idea that explicit instruction does not make a difference when kindergartners learn a grammatical element, and tentatively suggest that explicit instruction has a different effect on explicit knowledge than on implicit knowledge in this age group.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"122 1","pages":"201 - 228"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-08-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"74374098","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Learning to Read Interacts with Children’s Spoken Language Fluency 学习阅读与儿童口语流利度的相互作用
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-07-30 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1941032
A. Popescu, A. Noiray
ABSTRACT Until at least the end of adolescence, children articulate speech differently than adults. While this discrepancy is often attributed to the maturation of the speech motor system, we sought to demonstrate that the development of spoken language fluency is shaped by complex interactions across motor and cognitive domains. In this study, we specifically tested for a relationship between reading proficiency and coarticulatory organization, a fundamental correlate of spoken language fluency, used for both reading aloud and conversational speech. We conducted reading assessments and ultrasound-based kinematic measurements of intersegmental coarticulation in a group of 32 German children. In German, a language which supports rather consistent grapheme-to-phoneme relationships, reading aloud uses similar phoneme to speech motor gesture correspondences as well as coarticulatory mechanisms as conversational speech. Using general additive modeling we found that better readers exhibited lower degrees of intersegmental coarticulation than poorer readers. This study therefore provides evidence that reading proficiency interacts with coarticulatory patterns in beginning readers. It suggests that in addition to maturational factors, interactions between speech motor ability and other co-developing skills must be considered to fully account for spoken language fluency.
至少在青春期结束之前,儿童的语言表达方式与成人不同。虽然这种差异通常归因于言语运动系统的成熟,但我们试图证明口语流利性的发展是由运动和认知领域的复杂相互作用形成的。在这项研究中,我们专门测试了阅读熟练程度和协同发音组织之间的关系,协同发音组织是口语流利性的基本相关,用于大声朗读和会话演讲。我们对32名德国儿童进行了阅读评估和基于超声的节间协同关节运动学测量。在德语这种支持相当一致的字素-音素关系的语言中,大声朗读使用类似于语音运动手势对应的音素,以及会话语音的协同发音机制。使用一般的加性建模,我们发现较好的阅读者比较差的阅读者表现出较低的节间协同发音程度。因此,本研究提供了阅读能力与初级读者协同发音模式相互作用的证据。这表明,除了成熟因素外,必须考虑言语运动能力和其他共同发展技能之间的相互作用,才能充分解释口语流利性。
{"title":"Learning to Read Interacts with Children’s Spoken Language Fluency","authors":"A. Popescu, A. Noiray","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1941032","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1941032","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Until at least the end of adolescence, children articulate speech differently than adults. While this discrepancy is often attributed to the maturation of the speech motor system, we sought to demonstrate that the development of spoken language fluency is shaped by complex interactions across motor and cognitive domains. In this study, we specifically tested for a relationship between reading proficiency and coarticulatory organization, a fundamental correlate of spoken language fluency, used for both reading aloud and conversational speech. We conducted reading assessments and ultrasound-based kinematic measurements of intersegmental coarticulation in a group of 32 German children. In German, a language which supports rather consistent grapheme-to-phoneme relationships, reading aloud uses similar phoneme to speech motor gesture correspondences as well as coarticulatory mechanisms as conversational speech. Using general additive modeling we found that better readers exhibited lower degrees of intersegmental coarticulation than poorer readers. This study therefore provides evidence that reading proficiency interacts with coarticulatory patterns in beginning readers. It suggests that in addition to maturational factors, interactions between speech motor ability and other co-developing skills must be considered to fully account for spoken language fluency.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"64 1","pages":"151 - 170"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85337765","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Children’s Linguistic Repertoires Across Dialect and Standard Speech: Mirroring Input or Co-constructing Sociolinguistic Identities? 跨方言和标准语音的儿童语言库:镜像输入还是共同构建社会语言身份?
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-07-20 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1922282
Irmtraud Kaiser
ABSTRACT The present study analyses 3- to 6-year-old children’s dialect-standard repertoires in an Austrian-Bavarian sociolinguistic setting and investigates how far individual repertoires can be explained by input and sociodemographic factors. Adults’ linguistic repertoires in the area typically comprise a certain spectrum on the dialect-standard continuum but individual acquisition processes have hardly been studied yet. We collected language data from 49 children in five different communicative interactions each and analyzed the repertoire each child exhibits. The majority of children could be shown to have a bi-varietal repertoire at their disposal, but there were substantial numbers of children who exhibited either standard-only or dialect-only repertoires. We then examined the relationships between a child’s repertoire and potentially relevant input and sociodemographic variables. While language variety use in the home and maternal education did not prove significant predictors of children’s repertoires, gender, age, location, bilingualism and frequency of being read to did.
本研究分析了奥地利-巴伐利亚社会语言学背景下3- 6岁儿童的方言标准曲目,并调查了个人曲目在多大程度上可以由输入和社会人口因素解释。成人在该地区的语言库通常包括方言标准连续体上的一定频谱,但个体习得过程尚未得到研究。我们收集了49名儿童在5种不同的交际互动中的语言数据,并分析了每个儿童所表现出的语言能力。大多数儿童都能掌握两种语言,但也有相当数量的儿童只能掌握标准语言或方言语言。然后,我们检查了儿童的曲目与潜在的相关输入和社会人口变量之间的关系。而在家庭和母亲教育中使用的语言多样性并没有证明对儿童的语言能力、性别、年龄、地点、双语和被读给孩子听的频率有显著的预测作用。
{"title":"Children’s Linguistic Repertoires Across Dialect and Standard Speech: Mirroring Input or Co-constructing Sociolinguistic Identities?","authors":"Irmtraud Kaiser","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1922282","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1922282","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The present study analyses 3- to 6-year-old children’s dialect-standard repertoires in an Austrian-Bavarian sociolinguistic setting and investigates how far individual repertoires can be explained by input and sociodemographic factors. Adults’ linguistic repertoires in the area typically comprise a certain spectrum on the dialect-standard continuum but individual acquisition processes have hardly been studied yet. We collected language data from 49 children in five different communicative interactions each and analyzed the repertoire each child exhibits. The majority of children could be shown to have a bi-varietal repertoire at their disposal, but there were substantial numbers of children who exhibited either standard-only or dialect-only repertoires. We then examined the relationships between a child’s repertoire and potentially relevant input and sociodemographic variables. While language variety use in the home and maternal education did not prove significant predictors of children’s repertoires, gender, age, location, bilingualism and frequency of being read to did.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"45 1","pages":"41 - 61"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76777993","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Improvement of Communicative-pragmatic Ability in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Adapted Version of the Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment 自闭症谱系障碍青少年交际语用能力的提高:认知语用治疗的改编版本
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-06-20 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1924177
I. Gabbatore, C. Longobardi, F. Bosco
ABSTRACT Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex pathology that includes impaired social interaction abilities. Insufficient attention has been paid to programs specifically devoted to improving communicative-pragmatic skills. Moreover, the majority of studies have focused on children, while programs specifically developed for the adolescents are lacking. The present study aims to test the feasibility and acceptability of the adapted version of the Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment for adolescents (A-CPT), a 15-session group training, as well as its ability to improve the communicative-pragmatic performance of adolescents with ASD. Twenty-one verbally fluent adolescents with ASD took part in the training; they were assessed in three phases, i.e., before, after and at three-month follow-up, using the equivalent forms of the Assessment Battery for Communication (ABaCo), a tool for testing a wide range of pragmatic phenomena, such as direct and indirect speech acts, irony, deceit and violation of Grice’s maxims, expressed through linguistic, non-verbal, i.e., gestures, or paralinguistic expressive means. Furthermore, Theory of Mind (ToM) tasks and tests investigating the main cognitive domains, for example, Executive Functions (planning, shifting, working memory) and long-term memory, were administered. The results showed an improvement in participants’ performance in all the four scales of the ABaCo, i.e., linguistic, extralinguistic, paralinguistic and context scale; this improvement was maintained at follow-up assessment three months after the end of the program. No improvement was observed in the cognitive and ToM domains investigated, with the only exception of expressive vocabulary task. Despite the lack of a control group, the high degree of feasibility of the CPT, highlight the importance of more work needed in this research line.
自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)是一种复杂的病理,包括社会互动能力受损。对专门致力于提高沟通实用技能的项目重视不够。此外,大多数研究都集中在儿童身上,而专门为青少年开发的项目却缺乏。本研究旨在检验15期小组训练的青少年认知语用治疗(a - cpt)的可行性和可接受性,以及其对ASD青少年交际语用表现的改善能力。21名言语流利的ASD青少年参加了培训;他们被分为三个阶段进行评估,即在三个月的随访之前,之后和之后,使用沟通评估电池(ABaCo)的等效形式,这是一种测试广泛的语用现象的工具,如直接和间接的言语行为,讽刺,欺骗和违反格莱斯格言,通过语言,非语言,即手势或副语言表达手段表达。此外,还进行了心理理论(ToM)任务和测试,调查主要的认知领域,例如执行功能(计划、转移、工作记忆)和长期记忆。结果表明,被试在ABaCo的语言、外语言、副语言和语境四个维度上的表现均有显著提高;这种改善在项目结束后三个月的随访评估中保持不变。除表达性词汇任务外,在认知和ToM领域均未观察到任何改善。尽管缺乏对照组,但CPT的高度可行性,突出了在这一研究领域需要做更多工作的重要性。
{"title":"Improvement of Communicative-pragmatic Ability in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder: The Adapted Version of the Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment","authors":"I. Gabbatore, C. Longobardi, F. Bosco","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1924177","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1924177","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex pathology that includes impaired social interaction abilities. Insufficient attention has been paid to programs specifically devoted to improving communicative-pragmatic skills. Moreover, the majority of studies have focused on children, while programs specifically developed for the adolescents are lacking. The present study aims to test the feasibility and acceptability of the adapted version of the Cognitive Pragmatic Treatment for adolescents (A-CPT), a 15-session group training, as well as its ability to improve the communicative-pragmatic performance of adolescents with ASD. Twenty-one verbally fluent adolescents with ASD took part in the training; they were assessed in three phases, i.e., before, after and at three-month follow-up, using the equivalent forms of the Assessment Battery for Communication (ABaCo), a tool for testing a wide range of pragmatic phenomena, such as direct and indirect speech acts, irony, deceit and violation of Grice’s maxims, expressed through linguistic, non-verbal, i.e., gestures, or paralinguistic expressive means. Furthermore, Theory of Mind (ToM) tasks and tests investigating the main cognitive domains, for example, Executive Functions (planning, shifting, working memory) and long-term memory, were administered. The results showed an improvement in participants’ performance in all the four scales of the ABaCo, i.e., linguistic, extralinguistic, paralinguistic and context scale; this improvement was maintained at follow-up assessment three months after the end of the program. No improvement was observed in the cognitive and ToM domains investigated, with the only exception of expressive vocabulary task. Despite the lack of a control group, the high degree of feasibility of the CPT, highlight the importance of more work needed in this research line.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"34 1","pages":"62 - 80"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-06-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"82603580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
An Adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates CDI in 17 Arabic Dialects for Children Aged 8 to 30 Months 为8至30个月的儿童改编的麦克阿瑟-贝茨CDI 17种阿拉伯语方言
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-04-25 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1916502
A. Abdelwahab, Samuel H. Forbes, A. Cattani, Jeremy Goslin, Caroline Floccia
ABSTRACT Assessing a child’s language in the early years is critical to plan for an early intervention and maximize their learning potential. In a unique pan-Arabic approach to language development, we developed a new Arabic assessment tool, usable by parents and Early Years professionals to screen vocabulary in children between 8 months and 30 months across 17 Arab countries. Departing from the two relevant original Communicative Development Inventory forms (CDI: Words and Gestures and CDI: Words and Sentences, Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Reznick, & Bates, 2007), our Arabic CDI focuses on Words Only (Short Form), and assesses comprehension and production of a list of 100 words in 17 main dialects or Arabic, through a parental report. Data were collected from 436 Egyptian children and 168 children from the remaining 16 countries. Quasi-binomial model fits on Egyptian and Other Dialects comprehension and production data showed that Egyptian vocabulary norms could be reasonably extrapolated to the Other Dialects sample, as a first indication that the tool might be usable across the different countries.
早期评估儿童的语言能力对于制定早期干预计划和最大限度地发挥儿童的学习潜力至关重要。在一种独特的泛阿拉伯语语言发展方法中,我们开发了一种新的阿拉伯语评估工具,供父母和幼儿教育专业人员使用,用于筛选17个阿拉伯国家8个月至30个月儿童的词汇量。与两种相关的原始交际发展量表(CDI:单词和手势和CDI:单词和句子,Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Reznick, & Bates, 2007)不同,我们的阿拉伯语CDI只关注单词(简短形式),并通过家长报告评估17种主要方言或阿拉伯语的100个单词的理解和产生。数据来自436名埃及儿童和其他16个国家的168名儿童。拟二项式模型适合于埃及语和其他方言的理解和生产数据,表明埃及语的词汇规范可以合理地外推到其他方言样本,这是该工具可能在不同国家可用的第一个迹象。
{"title":"An Adaptation of the MacArthur-Bates CDI in 17 Arabic Dialects for Children Aged 8 to 30 Months","authors":"A. Abdelwahab, Samuel H. Forbes, A. Cattani, Jeremy Goslin, Caroline Floccia","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1916502","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1916502","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Assessing a child’s language in the early years is critical to plan for an early intervention and maximize their learning potential. In a unique pan-Arabic approach to language development, we developed a new Arabic assessment tool, usable by parents and Early Years professionals to screen vocabulary in children between 8 months and 30 months across 17 Arab countries. Departing from the two relevant original Communicative Development Inventory forms (CDI: Words and Gestures and CDI: Words and Sentences, Fenson, Marchman, Thal, Reznick, & Bates, 2007), our Arabic CDI focuses on Words Only (Short Form), and assesses comprehension and production of a list of 100 words in 17 main dialects or Arabic, through a parental report. Data were collected from 436 Egyptian children and 168 children from the remaining 16 countries. Quasi-binomial model fits on Egyptian and Other Dialects comprehension and production data showed that Egyptian vocabulary norms could be reasonably extrapolated to the Other Dialects sample, as a first indication that the tool might be usable across the different countries.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"94 1","pages":"425 - 446"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"75014851","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
The Effect of the Number of Labeled Objects on Novel Referent Selection Across Short and Long Time Delays 标记对象数目对长、短时间延迟新参照选择的影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-04-06 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1909483
Erica H Wojcik
ABSTRACT Children often hear many new words in one conversation, and yet word learning research overwhelmingly focuses on how children learn and retrieve the meanings of single words. The current experiment tests how the number of labeled objects affects preschoolers’ novel word referent selection immediately after encoding and after a one-week delay. Seventy 3- to 6-year-olds were exposed to four novel objects. Half of the participants were given novel labels for two of the objects and half were given novel labels for all four. Label-referent mapping was tested with a four alternative forced-choice pointing task both immediately after exposure and one week later. Children performed worse overall after a week delay, replicating past work on novel word retention. While children performed significantly worse overall in the Four-Label condition, exploratory analyses revealed that this effect was driven solely by the second test trial immediately after exposure. Analyses suggest that referent selection is strongly influenced by in-the-moment constraints, such as label salience and pragmatic biases, and that these constraints are strongest immediately after novel word exposure.
儿童经常在一次对话中听到许多新单词,然而单词学习研究绝大多数集中在儿童如何学习和检索单个单词的含义上。目前的实验测试了标记对象的数量如何影响学龄前儿童在编码后立即和延迟一周后的新词语指称选择。73到6岁的孩子被暴露在四个新奇的物体面前。一半的参与者得到了其中两件物品的新标签,另一半得到了所有四件物品的新标签。标签-参照映射在暴露后立即和一周后用四种可选的强迫选择指向任务进行测试。延迟一周后,孩子们的整体表现更差,重复了过去在新单词记忆方面的工作。虽然儿童在四标签条件下的总体表现明显较差,但探索性分析显示,这种影响仅由暴露后立即进行的第二次试验驱动。分析表明,指称选择受到即时约束的强烈影响,例如标签显著性和语用偏见,这些约束在新单词暴露后立即最强。
{"title":"The Effect of the Number of Labeled Objects on Novel Referent Selection Across Short and Long Time Delays","authors":"Erica H Wojcik","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1909483","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1909483","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Children often hear many new words in one conversation, and yet word learning research overwhelmingly focuses on how children learn and retrieve the meanings of single words. The current experiment tests how the number of labeled objects affects preschoolers’ novel word referent selection immediately after encoding and after a one-week delay. Seventy 3- to 6-year-olds were exposed to four novel objects. Half of the participants were given novel labels for two of the objects and half were given novel labels for all four. Label-referent mapping was tested with a four alternative forced-choice pointing task both immediately after exposure and one week later. Children performed worse overall after a week delay, replicating past work on novel word retention. While children performed significantly worse overall in the Four-Label condition, exploratory analyses revealed that this effect was driven solely by the second test trial immediately after exposure. Analyses suggest that referent selection is strongly influenced by in-the-moment constraints, such as label salience and pragmatic biases, and that these constraints are strongest immediately after novel word exposure.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"22 1","pages":"411 - 424"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-04-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"84726851","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infant-directed Speech by Dutch Fathers: Increased Pitch Variability within and across Utterances 荷兰父亲的婴儿导向语:话语内部和话语之间音调变化的增加
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-03-21 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698
Titia Benders, J. StGeorge, R. Fletcher
ABSTRACT Although both fathers and mothers speak differently in infant-directed speech (IDS) compared to adult-directed speech (ADS), the acoustic characteristics of present-day paternal IDS are still insufficiently understood. To extend this understanding, 11 fathers and 17 mothers in The Netherlands were recorded interacting with their infant (260–476 days old; for IDS) and with an adult experimenter (for ADS). Both fathers and mothers were found to raise their average pitch, expand their pitch variability within utterance, and increase their pitch variability across utterances in IDS. Moreover, fathers increased their pitch variability within and across utterances more than mothers. The IDS produced by present-day Dutch-speaking fathers is thus acoustically highly dynamic, in line with fathers’ energetic interaction style.
尽管父亲和母亲在婴儿指向语(IDS)中的说话方式与成人指向语(ADS)不同,但目前人们对父亲的婴儿指向语(IDS)的声学特征仍知之甚少。为了进一步理解这一点,荷兰的11位父亲和17位母亲记录了他们与婴儿(260-476天;对于IDS)和一个成年实验者(对于ADS)。研究发现,父亲和母亲都提高了他们的平均音高,扩大了他们话语内的音高变异性,并增加了他们在IDS话语中的音高变异性。此外,父亲比母亲更多地增加了话语内部和话语之间的音调变化。如今讲荷兰语的父亲制作的IDS在声学上是高度动态的,符合父亲们精力充沛的互动风格。
{"title":"Infant-directed Speech by Dutch Fathers: Increased Pitch Variability within and across Utterances","authors":"Titia Benders, J. StGeorge, R. Fletcher","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Although both fathers and mothers speak differently in infant-directed speech (IDS) compared to adult-directed speech (ADS), the acoustic characteristics of present-day paternal IDS are still insufficiently understood. To extend this understanding, 11 fathers and 17 mothers in The Netherlands were recorded interacting with their infant (260–476 days old; for IDS) and with an adult experimenter (for ADS). Both fathers and mothers were found to raise their average pitch, expand their pitch variability within utterance, and increase their pitch variability across utterances in IDS. Moreover, fathers increased their pitch variability within and across utterances more than mothers. The IDS produced by present-day Dutch-speaking fathers is thus acoustically highly dynamic, in line with fathers’ energetic interaction style.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"1 1","pages":"292 - 325"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-03-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83035749","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Is Regularization Uniform across Linguistic Levels? Comparing Learning and Production of Unconditioned Probabilistic Variation in Morphology and Word Order 正则化在语言层次上是统一的吗?词法和词序的无条件概率变异的学习和产生比较
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-02-19 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1876697
Carmen Saldana, Kenny Smith, S. Kirby, J. Culbertson
ABSTRACT Languages exhibit variation at all linguistic levels, from phonology, to the lexicon, to syntax. Importantly, that variation tends to be (at least partially) conditioned on some aspect of the social or linguistic context. When variation is unconditioned, language learners regularize it – removing some or all variants, or conditioning variant use on context. Previous studies using artificial language learning experiments have documented regularizing behavior in the learning of lexical, morphological, and syntactic variation. These studies implicitly assume that regularization reflects uniform mechanisms and processes across linguistic levels. However, studies on natural language learning and pidgin/creole formation suggest that morphological and syntactic variation may be treated differently. In particular, there is evidence that morphological variation may be more susceptible to regularization. Here we provide the first systematic comparison of the strength of regularization across these two linguistic levels. In line with previous studies, we find that the presence of a favored variant can induce different degrees of regularization. However, when input languages are carefully matched – with comparable initial variability, and no variant-specific biases – regularization can be comparable across morphology and word order. This is the case regardless of whether the task is explicitly communicative. Overall, our findings suggest an overarching regularizing mechanism at work, with apparent differences among levels likely due to differences in inherent complexity or variant-specific biases. Differences between production and encoding in our tasks further suggest this overarching mechanism is driven by production.
语言在各个语言层次上都表现出差异,从音韵、词汇到句法。重要的是,这种差异往往(至少部分地)取决于社会或语言环境的某些方面。当变异是无条件的,语言学习者将其规范化-去除部分或全部变异,或根据上下文限制变异的使用。先前使用人工语言学习实验的研究已经记录了正则化行为在词汇、形态和句法变化的学习中。这些研究隐含地假设正则化反映了跨语言层次的统一机制和过程。然而,对自然语言学习和皮钦语/克里奥尔语形成的研究表明,形态和句法的变化可能被区别对待。特别是,有证据表明形态变异可能更容易受到正则化的影响。在这里,我们首次系统地比较了这两个语言层次上的正则化强度。与先前的研究一致,我们发现一个有利变体的存在可以诱导不同程度的正则化。然而,当输入语言被仔细匹配时——具有可比的初始可变性,并且没有变体特定的偏差——正则化可以跨词法和词序进行比较。无论任务是否具有明确的交际性,情况都是如此。总的来说,我们的研究结果表明,在工作中有一个总体的规则机制,由于内在复杂性或变异特异性偏差的差异,水平之间存在明显差异。在我们的任务中,生产和编码之间的差异进一步表明,这种首要机制是由生产驱动的。
{"title":"Is Regularization Uniform across Linguistic Levels? Comparing Learning and Production of Unconditioned Probabilistic Variation in Morphology and Word Order","authors":"Carmen Saldana, Kenny Smith, S. Kirby, J. Culbertson","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1876697","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876697","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Languages exhibit variation at all linguistic levels, from phonology, to the lexicon, to syntax. Importantly, that variation tends to be (at least partially) conditioned on some aspect of the social or linguistic context. When variation is unconditioned, language learners regularize it – removing some or all variants, or conditioning variant use on context. Previous studies using artificial language learning experiments have documented regularizing behavior in the learning of lexical, morphological, and syntactic variation. These studies implicitly assume that regularization reflects uniform mechanisms and processes across linguistic levels. However, studies on natural language learning and pidgin/creole formation suggest that morphological and syntactic variation may be treated differently. In particular, there is evidence that morphological variation may be more susceptible to regularization. Here we provide the first systematic comparison of the strength of regularization across these two linguistic levels. In line with previous studies, we find that the presence of a favored variant can induce different degrees of regularization. However, when input languages are carefully matched – with comparable initial variability, and no variant-specific biases – regularization can be comparable across morphology and word order. This is the case regardless of whether the task is explicitly communicative. Overall, our findings suggest an overarching regularizing mechanism at work, with apparent differences among levels likely due to differences in inherent complexity or variant-specific biases. Differences between production and encoding in our tasks further suggest this overarching mechanism is driven by production.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"298 1","pages":"158 - 188"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"79676176","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Positive Effects of Passive Voice Exposure on Children’s Passive Production During a Classroom Story-telling Training 课堂讲故事训练中被动语态暴露对儿童被动产生的积极影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 0 LANGUAGE & LINGUISTICS Pub Date : 2021-02-17 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1875830
M. Garraffa, Francesca Smart, M. Obregón
ABSTRACT The present study investigated the effect of classroom-based syntactic training on children’s abilities to produce passive sentences. Thirty-three monolingual English children (mean age 5;2), were involved in passive-voice training based on storytelling sessions within a priming design. The training was delivered in a classroom setting, with two classes randomly allocated to either an active sentence or a passive sentence training structure. All children were individually tested at post-training. Children in the passive condition generated 3.6 more passives than the children in the active voice condition. Pre-training language and memory abilities, as measured by both grammatical level with a standardized sentence comprehension task (TROG-2) and a verbal working memory task (Digit Span), were unrelated to number of passives produced at post training. The study supports and expands recent evidence on the benefit of rich language exposure in the classroom context and on the quick dynamic adaptation of the implicit learning mechanisms to language exposure activities.
摘要本研究旨在探讨基于课堂的句法训练对儿童被动句生成能力的影响。在启动设计中,33名单语英语儿童(平均年龄5岁;2岁)参与了基于讲故事环节的被动语态训练。训练是在教室环境中进行的,两个班级随机分配到主动句或被动句训练结构中。所有儿童均在训练后接受单独测试。被动语态下的儿童比主动语态下的儿童多产生3.6个被动语态。通过标准化句子理解任务(TROG-2)和言语工作记忆任务(数字广度)的语法水平来衡量,训练前的语言和记忆能力与训练后产生的被动语态数量无关。该研究支持并扩展了最近关于丰富的课堂语言暴露的益处以及内隐学习机制对语言暴露活动的快速动态适应的证据。
{"title":"Positive Effects of Passive Voice Exposure on Children’s Passive Production During a Classroom Story-telling Training","authors":"M. Garraffa, Francesca Smart, M. Obregón","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1875830","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1875830","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The present study investigated the effect of classroom-based syntactic training on children’s abilities to produce passive sentences. Thirty-three monolingual English children (mean age 5;2), were involved in passive-voice training based on storytelling sessions within a priming design. The training was delivered in a classroom setting, with two classes randomly allocated to either an active sentence or a passive sentence training structure. All children were individually tested at post-training. Children in the passive condition generated 3.6 more passives than the children in the active voice condition. Pre-training language and memory abilities, as measured by both grammatical level with a standardized sentence comprehension task (TROG-2) and a verbal working memory task (Digit Span), were unrelated to number of passives produced at post training. The study supports and expands recent evidence on the benefit of rich language exposure in the classroom context and on the quick dynamic adaptation of the implicit learning mechanisms to language exposure activities.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":"39 1","pages":"241 - 253"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-02-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83574457","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Language Learning and Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1