首页 > 最新文献

Psychologica Belgica最新文献

英文 中文
Submissive Behavior and Cyber Bullying: A Study on the Mediator Roles of Cyber Victimization and Moral Disengagement. 顺从行为与网络欺凌:网络受害与道德脱离的中介作用研究。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2020-01-03 DOI: 10.5334/pb.509
Bahtiyar Eraslan-Çapan, Fuad Bakioğlu

In order to prevent cyberbullying and cyber-victim behaviors that are very common among adolescents, it is important to investigate the factors that underlie these behaviors. The purpose of the present study was to examine the mediator roles of cyber victimization and moral disengagement in the relationship between submissive behavior and cyber bullying. The participants involved 370 Turkish adolescents (female: 47%; male, 53%). The age of participants ranged between 12 and 19 years (M = 15.92, SD = 1.87). Data were collected using the Submissive Behavior Scale, the Cyber Bullying Scale, the Cyber Victimization Scale, and the Moral Disengagement Scale. The data were analyzed using structural equation modeling. A bootstrapping analysis was conducted in order to determine any indirect effects. Structural equation modeling results provided evidence of indirect effects of submissive behavior on cyber bullying mediated by cyber victimization and moral disengagement. Bootstrapping showed that submissive behavior exerted a significant indirect effect on cyber bullying via cyber victimization and moral disengagement. The findings emphasized the role of youth cyber victimization and moral disengagement in explaining the relationship between submissive behavior and cyber bullying. The results of the study were discussed based on relevant literature, and suggestions for future studies were made.

为了防止青少年中非常常见的网络欺凌和网络受害者行为,调查这些行为背后的因素是很重要的。本研究旨在探讨网络受害和道德脱离在服从行为与网络欺凌关系中的中介作用。参与者包括370名土耳其青少年(女性占47%;男性,53%)。参与者年龄在12 - 19岁之间(M = 15.92, SD = 1.87)。使用顺从行为量表、网络欺凌量表、网络受害量表和道德脱离量表收集数据。采用结构方程模型对数据进行分析。为了确定任何间接影响,进行了自举分析。结构方程模型的结果为网络受害和道德脱离介导的服从行为对网络欺凌的间接影响提供了证据。Bootstrapping表明,服从行为通过网络受害和道德脱离对网络欺凌产生显著的间接影响。研究结果强调了青少年网络受害和道德脱离在解释顺从行为和网络欺凌之间的关系中的作用。结合相关文献对研究结果进行了讨论,并对今后的研究提出了建议。
{"title":"Submissive Behavior and Cyber Bullying: A Study on the Mediator Roles of Cyber Victimization and Moral Disengagement.","authors":"Bahtiyar Eraslan-Çapan,&nbsp;Fuad Bakioğlu","doi":"10.5334/pb.509","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.509","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In order to prevent cyberbullying and cyber-victim behaviors that are very common among adolescents, it is important to investigate the factors that underlie these behaviors. The purpose of the present study was to examine the mediator roles of cyber victimization and moral disengagement in the relationship between submissive behavior and cyber bullying. The participants involved 370 Turkish adolescents (female: 47%; male, 53%). The age of participants ranged between 12 and 19 years (M = 15.92, SD = 1.87). Data were collected using the Submissive Behavior Scale, the Cyber Bullying Scale, the Cyber Victimization Scale, and the Moral Disengagement Scale. The data were analyzed using structural equation modeling. A bootstrapping analysis was conducted in order to determine any indirect effects. Structural equation modeling results provided evidence of indirect effects of submissive behavior on cyber bullying mediated by cyber victimization and moral disengagement. Bootstrapping showed that submissive behavior exerted a significant indirect effect on cyber bullying via cyber victimization and moral disengagement. The findings emphasized the role of youth cyber victimization and moral disengagement in explaining the relationship between submissive behavior and cyber bullying. The results of the study were discussed based on relevant literature, and suggestions for future studies were made.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"60 1","pages":"18-32"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6941236/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37523334","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 11
Clarifying the Role of Negative Emotions in the Origin and Control of Impulsive Actions. 澄清负面情绪在冲动行为的起源和控制中的作用。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2020-01-02 DOI: 10.5334/pb.502
Charlotte Eben, Joël Billieux, Frederick Verbruggen

This critical review elaborates on the origin of impulsive actions and how these can be controlled. We focus in particular on the role of negative events. First, we outline how impulsive actions often originate from negative events that are (emotionally) appraised. A discrepancy between this current state and a desired goal state leads to action tendencies. The urgency of the resulting action depends on the importance of the goal and the size of the discrepancy. Second, we discuss how such impulsive actions can be regulated or controlled e.g. by biasing competition between different options, or by completely suppressing all motor output. Importantly, such control mechanisms might also depend on emotional factors. To reconcile these findings, we present a coherent theoretical framework, taking into account various cognitive, affective, and motivational mechanisms as well as contextual factors that play a crucial role in the origin and control of impulsive actions.

这篇评论阐述了冲动行为的起源以及如何控制冲动行为。我们尤其关注负面事件的作用。首先,我们概述了冲动行为通常是如何源于负面事件的(情感)评价。当前状态与预期目标状态之间的差异会导致行动倾向。行动的紧迫性取决于目标的重要性和差异的大小。其次,我们将讨论如何调节或控制这种冲动行为,例如,通过偏置不同选项之间的竞争,或通过完全抑制所有运动输出。重要的是,这种控制机制可能还取决于情绪因素。为了协调这些发现,我们提出了一个连贯的理论框架,其中考虑到了在冲动行为的起源和控制中起关键作用的各种认知、情感和动机机制以及情境因素。
{"title":"Clarifying the Role of Negative Emotions in the Origin and Control of Impulsive Actions.","authors":"Charlotte Eben, Joël Billieux, Frederick Verbruggen","doi":"10.5334/pb.502","DOIUrl":"10.5334/pb.502","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This critical review elaborates on the origin of impulsive actions and how these can be controlled. We focus in particular on the role of negative events. First, we outline how impulsive actions often originate from negative events that are (emotionally) appraised. A discrepancy between this current state and a desired goal state leads to action tendencies. The urgency of the resulting action depends on the importance of the goal and the size of the discrepancy. Second, we discuss how such impulsive actions can be regulated or controlled e.g. by biasing competition between different options, or by completely suppressing all motor output. Importantly, such control mechanisms might also depend on emotional factors. To reconcile these findings, we present a coherent theoretical framework, taking into account various cognitive, affective, and motivational mechanisms as well as contextual factors that play a crucial role in the origin and control of impulsive actions.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"60 1","pages":"1-17"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2020-01-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6941237/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"37523333","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Advantage in Children Enrolled in a Second-Language Immersion Elementary School Program for One Year 参加一年第二语言浸入式小学课程的儿童的认知优势
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-10-21 DOI: 10.5334/pb.469
Cristina Barbu, Audrey Gonzalez, Sophie Gillet, M. Poncelet
Early bilingualism has been shown to improve attentional and executive functioning. Nicolay and Poncelet (2013a, 2015) have shown that an early immersion program in school of 3 years improves the completion of tasks assessing these skills. This study aimed to determine whether similar benefits might be present after only 1 year of immersion education. The study also observed whether these potential advantages might also have a positive effect on the academic achievement. Participants included 59 immersed children and 57 monolingual controls. The two groups were compared using the same tasks as those employed by Nicolay and Poncelet (2015). The immersed children showed faster responses in comparison to monolinguals on the selective auditory task. No significant differences were observed on the other attentional, executive, or academic tasks. These outcomes suggest that a period of immersion education as short as 1 year can yield cognitive advantages associated with bilingualism.
早期的双语已经被证明可以改善注意力和执行功能。Nicolay和Ponselet(2013a,2015)已经表明,在学校进行3年的早期沉浸式课程可以提高评估这些技能的任务的完成率。这项研究的目的是确定仅在接受1年的沉浸式教育后是否会出现类似的益处。该研究还观察了这些潜在优势是否也会对学业成绩产生积极影响。参与者包括59名儿童和57名单语对照。使用与Nicolay和Ponselet(2015)相同的任务对两组进行比较。与单语儿童相比,沉浸在其中的儿童在选择性听觉任务中表现出更快的反应。在其他注意力、执行或学术任务上没有观察到显著差异。这些结果表明,短至1年的沉浸式教育可以产生与双语相关的认知优势。
{"title":"Cognitive Advantage in Children Enrolled in a Second-Language Immersion Elementary School Program for One Year","authors":"Cristina Barbu, Audrey Gonzalez, Sophie Gillet, M. Poncelet","doi":"10.5334/pb.469","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.469","url":null,"abstract":"Early bilingualism has been shown to improve attentional and executive functioning. Nicolay and Poncelet (2013a, 2015) have shown that an early immersion program in school of 3 years improves the completion of tasks assessing these skills. This study aimed to determine whether similar benefits might be present after only 1 year of immersion education. The study also observed whether these potential advantages might also have a positive effect on the academic achievement. Participants included 59 immersed children and 57 monolingual controls. The two groups were compared using the same tasks as those employed by Nicolay and Poncelet (2015). The immersed children showed faster responses in comparison to monolinguals on the selective auditory task. No significant differences were observed on the other attentional, executive, or academic tasks. These outcomes suggest that a period of immersion education as short as 1 year can yield cognitive advantages associated with bilingualism.","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"416 - 435"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"44899586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 19
Norm and Deviance-Seeking Personal Orientation Scale (NDPOS) Adapted to the Organisational Context 适应组织情境的规范与偏差寻求型个人取向量表(NDPOS
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-10-21 DOI: 10.5334/pb.462
G. Déprez, A. Battistelli, Mirko Antino
Deviance theory introduces a behavioural view on constructive and destructive deviance to explain how an individual’s intent can harm or improve organisational well-being. However, to our knowledge, no scale exists that evaluates the personal orientation aspect of deviance and normativity. This article discusses the creation of the Norm and Deviance-Seeking Personal Orientation Scale (NDPOS). To create this scale, we studied the psychometric properties of the instrument using data from French workers. NDPOS exploratory analysis indicated a 12-item scale composed of four factors: normative conformity, normative rule adequacy, deviant performance seeking, and deviant proactivity seeking. Confirmatory factor analysis corroborated the factorial structure in four sub-scales. Convergent and discriminant validity indicated that deviant dimensions were positively related to expressing voice, cognitive flexibility, and deviant behaviours, whereas normativity dimensions were negatively or not related to these behaviours. Furthermore, opposite relations between the conformity construct and the four factors were observed. Practical implications and suggestions for the development of future research on constructive deviance theory are discussed.
偏差理论引入了一种关于建设性和破坏性偏差的行为观,以解释个人的意图如何损害或改善组织福祉。然而,据我们所知,不存在评估偏差和规范性的个人取向方面的量表。本文探讨了人格定向量表(NDPOS)的编制。为了创建这个量表,我们使用法国工人的数据研究了该仪器的心理测量特性。NDPOS探索性分析表明,12项量表由四个因素组成:规范一致性、规范规则充分性、越轨绩效寻求和越轨积极性寻求。确证因子分析在四个子量表中确证了因子结构。收敛和判别有效性表明,偏离维度与表达声音、认知灵活性和偏离行为呈正相关,而规范维度与这些行为呈负相关或不相关。此外,一致性结构与四个因素之间存在相反的关系。讨论了建设性偏差理论的实际意义和对未来研究发展的建议。
{"title":"Norm and Deviance-Seeking Personal Orientation Scale (NDPOS) Adapted to the Organisational Context","authors":"G. Déprez, A. Battistelli, Mirko Antino","doi":"10.5334/pb.462","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.462","url":null,"abstract":"Deviance theory introduces a behavioural view on constructive and destructive deviance to explain how an individual’s intent can harm or improve organisational well-being. However, to our knowledge, no scale exists that evaluates the personal orientation aspect of deviance and normativity. This article discusses the creation of the Norm and Deviance-Seeking Personal Orientation Scale (NDPOS). To create this scale, we studied the psychometric properties of the instrument using data from French workers. NDPOS exploratory analysis indicated a 12-item scale composed of four factors: normative conformity, normative rule adequacy, deviant performance seeking, and deviant proactivity seeking. Confirmatory factor analysis corroborated the factorial structure in four sub-scales. Convergent and discriminant validity indicated that deviant dimensions were positively related to expressing voice, cognitive flexibility, and deviant behaviours, whereas normativity dimensions were negatively or not related to these behaviours. Furthermore, opposite relations between the conformity construct and the four factors were observed. Practical implications and suggestions for the development of future research on constructive deviance theory are discussed.","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"393 - 415"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"43435135","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Psychometrics Anonymous: Does a Transparent Data Sharing Policy Affect Data Collection? 匿名心理测量:透明的数据共享政策会影响数据收集吗?
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-09-19 DOI: 10.5334/pb.503
J. Eberlen, Emmanuel Nicaise, Sarah Leveaux, Youri L. Mora, O. Klein
As researchers, we are advised to share our data to improve transparency and increase the reproducibility of experiments. Simultaneously, making data freely accessible can raise ethical questions regarding the participants’ privacy. We first outline the challenges regarding “open data” for researchers in light of the GDPR. Then, we turn to the impact of an open-access data sharing policy on the participants: could the participants’ knowledge about the future use of the data alter the data itself? Through two pre-registered studies (N = 193, collected on campus and N = 543, online participation), we investigate whether disclosing that anonymized data will be publicly shared vs. not shared influences a potential participants’ intention to take part in the study. Using both frequentist and Bayesian analysis, we conclude towards an absence of effect of a difference in data sharing policy on scores in the Big Five questionnaire and social desirability, careless response behavior, and results in the anchoring paradigm. In the second study, a lexicometric analysis of participants’ reactions to openly sharing data reveals a readiness to share data and support transparency under the condition of preserved anonymity. Hence, if anonymity can be ensured, there seems to be no methodological or ethical drawback in transparent and open data sharing policies for many psychological studies.
作为研究人员,我们被建议分享我们的数据,以提高透明度并增加实验的再现性。同时,让数据自由访问可能会引发有关参与者隐私的道德问题。我们首先根据GDPR概述了研究人员在“开放数据”方面面临的挑战。然后,我们转向开放访问数据共享政策对参与者的影响:参与者对数据未来使用的了解会改变数据本身吗?通过两项预先注册的研究(N=193,在校园内收集,N=543,在线参与),我们调查了披露匿名数据将公开共享与不共享是否会影响潜在参与者参与研究的意愿。使用频率分析和贝叶斯分析,我们得出结论,数据共享政策的差异对五大问卷的得分和社会期望、粗心的反应行为没有影响,并导致锚定范式。在第二项研究中,对参与者对公开共享数据的反应进行字典计量分析,揭示了在保持匿名的条件下共享数据和支持透明度的意愿。因此,如果能够确保匿名性,那么对于许多心理学研究来说,透明和公开的数据共享政策似乎没有方法或道德缺陷。
{"title":"Psychometrics Anonymous: Does a Transparent Data Sharing Policy Affect Data Collection?","authors":"J. Eberlen, Emmanuel Nicaise, Sarah Leveaux, Youri L. Mora, O. Klein","doi":"10.5334/pb.503","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.503","url":null,"abstract":"As researchers, we are advised to share our data to improve transparency and increase the reproducibility of experiments. Simultaneously, making data freely accessible can raise ethical questions regarding the participants’ privacy. We first outline the challenges regarding “open data” for researchers in light of the GDPR. Then, we turn to the impact of an open-access data sharing policy on the participants: could the participants’ knowledge about the future use of the data alter the data itself? Through two pre-registered studies (N = 193, collected on campus and N = 543, online participation), we investigate whether disclosing that anonymized data will be publicly shared vs. not shared influences a potential participants’ intention to take part in the study. Using both frequentist and Bayesian analysis, we conclude towards an absence of effect of a difference in data sharing policy on scores in the Big Five questionnaire and social desirability, careless response behavior, and results in the anchoring paradigm. In the second study, a lexicometric analysis of participants’ reactions to openly sharing data reveals a readiness to share data and support transparency under the condition of preserved anonymity. Hence, if anonymity can be ensured, there seems to be no methodological or ethical drawback in transparent and open data sharing policies for many psychological studies.","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"373 - 392"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-09-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49620575","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
A Social Psychological Model of Scientific Practices: Explaining Research Practices and Outlining the Potential for Successful Reforms. 科学实践的社会心理模型:解释研究实践并概述成功改革的潜力。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-09-12 DOI: 10.5334/pb.496
Lee Jussim, Jon A Krosnick, Sean T Stevens, Stephanie M Anglin

A crescendo of incidents have raised concerns about whether scientific practices in psychology may be suboptimal, sometimes leading to the publication, dissemination, and application of unreliable or misinterpreted findings. Psychology has been a leader in identifying possibly suboptimal practices and proposing reforms that might enhance the efficiency of the scientific process and the publication of robust evidence and interpretations. To help shape future efforts, this paper offers a model of the psychological and socio-structural forces and processes that may influence scientists' practices. The model identifies practices targeted by interventions and reforms, and which practices remain unaddressed. The model also suggests directions for empirical research to assess how best to enhance the effectiveness of psychological inquiry.

越来越多的事件引发了人们的担忧,即心理学的科学实践是否是次优的,有时会导致不可靠或误解的研究结果的发布、传播和应用。心理学在识别可能不太理想的做法和提出可能提高科学过程效率以及发表有力证据和解释的改革方面一直处于领先地位。为了帮助塑造未来的努力,本文提供了一个可能影响科学家实践的心理和社会结构力量和过程的模型。该模式确定了干预和改革的目标做法,以及哪些做法尚未得到解决。该模型还为实证研究提供了方向,以评估如何最好地提高心理探究的有效性。
{"title":"A Social Psychological Model of Scientific Practices: Explaining Research Practices and Outlining the Potential for Successful Reforms.","authors":"Lee Jussim,&nbsp;Jon A Krosnick,&nbsp;Sean T Stevens,&nbsp;Stephanie M Anglin","doi":"10.5334/pb.496","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.496","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A crescendo of incidents have raised concerns about whether scientific practices in psychology may be suboptimal, sometimes leading to the publication, dissemination, and application of unreliable or misinterpreted findings. Psychology has been a leader in identifying possibly suboptimal practices and proposing reforms that might enhance the efficiency of the scientific process and the publication of robust evidence and interpretations. To help shape future efforts, this paper offers a model of the psychological and socio-structural forces and processes that may influence scientists' practices. The model identifies practices targeted by interventions and reforms, and which practices remain unaddressed. The model also suggests directions for empirical research to assess how best to enhance the effectiveness of psychological inquiry.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"353-372"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-09-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6743032/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41215756","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
Preregistration of Analyses of Preexisting Data. 预先注册现有数据分析。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-08-22 DOI: 10.5334/pb.493
Gaëtan Mertens, Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos

The preregistration of a study's hypotheses, methods, and data-analyses steps is becoming a popular psychological research practice. To date, most of the discussion on study preregistration has focused on the preregistration of studies that include the collection of original data. However, much of the research in psychology relies on the (re-)analysis of preexisting data. Importantly, this type of studies is different from original studies as researchers cannot change major aspects of the study (e.g., experimental manipulations, sample size). Here, we provide arguments as to why it is useful to preregister analyses of preexisting data, discuss practical considerations, consider potential concerns, and introduce a preregistration template tailored for studies focused on the analyses of preexisting data. We argue that the preregistration of hypotheses and data-analyses for analyses of preexisting data is an important step towards more transparent psychological research.

预先登记研究的假设、方法和数据分析步骤正成为一种流行的心理学研究实践。到目前为止,大多数关于研究预登记的讨论都集中在研究的预登记上,包括原始数据的收集。然而,心理学的大部分研究都依赖于对先前存在的数据的(重新)分析。重要的是,这类研究与原始研究不同,因为研究人员无法改变研究的主要方面(例如,实验操作、样本量)。在这里,我们提供了关于为什么对现有数据进行预登记分析是有用的论点,讨论了实际考虑因素,考虑了潜在的问题,并引入了一个为专注于分析现有数据的研究量身定制的预登记模板。我们认为,为分析先前存在的数据而预先登记假设和数据分析是朝着更透明的心理研究迈出的重要一步。
{"title":"Preregistration of Analyses of Preexisting Data.","authors":"Gaëtan Mertens,&nbsp;Angelos-Miltiadis Krypotos","doi":"10.5334/pb.493","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.493","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The preregistration of a study's hypotheses, methods, and data-analyses steps is becoming a popular psychological research practice. To date, most of the discussion on study preregistration has focused on the preregistration of studies that include the collection of original data. However, much of the research in psychology relies on the (re-)analysis of preexisting data. Importantly, this type of studies is different from original studies as researchers cannot change major aspects of the study (e.g., experimental manipulations, sample size). Here, we provide arguments as to why it is useful to preregister analyses of preexisting data, discuss practical considerations, consider potential concerns, and introduce a preregistration template tailored for studies focused on the analyses of preexisting data. We argue that the preregistration of hypotheses and data-analyses for analyses of preexisting data is an important step towards more transparent psychological research.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"338-352"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6706998/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41215809","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 24
Open Up - the Mission Statement of the Control of Impulsive Action (Ctrl-ImpAct) Lab on Open Science. 开放-冲动行为控制实验室关于开放科学的使命声明。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-08-20 DOI: 10.5334/pb.494
Christina B Reimer, Zhang Chen, Carsten Bundt, Charlotte Eben, Raquel E London, Sirarpi Vardanian

The present paper is the mission statement of the Control of Impulsive Action (Ctrl-ImpAct) Lab regarding Open Science. As early-career researchers (ECRs) in the lab, we first state our personal motivation to conduct research based on the principles of Open Science. We then describe how we incorporate four specific Open Science practices (i.e., Open Methodology, Open Data, Open Source, and Open Access) into our scientific workflow. In more detail, we explain how Open Science practices are embedded into the so-called 'co-pilot' system in our lab. The 'co-pilot' researcher is involved in all tasks of the 'pilot' researcher, that is designing a study, double-checking experimental and data analysis scripts, as well as writing the manuscript. The lab has set up this co-pilot system to increase transparency, reduce potential errors that could occur during the entire workflow, and to intensify collaborations between lab members. Finally, we discuss potential solutions for general problems that could arise when practicing Open Science.

本文是冲动行为控制实验室关于开放科学的使命声明。作为实验室中的早期职业研究人员,我们首先陈述我们个人基于开放科学原则进行研究的动机。然后,我们描述了我们如何将四种特定的开放科学实践(即开放方法论、开放数据、开源和开放获取)纳入我们的科学工作流程。更详细地说,我们解释了开放科学实践是如何嵌入我们实验室所谓的“副试点”系统的。“副试点“研究人员参与了“试点”研究人员的所有任务,即设计研究、仔细检查实验和数据分析脚本,以及撰写手稿。实验室建立了这个联合试点系统,以提高透明度,减少整个工作流程中可能发生的潜在错误,并加强实验室成员之间的合作。最后,我们讨论了在实践开放科学时可能出现的一般问题的潜在解决方案。
{"title":"Open Up - the Mission Statement of the Control of Impulsive Action (Ctrl-ImpAct) Lab on Open Science.","authors":"Christina B Reimer,&nbsp;Zhang Chen,&nbsp;Carsten Bundt,&nbsp;Charlotte Eben,&nbsp;Raquel E London,&nbsp;Sirarpi Vardanian","doi":"10.5334/pb.494","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.494","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present paper is the mission statement of the Control of Impulsive Action (Ctrl-ImpAct) Lab regarding Open Science. As early-career researchers (ECRs) in the lab, we first state our personal motivation to conduct research based on the principles of Open Science. We then describe how we incorporate four specific Open Science practices (i.e., Open Methodology, Open Data, Open Source, and Open Access) into our scientific workflow. In more detail, we explain how Open Science practices are embedded into the so-called 'co-pilot' system in our lab. The 'co-pilot' researcher is involved in all tasks of the 'pilot' researcher, that is designing a study, double-checking experimental and data analysis scripts, as well as writing the manuscript. The lab has set up this co-pilot system to increase transparency, reduce potential errors that could occur during the entire workflow, and to intensify collaborations between lab members. Finally, we discuss potential solutions for general problems that could arise when practicing Open Science.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"321-337"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-08-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6707000/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41215808","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 6
High-Performance Work Systems and Well-Being: Mediating Role of Work-to-Family Interface 高绩效工作系统与幸福感:工作-家庭界面的中介作用
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-08-13 DOI: 10.5334/pb.473
A. Babic, F. Stinglhamber, I. Hansez
Regarding the effects of High-Performance Work Systems (HPWS), we can draw two conclusions. First, existing studies on the effects of HPWS on employees’ well-being at work are scarce. Second, few studies have considered the relationships between HPWS and work-to-family interface (i.e., work-to-family enrichment, WFE; and work-to-family conflict, WFC). Only one previous study conducted on a Portuguese sample (i.e., Carvalho & Chambel, 2016) has examined the relationships between these concepts in a comprehensive model. Our study aims to replicate one part of Carvalho and Chambel’s model but also to extend previous work. We investigated a model of HPWS-employees’ well-being at work (i.e., job engagement and job strain) relationships by considering work-to-family interface as a mediator. We surveyed 170 employees of a Belgian company. Data were analysed using structural equation modelling and bootstrapping method. WFE partially mediates the relationships between HPWS and job engagement, whereas WFC partially mediates the relationships between HPWS and job strain. Our study, confirming the results of Carvalho and Chambel (2016), highlights the important role of HPWS in the development of employees’ well-being at work. Working in an organization where HPWS are applied leads employees to perceive more enrichment and less conflict between their work and family lives, making them more engaged in and less stressed by their work.
关于高性能工作系统(HPWS)的影响,我们可以得出两个结论。首先,关于HPWS对员工工作幸福感影响的现有研究很少。其次,很少有研究考虑HPWS与工作与家庭界面之间的关系(即工作与家庭的丰富,WFE;以及工作与家庭冲突,WFC)。此前只有一项针对葡萄牙样本的研究(即Carvalho&Chambel,2016)在一个综合模型中检验了这些概念之间的关系。我们的研究旨在复制卡瓦略和张贝尔模型的一部分,但也扩展了之前的工作。我们通过将工作与家庭界面作为中介,研究了HPWS员工的工作幸福感(即工作投入和工作压力)关系模型。我们调查了一家比利时公司的170名员工。使用结构方程建模和自举方法对数据进行分析。WFE部分介导HPWS与工作投入之间的关系,而WFC部分介导HP WS与工作紧张之间的关系。我们的研究证实了Carvalho和Chambel(2016)的结果,强调了HPWS在员工工作幸福感发展中的重要作用。在应用HPWS的组织中工作会让员工感觉到工作和家庭生活之间的丰富性和冲突性,使他们更投入工作,压力更小。
{"title":"High-Performance Work Systems and Well-Being: Mediating Role of Work-to-Family Interface","authors":"A. Babic, F. Stinglhamber, I. Hansez","doi":"10.5334/pb.473","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.473","url":null,"abstract":"Regarding the effects of High-Performance Work Systems (HPWS), we can draw two conclusions. First, existing studies on the effects of HPWS on employees’ well-being at work are scarce. Second, few studies have considered the relationships between HPWS and work-to-family interface (i.e., work-to-family enrichment, WFE; and work-to-family conflict, WFC). Only one previous study conducted on a Portuguese sample (i.e., Carvalho & Chambel, 2016) has examined the relationships between these concepts in a comprehensive model. Our study aims to replicate one part of Carvalho and Chambel’s model but also to extend previous work. We investigated a model of HPWS-employees’ well-being at work (i.e., job engagement and job strain) relationships by considering work-to-family interface as a mediator. We surveyed 170 employees of a Belgian company. Data were analysed using structural equation modelling and bootstrapping method. WFE partially mediates the relationships between HPWS and job engagement, whereas WFC partially mediates the relationships between HPWS and job strain. Our study, confirming the results of Carvalho and Chambel (2016), highlights the important role of HPWS in the development of employees’ well-being at work. Working in an organization where HPWS are applied leads employees to perceive more enrichment and less conflict between their work and family lives, making them more engaged in and less stressed by their work.","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"301 - 320"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-08-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41791155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 12
Recognition Times for 54 Thousand Dutch Words: Data from the Dutch Crowdsourcing Project. 54000个荷兰语单词的识别次数:来自荷兰众包项目的数据。
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q1 PSYCHOLOGY, MULTIDISCIPLINARY Pub Date : 2019-07-17 DOI: 10.5334/pb.491
Marc Brysbaert, Emmanuel Keuleers, Paweł Mandera

We present a new database of Dutch word recognition times for a total of 54 thousand words, called the Dutch Crowdsourcing Project. The data were collected with an internet vocabulary test. The database is limited to native Dutch speakers. Participants were asked to indicate which words they knew. Their response times were registered, even though the participants were not asked to respond as fast as possible. Still, the response times correlate around .7 with the response times of the Dutch Lexicon Projects for shared words. Also results of virtual experiments indicate that the new response times are a valid addition to the Dutch Lexicon Projects. This not only means that we have useful response times for some 20 thousand extra words, but we now also have data on differences in response latencies as a function of education and age. The new data correspond better to word use in the Netherlands.

我们提出了一个新的荷兰单词识别时间数据库,共有54000个单词,名为荷兰众包项目。这些数据是通过网络词汇测试收集的。该数据库仅限于以荷兰语为母语的人。参与者被要求指出他们知道哪些单词。尽管参与者没有被要求尽可能快地做出反应,但他们的反应时间还是被记录下来了。尽管如此,响应时间与荷兰词典项目对共享单词的响应时间约为0.7。虚拟实验的结果也表明,新的响应时间是对荷兰词典项目的有效补充。这不仅意味着我们有大约2万个额外单词的有用响应时间,而且我们现在也有关于响应延迟随教育程度和年龄变化的数据。新数据更符合荷兰的用词情况。
{"title":"Recognition Times for 54 Thousand Dutch Words: Data from the Dutch Crowdsourcing Project.","authors":"Marc Brysbaert,&nbsp;Emmanuel Keuleers,&nbsp;Paweł Mandera","doi":"10.5334/pb.491","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.5334/pb.491","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present a new database of Dutch word recognition times for a total of 54 thousand words, called the Dutch Crowdsourcing Project. The data were collected with an internet vocabulary test. The database is limited to native Dutch speakers. Participants were asked to indicate which words they knew. Their response times were registered, even though the participants were not asked to respond as fast as possible. Still, the response times correlate around .7 with the response times of the Dutch Lexicon Projects for shared words. Also results of virtual experiments indicate that the new response times are a valid addition to the Dutch Lexicon Projects. This not only means that we have useful response times for some 20 thousand extra words, but we now also have data on differences in response latencies as a function of education and age. The new data correspond better to word use in the Netherlands.</p>","PeriodicalId":46662,"journal":{"name":"Psychologica Belgica","volume":"59 1","pages":"281-300"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2019-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6659767/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41215810","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
期刊
Psychologica Belgica
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1