首页 > 最新文献

Language Learning and Development最新文献

英文 中文
Difference or delay? Syntax, semantics, and verb vocabulary development in typically developing and late-talking toddlers. 差异还是延迟?典型发育和晚说话幼儿的句法、语义和动词词汇发展。
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1977645
Sabrina Horvath, Justin B Kueser, Jaelyn Kelly, Arielle Borovsky

While semantic and syntactic properties of verb meaning can impact the success of verb learning at a single age, developmental changes in how these factors influence acquisition are largely unexplored. We ask whether the impact of syntactic and semantic properties on verb vocabulary development varies with age and language ability for toddlers aged 16 to 30 months in a large sample (N = 5520, N Late Talkers = 821; N Typically Developing = 4699, cutoff = 15th percentile) of vocabulary checklist data from the MacArthur- Bates Communicative Development Inventory (MBCDI). Verbs from the MBCDI were coded for their syntactic and semantic properties, including manner/result meanings, durative/punctual events, and syntactic complexity. Both late talkers and typically developing children were less likely to produce syntactically complex verbs at younger ages as compared to older ages. Group differences emerged for manner/result: Typically developing children were more likely to produce manner verbs at all ages, but late talkers were more likely to produce result verbs. Regardless of group, children who produced more manner versus result verbs also had larger verb vocabulary sizes overall. These results suggest that late talkers and typically developing toddlers differ in how they build their verb vocabularies.

虽然动词意义的语义和句法特性可以影响单个年龄的动词学习成功,但这些因素如何影响习得的发展变化在很大程度上尚未得到探索。我们对16 ~ 30个月的幼儿(N = 5520, N = 821; N = 821)进行了大样本调查,研究句法和语义特征对动词词汇发展的影响是否随年龄和语言能力而变化。麦克阿瑟-贝茨交际发展量表(MBCDI)的词汇表数据的N典型发展= 4699,截止点=第15百分位)。来自MBCDI的动词根据其语法和语义属性进行编码,包括方式/结果含义、持续/准时事件和语法复杂性。与年龄较大的孩子相比,晚说话的孩子和正常发育的孩子在年龄较小的时候都不太可能产生句法复杂的动词。在方式/结果方面出现了组间差异:正常发育的孩子在所有年龄段都更有可能产生方式动词,但晚说话的孩子更有可能产生结果动词。无论哪一组,产生更多方式动词和结果动词的孩子总体上也有更大的动词词汇量。这些结果表明,晚说话者和正常发育的幼儿在如何构建动词词汇方面存在差异。
{"title":"Difference or delay? Syntax, semantics, and verb vocabulary development in typically developing and late-talking toddlers.","authors":"Sabrina Horvath,&nbsp;Justin B Kueser,&nbsp;Jaelyn Kelly,&nbsp;Arielle Borovsky","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1977645","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1977645","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While semantic and syntactic properties of verb meaning can impact the success of verb learning at a single age, developmental changes in how these factors influence acquisition are largely unexplored. We ask whether the impact of syntactic and semantic properties on verb vocabulary development varies with age and language ability for toddlers aged 16 to 30 months in a large sample (<i>N</i> = 5520, <i>N</i> <sub>Late Talkers</sub> = 821; <i>N</i> <sub>Typically Developing</sub> = 4699, cutoff = 15th percentile) of vocabulary checklist data from the MacArthur- Bates Communicative Development Inventory (MBCDI). Verbs from the MBCDI were coded for their syntactic and semantic properties, including manner/result meanings, durative/punctual events, and syntactic complexity. Both late talkers and typically developing children were less likely to produce syntactically complex verbs at younger ages as compared to older ages. Group differences emerged for manner/result: Typically developing children were more likely to produce manner verbs at all ages, but late talkers were more likely to produce result verbs. Regardless of group, children who produced more manner versus result verbs also had larger verb vocabulary sizes overall. These results suggest that late talkers and typically developing toddlers differ in how they build their verb vocabularies.</p>","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2022-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9159542/pdf/nihms-1739008.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10450517","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 9
Semantic Preview Benefit of Tibetan-Chinese Bilinguals during Chinese Reading 藏汉双语者在汉语阅读中的语义预览效应
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-28 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.2003198
Xu Xiao, Gaoding Jia, Aiping Wang
ABSTRACT When reading Chinese, skilled native readers regularly gain a preview benefit (PB) when the parafoveal word is orthographically or semantically related to the target word. Evidence shows that non-native, beginning Chinese readers can obtain an orthographic PB during Chinese reading, which indicates the parafoveal processing of low-level visual information. However, whether non-native Chinese readers who are more proficient in Chinese can make use of high-level parafoveal information remains unknown. Therefore, this study examined parafoveal processing during Chinese reading among Tibetan-Chinese bilinguals with high Chinese proficiency and compared their PB effects with those from native Chinese readers. Tibetan-Chinese bilinguals demonstrated both orthographic and semantic PB but did not show phonological PB and only differed from native Chinese in the identical PB when preview characters were identical to the targets. These findings demonstrate that non-native Chinese readers can extract semantic information from parafoveal preview during Chinese reading and highlight the modulation of parafoveal processing efficiency by reading proficiency. The results are in line with the direct route to access the mental lexicon of visual Chinese characters among non-native Chinese speakers.
摘要:熟练的母语读者在阅读汉语时,当准中心词在正字法或语义上与目标词相关时,通常会获得预览效应。有证据表明,非母语、初学汉语的读者在汉语阅读过程中可以获得正字型视觉信息,这表明在阅读过程中存在着低级视觉信息的旁中央凹加工。然而,非母语的汉语读者是否能够更熟练地使用高水平的中央凹旁信息仍然是未知的。因此,本研究考察了高汉语水平藏汉双语者在汉语阅读过程中的旁中央凹加工,并比较了他们与母语汉语读者的阅读效应。藏汉双语者在正字法和语义学上均表现出显性显性,但在语音上不表现出显性显性,仅在预览字与目标字相同时才表现出与汉语母语者相同的显性显性。研究结果表明,非母语汉语读者在阅读过程中可以从旁中央凹预览中提取语义信息,并突出了阅读水平对旁中央凹加工效率的调节作用。研究结果与非母语人士直接获取视觉汉字心理词汇的途径一致。
{"title":"Semantic Preview Benefit of Tibetan-Chinese Bilinguals during Chinese Reading","authors":"Xu Xiao, Gaoding Jia, Aiping Wang","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.2003198","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.2003198","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT When reading Chinese, skilled native readers regularly gain a preview benefit (PB) when the parafoveal word is orthographically or semantically related to the target word. Evidence shows that non-native, beginning Chinese readers can obtain an orthographic PB during Chinese reading, which indicates the parafoveal processing of low-level visual information. However, whether non-native Chinese readers who are more proficient in Chinese can make use of high-level parafoveal information remains unknown. Therefore, this study examined parafoveal processing during Chinese reading among Tibetan-Chinese bilinguals with high Chinese proficiency and compared their PB effects with those from native Chinese readers. Tibetan-Chinese bilinguals demonstrated both orthographic and semantic PB but did not show phonological PB and only differed from native Chinese in the identical PB when preview characters were identical to the targets. These findings demonstrate that non-native Chinese readers can extract semantic information from parafoveal preview during Chinese reading and highlight the modulation of parafoveal processing efficiency by reading proficiency. The results are in line with the direct route to access the mental lexicon of visual Chinese characters among non-native Chinese speakers.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-12-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77163537","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Verbal and More: Multimodality in Adults’ and Toddlers’ Spontaneous Repetitions 语言和更多:成人和幼儿自发重复的多模态
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-12-28 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.2008939
Marta Casla Soler, Eva Murillo, Silvia Nieva, Jessica Rodríguez, Celia Méndez-Cabezas, Irene Rujas
ABSTRACT This study investigated verbal imitation from a multimodal point of view, considering the mutual influence of children’s and adults’ participation. Sixteen Spanish-speaking children were observed longitudinally at 21, 24, and 30 months of age in natural settings. We analyzed the multimodal characteristics of children’s and adults’ repetitions, considering whether they were verbal, verbal-gestural, or gestural. In addition, we also analyzed the multimodal characteristics of the utterances that were repeated (source). Measures of vocabulary and grammatical levels were also taken into account at the three points in development. Results showed that verbal-gestural repetitions were frequent in the speech of children and adults, although not as frequent as verbal repetitions. Nevertheless, verbal-gestural speech was reproduced more frequently than verbal speech. Adults were more likely to reproduce children’s speech when it included gestures, which was also related to children’s linguistic level. Furthermore, children and adults synchronize their multimodal communicative behaviors, coordinating the modality of their repetitions with the modality of the source speech. The results are discussed taking into account the need to study the multimodal characteristics of child-directed speech, as well as the need to study verbal repetition and multimodal communicative behaviors simultaneously, as forms of interaction that are essential to language development.
摘要本研究从多模态的角度考察了儿童和成人参与的相互影响。16名讲西班牙语的儿童在21、24和30个月大时在自然环境中进行纵向观察。我们分析了儿童和成人重复的多模态特征,考虑他们是言语的,言语-手势的,还是手势的。此外,我们还分析了重复话语的多模态特征(来源)。在发展的三个阶段,词汇和语法水平的衡量也被考虑在内。结果表明,在儿童和成人的讲话中,语言-手势的重复频率很高,尽管没有语言重复那么频繁。然而,语言手势语言的再现频率高于口头语言。成年人更有可能复制儿童的语言,当它包含手势时,这也与儿童的语言水平有关。此外,儿童和成人的多模态交际行为是同步的,他们重复的情态与源语的情态是协调的。讨论的结果考虑到需要研究儿童定向言语的多模态特征,以及需要同时研究言语重复和多模态交际行为,作为对语言发展至关重要的互动形式。
{"title":"Verbal and More: Multimodality in Adults’ and Toddlers’ Spontaneous Repetitions","authors":"Marta Casla Soler, Eva Murillo, Silvia Nieva, Jessica Rodríguez, Celia Méndez-Cabezas, Irene Rujas","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.2008939","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.2008939","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT This study investigated verbal imitation from a multimodal point of view, considering the mutual influence of children’s and adults’ participation. Sixteen Spanish-speaking children were observed longitudinally at 21, 24, and 30 months of age in natural settings. We analyzed the multimodal characteristics of children’s and adults’ repetitions, considering whether they were verbal, verbal-gestural, or gestural. In addition, we also analyzed the multimodal characteristics of the utterances that were repeated (source). Measures of vocabulary and grammatical levels were also taken into account at the three points in development. Results showed that verbal-gestural repetitions were frequent in the speech of children and adults, although not as frequent as verbal repetitions. Nevertheless, verbal-gestural speech was reproduced more frequently than verbal speech. Adults were more likely to reproduce children’s speech when it included gestures, which was also related to children’s linguistic level. Furthermore, children and adults synchronize their multimodal communicative behaviors, coordinating the modality of their repetitions with the modality of the source speech. The results are discussed taking into account the need to study the multimodal characteristics of child-directed speech, as well as the need to study verbal repetition and multimodal communicative behaviors simultaneously, as forms of interaction that are essential to language development.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-12-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"87965297","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Type of Feedback Provided Can Affect Morphological Rule Learning of Young Children 反馈类型对幼儿词形规则学习的影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-11-02 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1986399
Sara Ferman, Sapir Amira Shmuel, Y. Zaltz
ABSTRACT The acquisition of a new morphological rule can be influenced by numerous factors, including the type of feedback provided during learning. The present study aimed to test the effect of different feedback types on children’s ability to learn and generalize an artificial morphological rule (AMR). Two groups of eight-year-olds learned to judge and produce repeated and new (generalization) items representing the AMR during ten training sessions. One group (n = 7) received only corrective feedback, that is, heard “the correct answer is … ” after each incorrect answer, whereas the other group (n = 8) received corrective feedback following verification feedback, that is, heard “incorrect, the correct answer is … .” Performance in terms of accuracy and reaction times was compared to that of an additional eight-year-old group (n = 8) from a previous study who received only verification feedback, that is, heard “incorrect” following each incorrect answer. The data analysis that was conducted for all three groups (N = 23 total), with ten observations for each child revealed that corrective feedback improved implicit learning of the AMR and in some cases also allowed generalization to new items. The combination of verification and corrective feedback, however, yielded the best performance in generalizing the AMR, possibly by stimulating both implicit and explicit processes. These preliminary findings suggest that corrective feedback, and even more so combined corrective+verification feedback, can enhance procedural and declarative learning processes of young school-age children. Future studies may be necessary to test this inference in a larger group of school-age children, and across ages.
新形态规则的习得受到许多因素的影响,包括学习过程中提供的反馈类型。本研究旨在探讨不同类型的反馈对儿童人工形态规则学习和概括能力的影响。两组8岁的孩子在10次训练中学会了判断和产生代表AMR的重复和新的(概括)项目。其中一组(n = 7)只收到纠正性反馈,即每次回答错误后听到“正确答案是……”,而另一组(n = 8)在收到验证性反馈后收到纠正性反馈,即听到“不正确,正确答案是... .”。在准确性和反应时间方面的表现与先前研究中另一组8岁儿童(n = 8)进行比较,该组只收到验证性反馈,即每个不正确的答案后面都跟着“不正确”。对所有三组(N = 23)进行的数据分析,对每个孩子进行10次观察,显示纠正反馈改善了AMR的内隐学习,在某些情况下还允许对新项目的概括。然而,验证和纠正反馈的结合可能通过同时刺激内隐和外显过程,在泛化AMR方面产生了最好的性能。这些初步研究结果表明,纠正性反馈,甚至是纠正性反馈+验证性反馈的结合,可以提高学龄儿童的程序性和陈述性学习过程。未来的研究可能需要在更大的学龄儿童群体和不同年龄的儿童中检验这一推论。
{"title":"The Type of Feedback Provided Can Affect Morphological Rule Learning of Young Children","authors":"Sara Ferman, Sapir Amira Shmuel, Y. Zaltz","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1986399","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1986399","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The acquisition of a new morphological rule can be influenced by numerous factors, including the type of feedback provided during learning. The present study aimed to test the effect of different feedback types on children’s ability to learn and generalize an artificial morphological rule (AMR). Two groups of eight-year-olds learned to judge and produce repeated and new (generalization) items representing the AMR during ten training sessions. One group (n = 7) received only corrective feedback, that is, heard “the correct answer is … ” after each incorrect answer, whereas the other group (n = 8) received corrective feedback following verification feedback, that is, heard “incorrect, the correct answer is … .” Performance in terms of accuracy and reaction times was compared to that of an additional eight-year-old group (n = 8) from a previous study who received only verification feedback, that is, heard “incorrect” following each incorrect answer. The data analysis that was conducted for all three groups (N = 23 total), with ten observations for each child revealed that corrective feedback improved implicit learning of the AMR and in some cases also allowed generalization to new items. The combination of verification and corrective feedback, however, yielded the best performance in generalizing the AMR, possibly by stimulating both implicit and explicit processes. These preliminary findings suggest that corrective feedback, and even more so combined corrective+verification feedback, can enhance procedural and declarative learning processes of young school-age children. Future studies may be necessary to test this inference in a larger group of school-age children, and across ages.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-11-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"80196008","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early Acquisition of Plural Morphology in a Classifier Language: Data from Korean 2-4 Year Olds 分类语言中复数词法的早期习得:来自韩国2-4岁儿童的数据
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-10-12 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1922280
Dorothy Ahn, J. Snedeker
ABSTRACT Korean is a classifier language in which bare nouns are not obligatorily number-marked. Children learning other classifier languages like Japanese and Mandarin are late in learning the plural morpheme. In this paper, we present two datasets that suggest that Korean plural marker -tul is acquired much earlier, in contrast to what has been previously claimed. In a comprehension study, we find that Korean children begin acquiring this morpheme by age 3, showing adult-like performance by age 4. We suggest that the higher frequency of plural marking on both types and tokens of nouns and the consistent marking of plural in the domain of definite nouns may facilitate Korean plural acquisition.
韩语是一种分类语言,它的裸名词没有强制性的数字标记。学习其他分类语言(如日语和普通话)的孩子学习复数语素的时间较晚。在本文中,我们提出了两个数据集,表明韩国复数标记-tul是更早获得的,与之前所声称的相反。在一项理解研究中,我们发现韩国儿童在3岁时开始习得这个语素,在4岁时表现出成人的表现。我们认为,在名词类型和标记上复数标记的频率较高,以及在确定名词领域中一致的复数标记可能有助于韩国语的复数习得。
{"title":"Early Acquisition of Plural Morphology in a Classifier Language: Data from Korean 2-4 Year Olds","authors":"Dorothy Ahn, J. Snedeker","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1922280","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1922280","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Korean is a classifier language in which bare nouns are not obligatorily number-marked. Children learning other classifier languages like Japanese and Mandarin are late in learning the plural morpheme. In this paper, we present two datasets that suggest that Korean plural marker -tul is acquired much earlier, in contrast to what has been previously claimed. In a comprehension study, we find that Korean children begin acquiring this morpheme by age 3, showing adult-like performance by age 4. We suggest that the higher frequency of plural marking on both types and tokens of nouns and the consistent marking of plural in the domain of definite nouns may facilitate Korean plural acquisition.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72931580","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Poverty of the Stimulus Without Tears 贫穷的刺激没有眼泪
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-10-04 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1981908
Tears, Lisa Pearl
ABSTRACT Poverty of the stimulus has been at the heart of ferocious and tear-filled debates at the nexus of psychology, linguistics, and philosophy for decades. This review is intended as a guide for readers without a formal linguistics or philosophy background, focusing on what poverty of the stimulus is and how it’s been interpreted, which is traditionally where the tears have come in. I discuss poverty of the stimulus from the perspective of language development, highlighting how poverty of the stimulus relates to expectations about learning and the data available to learn from. I describe common interpretations of what poverty of the stimulus means when it occurs, and approaches for determining when poverty of the stimulus is in fact occurring. I close with illustrative examples of poverty of the stimulus in the domains of syntax, lexical semantics, and phonology, and discuss the value of identifying instances of poverty of the stimulus when it comes to understanding language development.
几十年来,刺激计划的缺失一直是心理学、语言学和哲学之间激烈而充满泪水的争论的核心。这篇评论旨在为没有正式语言学或哲学背景的读者提供指导,重点关注刺激的贫困是什么,以及它是如何被解释的,这是传统上眼泪的来源。我从语言发展的角度讨论了刺激的缺乏,强调了刺激的缺乏如何与学习期望和可用的学习数据相关。我描述了刺激缺乏发生时对刺激缺乏意味着什么的常见解释,以及确定刺激缺乏何时实际发生的方法。最后,我列举了在句法、词汇语义和音韵学领域中刺激缺乏的例子,并讨论了在理解语言发展时识别刺激缺乏实例的价值。
{"title":"Poverty of the Stimulus Without Tears","authors":"Tears, Lisa Pearl","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1981908","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1981908","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Poverty of the stimulus has been at the heart of ferocious and tear-filled debates at the nexus of psychology, linguistics, and philosophy for decades. This review is intended as a guide for readers without a formal linguistics or philosophy background, focusing on what poverty of the stimulus is and how it’s been interpreted, which is traditionally where the tears have come in. I discuss poverty of the stimulus from the perspective of language development, highlighting how poverty of the stimulus relates to expectations about learning and the data available to learn from. I describe common interpretations of what poverty of the stimulus means when it occurs, and approaches for determining when poverty of the stimulus is in fact occurring. I close with illustrative examples of poverty of the stimulus in the domains of syntax, lexical semantics, and phonology, and discuss the value of identifying instances of poverty of the stimulus when it comes to understanding language development.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-10-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85594280","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 7
Objects Shape Activation during Spoken Word Recognition in Preschoolers with Typical and Atypical Language Development: An Eye-tracking Study 典型和非典型语言发展学龄前儿童口语单词识别过程中物体形状激活的眼动研究
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-09-24 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1977643
A. Helo, Ernesto Guerra, C. J. Coloma, María Antonia Reyes, P. Rämä
ABSTRACT Visually situated spoken words activate phonological, visual, and semantic representations guiding overt attention during visual exploration. We compared the activation of these representations in children with and without developmental language disorder (DLD) across four eye-tracking experiments, with a particular focus on visual (shape) representations. Two types of trials were presented in each experiment. In Experiment 1, participants heard a word while seeing (1) an object visually associated with the spoken word (i.e., shape competitor) together with a phonologically related object (i.e., cohort competitor), or (2) a shape competitor with an unrelated object. In Experiment 2 and 3, participants heard a word while seeing (1) a shape competitor with an object semantically related to the spoken word (i.e., semantic competitor), or (2) a shape competitor with an unrelated object. In Experiment 4, children heard a word while seeing a semantic competitor with (1) the visual referent of the spoken or (2) with an unrelated object. The visual context was previewed for three seconds before the spoken word, except for Experiment 2, where it appeared at the onset of the spoken word (i.e., no preview). The results showed that when a preview was provided both groups were equally attracted by cohort and semantic competitors and preferred the shape competitors over the unrelated objects. However, shape preference disappeared in the DLD group when no preview was provided and when the shape competitor was presented with a semantic competitor. Our results indicate that children with DLD have a less efficient retrieval of shape representation during word recognition compared to typically developing children.
视觉定位的口语单词激活语音、视觉和语义表征,引导视觉探索过程中的显性注意力。我们通过四次眼动追踪实验,比较了有发展性语言障碍(DLD)和没有发展性语言障碍(DLD)的儿童这些表征的激活情况,特别关注视觉(形状)表征。在每个实验中提出了两种类型的试验。在实验1中,参与者在听到一个单词的同时,看到(1)视觉上与口语单词相关的物体(即形状竞争者)和语音上相关的物体(即队列竞争者),或(2)形状竞争者和不相关的物体。在实验2和实验3中,参与者在听到一个单词的同时看到(1)一个形状竞争者和一个语义上相关的物体(即语义竞争者),或(2)一个形状竞争者和一个不相关的物体。在实验4中,孩子们在听到一个单词的同时,看到一个与(1)说话的视觉所指或(2)不相关的物体的语义竞争对手。除了实验2外,视觉语境在口语单词开始时出现(即没有预览),在口语单词出现前三秒预览。结果表明,当提供预览时,两组人都同样被队列竞争者和语义竞争者所吸引,并且更倾向于形状竞争者而不是不相关的物体。然而,当不提供预览和当形状竞争对手与语义竞争对手一起呈现时,DLD组的形状偏好消失。我们的研究结果表明,与正常发育的儿童相比,患有DLD的儿童在单词识别过程中对形状表征的检索效率较低。
{"title":"Objects Shape Activation during Spoken Word Recognition in Preschoolers with Typical and Atypical Language Development: An Eye-tracking Study","authors":"A. Helo, Ernesto Guerra, C. J. Coloma, María Antonia Reyes, P. Rämä","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1977643","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1977643","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Visually situated spoken words activate phonological, visual, and semantic representations guiding overt attention during visual exploration. We compared the activation of these representations in children with and without developmental language disorder (DLD) across four eye-tracking experiments, with a particular focus on visual (shape) representations. Two types of trials were presented in each experiment. In Experiment 1, participants heard a word while seeing (1) an object visually associated with the spoken word (i.e., shape competitor) together with a phonologically related object (i.e., cohort competitor), or (2) a shape competitor with an unrelated object. In Experiment 2 and 3, participants heard a word while seeing (1) a shape competitor with an object semantically related to the spoken word (i.e., semantic competitor), or (2) a shape competitor with an unrelated object. In Experiment 4, children heard a word while seeing a semantic competitor with (1) the visual referent of the spoken or (2) with an unrelated object. The visual context was previewed for three seconds before the spoken word, except for Experiment 2, where it appeared at the onset of the spoken word (i.e., no preview). The results showed that when a preview was provided both groups were equally attracted by cohort and semantic competitors and preferred the shape competitors over the unrelated objects. However, shape preference disappeared in the DLD group when no preview was provided and when the shape competitor was presented with a semantic competitor. Our results indicate that children with DLD have a less efficient retrieval of shape representation during word recognition compared to typically developing children.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"88223829","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
The Impact of Auditory Perceptual Training on the Perception and Production of English Vowels by Cypriot Greek Children and Adults 听觉知觉训练对塞浦路斯希腊族儿童和成人英语元音的感知和产生的影响
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-09-23 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1977644
G. Georgiou
ABSTRACT The purpose of this study is to investigate the effect of auditory perceptual phonetic training on the identification and production of English vowels by Cypriot Greek children and adults. Another two groups of Cypriot Greek child and adult speakers served as controls. The trained groups participated in the pretest, training, and posttest phase, while the controls completed only the pretest and posttest phase. The results showed that perceptual training improved identification accuracy, with children showing greater gains than adults. Although the performance of adults was poorer than the performance of children, their phonological system did undergo substantial alteration through perceptual phonetic training as they significantly improved their identifications in the posttest. Also, the results support a common mental space for the speech perception and production domains since the perceptually-oriented training affected the learners’ productions. However, transfer of improvements in production was observed only to some extent in children and not in adults, suggesting that training has an impact mostly on the trained modality and that some production improvement after perceptual training might be more evident in younger learners.
摘要本研究旨在探讨听觉感知语音训练对塞浦路斯希腊族儿童和成人识别和产生英语元音的影响。另外两组说希腊语的塞浦路斯儿童和成人作为对照。训练组参加前测、训练和后测阶段,而对照组只完成前测和后测阶段。结果显示,知觉训练提高了识别的准确性,儿童比成人表现出更大的进步。虽然成人的表现不如儿童,但他们的语音系统确实通过知觉语音训练发生了实质性的改变,因为他们在后测中显著提高了他们的识别能力。此外,由于以感知为导向的训练影响学习者的产出,结果支持语音感知和产出域的共同心理空间。然而,仅在儿童中观察到一定程度的生产改进转移,而在成人中没有观察到,这表明训练主要对训练的模态产生影响,并且在较年轻的学习者中,知觉训练后的一些生产改进可能更为明显。
{"title":"The Impact of Auditory Perceptual Training on the Perception and Production of English Vowels by Cypriot Greek Children and Adults","authors":"G. Georgiou","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1977644","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1977644","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT The purpose of this study is to investigate the effect of auditory perceptual phonetic training on the identification and production of English vowels by Cypriot Greek children and adults. Another two groups of Cypriot Greek child and adult speakers served as controls. The trained groups participated in the pretest, training, and posttest phase, while the controls completed only the pretest and posttest phase. The results showed that perceptual training improved identification accuracy, with children showing greater gains than adults. Although the performance of adults was poorer than the performance of children, their phonological system did undergo substantial alteration through perceptual phonetic training as they significantly improved their identifications in the posttest. Also, the results support a common mental space for the speech perception and production domains since the perceptually-oriented training affected the learners’ productions. However, transfer of improvements in production was observed only to some extent in children and not in adults, suggesting that training has an impact mostly on the trained modality and that some production improvement after perceptual training might be more evident in younger learners.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"85857834","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
Cross-generational Phonetic Alignment between Mothers and Their Children 母亲和孩子之间的跨代语音一致性
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-09-22 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1979401
Thomas St. Pierre, Angela Cooper, Elizabeth K. Johnson
ABSTRACT Over time, people who spend a lot of time together (e.g., roommates) begin sounding alike. Even over the course of short conversations, interlocutors often become more acoustically similar to one another. This phenomenon – known as phonetic alignment – has been well studied in adult interactions, but much less is known about alignment patterns in intergenerational, adult-child dyads. In the current study, we investigated alignment between mothers and their children in a picture-naming task, as assessed using a perceptual similarity task and acoustic measures. Experiments 1 and 2 examined alignment in 2.5- and 4-year-old children and their mothers, both when mothers shadowed their children (Experiment 1), and when children shadowed their mothers (Experiment 2). Experiments 3 and 4 investigated long-term similarity between mothers and children when they were recorded separately. Results show that children and mothers aligned to one another in the shadowing task, regardless of who shadowed whom, and while there was no evidence for long-term alignment in younger children, there was some evidence of long-term alignment with 8-year-old children and their moms, but only for male children.
随着时间的推移,经常在一起的人(比如室友)听起来会越来越像。即使在简短的谈话过程中,对话者在声音上也会变得更加相似。这种被称为语音对齐的现象已经在成人互动中得到了很好的研究,但对代际、成人-儿童双体的语音对齐模式知之甚少。在当前的研究中,我们调查了母亲和孩子在图片命名任务中的一致性,使用感知相似性任务和声学测量来评估。实验1和2检查了2.5岁和4岁的孩子和他们的母亲,当母亲跟随他们的孩子时(实验1),当孩子跟随他们的母亲时(实验2)。实验3和4调查了母亲和孩子之间的长期相似性,当他们分开记录时。结果显示,孩子和母亲在跟随任务中彼此一致,无论谁跟随谁,虽然没有证据表明年幼的孩子长期一致,但有一些证据表明,8岁的孩子和他们的妈妈长期一致,但仅限于男孩。
{"title":"Cross-generational Phonetic Alignment between Mothers and Their Children","authors":"Thomas St. Pierre, Angela Cooper, Elizabeth K. Johnson","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1979401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1979401","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT Over time, people who spend a lot of time together (e.g., roommates) begin sounding alike. Even over the course of short conversations, interlocutors often become more acoustically similar to one another. This phenomenon – known as phonetic alignment – has been well studied in adult interactions, but much less is known about alignment patterns in intergenerational, adult-child dyads. In the current study, we investigated alignment between mothers and their children in a picture-naming task, as assessed using a perceptual similarity task and acoustic measures. Experiments 1 and 2 examined alignment in 2.5- and 4-year-old children and their mothers, both when mothers shadowed their children (Experiment 1), and when children shadowed their mothers (Experiment 2). Experiments 3 and 4 investigated long-term similarity between mothers and children when they were recorded separately. Results show that children and mothers aligned to one another in the shadowing task, regardless of who shadowed whom, and while there was no evidence for long-term alignment in younger children, there was some evidence of long-term alignment with 8-year-old children and their moms, but only for male children.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"77480378","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
How Adults and Children Interpret Disjunction under Negation in Dutch, French, Hungarian and Italian: A Cross-Linguistic Comparison 成人和儿童如何理解荷兰语、法语、匈牙利语和意大利语中否定下的分离:跨语言比较
IF 1.5 2区 文学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2021-09-21 DOI: 10.1080/15475441.2021.1941966
E. Pagliarini, Oana Lungu, A. van Hout, Lilla Pintér, Balázs Surányi, S. Crain, M. Guasti
ABSTRACT In English, a sentence like “The cat didn’t eat the carrot or the pepper” typically receives a “neither” interpretation; in Japanese it receives a “not this or not that” interpretation. These two interpretations are in a subset/superset relation, such that the “neither” interpretation (strong reading) asymmetrically entails the “not this or not that” interpretation (weak reading). This asymmetrical entailment raises a learnability problem. According to the Semantic Subset Principle, all language learners, regardless of the language they are exposed to, start by assigning the strong reading, since this interpretation makes such sentences true in the narrowest range of circumstances.). If the “neither” interpretation is children’s initial hypothesis, then children acquiring a superset language will be able to revise their initial hypothesis on the basis of positive evidence. The aim of the present study is to test an additional account proposed by Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti (2018) as a possible explanation for the earlier convergence to the adult grammar by Italian children. The hypothesis tested here is that the presence of a lexical form such as recursive né that unambiguously conveys a “neither” meaning, would lead children to converge earlier to the adult grammar due to a blocking effect of the recursive né form in the inventory of negated disjunction forms in a language. We compared data from Italian (taken from Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti, 2018), French, Hungarian and Dutch. Dutch was tested as baseline language. French and Hungarian have – similarly to Italian – a lexical form that unambiguously expresses the “neither” interpretation (ni ni and sem sem, respectively). Our results did not support this hypothesis however, and are discussed in the light of language-specific particularities of the syntax and semantics of negation.
在英语中,像“猫没有吃胡萝卜或辣椒”这样的句子通常会被理解为“两者都不是”;在日语中,它得到了“不是这个,不是那个”的解释。这两种解释是在一个子集/超集关系中,因此“非”解释(强阅读)不对称地需要“不是这个或不是那个”解释(弱阅读)。这种不对称的蕴涵引发了一个易学性问题。根据语义子集原则,所有的语言学习者,无论他们接触的语言是什么,都是从分配强阅读开始的,因为这种解释使这样的句子在最狭窄的情况下是正确的。如果“两者都不是”的解释是儿童的初始假设,那么获得超集语言的儿童将能够在积极证据的基础上修正他们的初始假设。本研究的目的是验证Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti(2018)提出的另一种解释,即意大利儿童较早向成人语法趋同的可能解释。这里测试的假设是,由于递归n形式在语言中否定析取形式的列表中的阻塞效应,诸如递归n形式这样明确表达“neither”意思的词汇形式的存在,会导致儿童更早地收敛于成人语法。我们比较了意大利语(取自Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti, 2018)、法语、匈牙利语和荷兰语的数据。荷兰语被测试为基准语言。与意大利语类似,法语和匈牙利语有一种明确表达“neither”解释的词汇形式(分别是ni ni和sem sem)。然而,我们的结果并不支持这一假设,并根据语言的语法和语义的特殊性进行了讨论。
{"title":"How Adults and Children Interpret Disjunction under Negation in Dutch, French, Hungarian and Italian: A Cross-Linguistic Comparison","authors":"E. Pagliarini, Oana Lungu, A. van Hout, Lilla Pintér, Balázs Surányi, S. Crain, M. Guasti","doi":"10.1080/15475441.2021.1941966","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1941966","url":null,"abstract":"ABSTRACT In English, a sentence like “The cat didn’t eat the carrot or the pepper” typically receives a “neither” interpretation; in Japanese it receives a “not this or not that” interpretation. These two interpretations are in a subset/superset relation, such that the “neither” interpretation (strong reading) asymmetrically entails the “not this or not that” interpretation (weak reading). This asymmetrical entailment raises a learnability problem. According to the Semantic Subset Principle, all language learners, regardless of the language they are exposed to, start by assigning the strong reading, since this interpretation makes such sentences true in the narrowest range of circumstances.). If the “neither” interpretation is children’s initial hypothesis, then children acquiring a superset language will be able to revise their initial hypothesis on the basis of positive evidence. The aim of the present study is to test an additional account proposed by Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti (2018) as a possible explanation for the earlier convergence to the adult grammar by Italian children. The hypothesis tested here is that the presence of a lexical form such as recursive né that unambiguously conveys a “neither” meaning, would lead children to converge earlier to the adult grammar due to a blocking effect of the recursive né form in the inventory of negated disjunction forms in a language. We compared data from Italian (taken from Pagliarini, Crain, Guasti, 2018), French, Hungarian and Dutch. Dutch was tested as baseline language. French and Hungarian have – similarly to Italian – a lexical form that unambiguously expresses the “neither” interpretation (ni ni and sem sem, respectively). Our results did not support this hypothesis however, and are discussed in the light of language-specific particularities of the syntax and semantics of negation.","PeriodicalId":46642,"journal":{"name":"Language Learning and Development","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2021-09-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"76692901","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"文学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
期刊
Language Learning and Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1