首页 > 最新文献

Dyslexia最新文献

英文 中文
Parafoveal processing and transposed-letter effects in dyslexic reading 失读症阅读中的旁中央凹加工和转位字母效应
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-07-11 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1721
Julie A. Kirkby, Rhiannon S. Barrington, Denis Drieghe, Simon P. Liversedge

During parafoveal processing, skilled readers encode letter identity independently of letter position (Johnson et al., 2007). In the current experiment, we examined orthographic parafoveal processing in readers with dyslexia. Specifically, the eye movements of skilled readers and adult readers with dyslexia were recorded during a boundary paradigm experiment (Rayner, 1975). Parafoveal previews were either identical to the target word (e.g., nearly), a transposed-letter preview (e.g., enarly), or a substituted-letter preview (e.g., acarly). Dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers demonstrated orthographic parafoveal preview benefits during silent sentence reading and both reading groups encoded letter identity and letter position information parafoveally. However, dyslexic adults showed, that very early in lexical processing, during parafoveal preview, the positional information of a word's initial letters were encoded less flexibly compared to during skilled adult reading. We suggest that dyslexic readers are less able to benefit from correct letter identity information (i.e., in the letter transposition previews) due to the lack of direct mapping of orthography to phonology. The current findings demonstrate that dyslexic readers show consistent and dyslexic-specific reading difficulties in foveal and parafoveal processing during silent sentence reading.

在旁中央凹加工过程中,熟练的阅读者对字母身份的编码与字母位置无关(Johnson et al., 2007)。在当前的实验中,我们研究了诵读困难的读者的正字法中央凹旁加工。具体来说,在边界范式实验中记录了熟练读者和患有阅读障碍的成年读者的眼球运动(Rayner, 1975)。旁中央预览要么与目标单词相同(例如,nearly),要么是调换字母的预览(例如,enarly),要么是替换字母的预览(例如,acarly)。阅读困难者和非阅读困难者在默读句子时均表现出正字法旁中央凹预览的好处,两组阅读者都表现出对字母身份和字母位置信息的旁中央凹编码。然而,有阅读障碍的成年人表明,在词汇加工的早期,在中央凹旁预览期间,单词首字母的位置信息编码比熟练成人阅读时更不灵活。我们认为,由于缺乏对正字法和音韵学的直接映射,阅读困难的读者不太能够从正确的字母身份信息中获益(即,在字母换位预览中)。目前的研究结果表明,诵读困难的读者在默读句子时,在中央凹和旁中央凹加工中表现出一致的和特定的诵读困难。
{"title":"Parafoveal processing and transposed-letter effects in dyslexic reading","authors":"Julie A. Kirkby,&nbsp;Rhiannon S. Barrington,&nbsp;Denis Drieghe,&nbsp;Simon P. Liversedge","doi":"10.1002/dys.1721","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1721","url":null,"abstract":"<p>During parafoveal processing, skilled readers encode letter identity independently of letter position (Johnson et al., 2007). In the current experiment, we examined orthographic parafoveal processing in readers with dyslexia. Specifically, the eye movements of skilled readers and adult readers with dyslexia were recorded during a boundary paradigm experiment (Rayner, 1975). Parafoveal previews were either identical to the target word (e.g., <i>nearly</i>), a transposed-letter preview (e.g., <i>enarly</i>), or a substituted-letter preview (e.g., <i>acarly</i>). Dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers demonstrated orthographic parafoveal preview benefits during silent sentence reading and both reading groups encoded letter identity and letter position information parafoveally. However, dyslexic adults showed, that very early in lexical processing, during parafoveal preview, the positional information of a word's initial letters were encoded less flexibly compared to during skilled adult reading. We suggest that dyslexic readers are less able to benefit from correct letter identity information (i.e., in the letter transposition previews) due to the lack of direct mapping of orthography to phonology. The current findings demonstrate that dyslexic readers show consistent and dyslexic-specific reading difficulties in foveal and parafoveal processing during silent sentence reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"359-374"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9545248/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40584328","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 5
Lower prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its impact on Chinese reading 汉语阅读障碍儿童韵律敏感性低下及其对汉语阅读的影响
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-07-05 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1720
Li-Chih Wang, Duo Liu, Lorinda Li-Ying Kwan-Chen, Kevin Kien-Hoa Chung, Ji-Kang Chen

The current study aims to examine prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its relation to Chinese reading in children with and without dyslexia. A total of 172 Chinese children from third grade to sixth grade in Taiwanese primary schools were recruited. Thirty (14 male) children were identified as having dyslexia, and the remaining children (N = 142; 67 male) were typically developing children matched with those with dyslexia as carefully as possible with respect to school, grade, and gender. Our results indicated that group differences were found for all three types of prosodic sensitivity. Moderation analyses showed that group had no significant interaction with prosodic sensitivity in predicting Chinese reading, so the participants in the two groups were combined in the following analyses. The results of the stepwise regression analyses showed that only lexical tone awareness could significantly predict Chinese character reading after controlling for phonological awareness, while only intonation awareness could significantly predict reading comprehension after controlling for Chinese character reading. The results provide preliminary evidence on the issue of prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its role in Chinese reading, which might provide a novel approach to the teaching of Chinese languages.

本研究旨在探讨汉语阅读障碍儿童的韵律敏感性及其与汉语阅读障碍儿童的关系。共有172名台湾小学三年级到六年级的中国学生被招募。30名(14名男性)儿童被确定为患有阅读障碍,其余儿童(N = 142;(67名男性)是典型的发育儿童,并尽可能仔细地根据学校、年级和性别将其与患有阅读障碍的儿童配对。我们的结果表明,这三种类型的韵律敏感性都存在组间差异。适度分析显示,在预测汉语阅读时,两组受试者的韵律敏感性没有显著的交互作用,因此我们将两组受试者合并进行分析。逐步回归分析结果显示,在控制语音意识后,只有词汇音调意识能显著预测汉字阅读,而在控制汉字阅读后,只有语调意识能显著预测阅读理解。本研究结果为汉语阅读障碍儿童的韵律敏感性问题及其在汉语阅读中的作用提供了初步证据,为汉语教学提供了新的思路。
{"title":"Lower prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its impact on Chinese reading","authors":"Li-Chih Wang,&nbsp;Duo Liu,&nbsp;Lorinda Li-Ying Kwan-Chen,&nbsp;Kevin Kien-Hoa Chung,&nbsp;Ji-Kang Chen","doi":"10.1002/dys.1720","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1720","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study aims to examine prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its relation to Chinese reading in children with and without dyslexia. A total of 172 Chinese children from third grade to sixth grade in Taiwanese primary schools were recruited. Thirty (14 male) children were identified as having dyslexia, and the remaining children (<i>N</i> = 142; 67 male) were typically developing children matched with those with dyslexia as carefully as possible with respect to school, grade, and gender. Our results indicated that group differences were found for all three types of prosodic sensitivity. Moderation analyses showed that group had no significant interaction with prosodic sensitivity in predicting Chinese reading, so the participants in the two groups were combined in the following analyses. The results of the stepwise regression analyses showed that only lexical tone awareness could significantly predict Chinese character reading after controlling for phonological awareness, while only intonation awareness could significantly predict reading comprehension after controlling for Chinese character reading. The results provide preliminary evidence on the issue of prosodic sensitivity in Chinese children with dyslexia and its role in Chinese reading, which might provide a novel approach to the teaching of Chinese languages.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"342-358"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-07-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"40563609","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The identification and classification of struggling readers based on the simple view of reading 基于简单阅读观的困难读者识别与分类
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-06-29 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1719
Mike Sleeman, John Everatt, Alison Arrow, Amanda Denston

The simple view of reading (SVR) predicts that reading difficulties can result from decoding difficulties, language comprehension difficulties, or a combination of these difficulties. However, classification studies have identified a fourth group of children whose reading difficulties are unexplained by the model. This may be due to the type of classification model used. The current research included 209 children in Grades 3–5 (8–10 years of age) from New Zealand. Children were classified using the traditional approach and a cluster analysis. In contrast to the traditional classification model, the cluster analysis approach eliminated the unexplained reading difficulties group, suggesting that poor readers can be accurately assigned to one of three groups, which are consistent with those predicted by the SVR. The second set of analyses compared the three poor reader groups across 14 measures of reading comprehension, decoding, language comprehension, phonological awareness, and rapid naming. All three groups demonstrated reading comprehension difficulties, but the dyslexia group showed particular weaknesses in word processing and phonological areas, the SCD group showed problems deriving meaning from oral language, and the mixed group showed general deficits in most measures. The findings suggest that the SVR does have the potential to determine reading profiles and differential intervention methods.

简单阅读观(SVR)预测,阅读困难可能是由解码困难、语言理解困难或这些困难的组合造成的。然而,分类研究已经确定了第四组儿童,他们的阅读困难是该模型无法解释的。这可能是由于所使用的分类模型的类型。目前的研究包括来自新西兰的209名3-5年级(8-10岁)的儿童。采用传统方法和聚类分析对儿童进行分类。与传统的分类模型相比,聚类分析方法消除了无法解释的阅读困难组,表明可以将不良读者准确地划分为三组中的一组,这与SVR预测的结果一致。第二组分析比较了三组阅读能力差的学生在阅读理解、解码、语言理解、语音意识和快速命名等14项指标上的表现。所有三组都表现出阅读理解困难,但失读症组在文字处理和语音方面表现出特别的弱点,SCD组表现出从口语中获取意义的问题,而混合组在大多数方面都表现出普遍的缺陷。研究结果表明,SVR确实具有确定阅读概况和差异干预方法的潜力。
{"title":"The identification and classification of struggling readers based on the simple view of reading","authors":"Mike Sleeman,&nbsp;John Everatt,&nbsp;Alison Arrow,&nbsp;Amanda Denston","doi":"10.1002/dys.1719","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1719","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The simple view of reading (SVR) predicts that reading difficulties can result from decoding difficulties, language comprehension difficulties, or a combination of these difficulties. However, classification studies have identified a fourth group of children whose reading difficulties are unexplained by the model. This may be due to the type of classification model used. The current research included 209 children in Grades 3–5 (8–10 years of age) from New Zealand. Children were classified using the traditional approach and a cluster analysis. In contrast to the traditional classification model, the cluster analysis approach eliminated the unexplained reading difficulties group, suggesting that poor readers can be accurately assigned to one of three groups, which are consistent with those predicted by the SVR. The second set of analyses compared the three poor reader groups across 14 measures of reading comprehension, decoding, language comprehension, phonological awareness, and rapid naming. All three groups demonstrated reading comprehension difficulties, but the dyslexia group showed particular weaknesses in word processing and phonological areas, the SCD group showed problems deriving meaning from oral language, and the mixed group showed general deficits in most measures. The findings suggest that the SVR does have the potential to determine reading profiles and differential intervention methods.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"256-275"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-06-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/b0/97/DYS-28-256.PMC9542070.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10275978","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 3
Removing the academic framing in student evaluations improves achievement in children with dyslexia: The mediating role of self-judgement of competence 去除学生评价中的学术框架可以提高阅读障碍儿童的成绩:能力自我判断的中介作用
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-27 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1713
Odile Rohmer, Nadège Doignon-Camus, Jean Audusseau, Séléna Trautmann, Anne-Clémence Chaillou, Maria Popa-Roch

Self-judgement is known to play a crucial role in academic achievement, and as such, may be expected to have an impact on students with dyslexia. Their self-judgements may reflect the negative stereotype of low competence that targets people with disabilities. Their repeated academic failures may lead to a negative association between “school” and “failure”. The aim of the present study was to investigate how such factors contribute to academic failure in students with dyslexia. Participants were 183 French middle school students. We assessed students' self-judgement and manipulated the framing of performance tasks so that students completed literacy tasks in both academic and non-academic forms. We expected a detrimental impact of dyslexia on performance in academic but not in non-academic tasks. We also expected self-judgement to account for this difference. Students with dyslexia perceive themselves as less competent than students without dyslexia. Significantly, structural equation modeling revealed that students with dyslexia performed poorly in academic tasks, compared to students without dyslexia. This difference no longer appeared in non-academic tasks. Self-judgement of competence is a predictor of the performance of students with and without dyslexia at school and their impact is related to how the academic features of the tasks are emphasized.

众所周知,自我判断在学业成绩中起着至关重要的作用,因此,可能会对患有阅读障碍的学生产生影响。他们的自我判断可能反映了针对残疾人的低能力的负面刻板印象。他们反复的学业失败可能会导致“学校”和“失败”之间的负面联系。本研究的目的是调查这些因素是如何导致有阅读障碍的学生学业失败的。参与者是183名法国中学生。我们评估了学生的自我判断,并操纵了任务的框架,以便学生完成学术和非学术形式的读写任务。我们预期阅读障碍对学业表现有不利影响,但对非学业任务没有影响。我们还期望自我判断能够解释这种差异。有阅读障碍的学生认为自己的能力不如没有阅读障碍的学生。值得注意的是,结构方程模型显示,与没有阅读障碍的学生相比,有阅读障碍的学生在学业任务中的表现较差。这种差异在非学术任务中不再出现。能力的自我判断是有阅读障碍和没有阅读障碍的学生在学校表现的一个预测指标,其影响与任务的学术特征如何被强调有关。
{"title":"Removing the academic framing in student evaluations improves achievement in children with dyslexia: The mediating role of self-judgement of competence","authors":"Odile Rohmer,&nbsp;Nadège Doignon-Camus,&nbsp;Jean Audusseau,&nbsp;Séléna Trautmann,&nbsp;Anne-Clémence Chaillou,&nbsp;Maria Popa-Roch","doi":"10.1002/dys.1713","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1713","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Self-judgement is known to play a crucial role in academic achievement, and as such, may be expected to have an impact on students with dyslexia. Their self-judgements may reflect the negative stereotype of low competence that targets people with disabilities. Their repeated academic failures may lead to a negative association between “school” and “failure”. The aim of the present study was to investigate how such factors contribute to academic failure in students with dyslexia. Participants were 183 French middle school students. We assessed students' self-judgement and manipulated the framing of performance tasks so that students completed literacy tasks in both academic and non-academic forms. We expected a detrimental impact of dyslexia on performance in academic but not in non-academic tasks. We also expected self-judgement to account for this difference. Students with dyslexia perceive themselves as less competent than students without dyslexia. Significantly, structural equation modeling revealed that students with dyslexia performed poorly in academic tasks, compared to students without dyslexia. This difference no longer appeared in non-academic tasks. Self-judgement of competence is a predictor of the performance of students with and without dyslexia at school and their impact is related to how the academic features of the tasks are emphasized.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"309-324"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/dys.1713","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"83303681","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 4
A stitch in time…: Comparing late-identified, late-emerging and early-identified dyslexia 一针及时…:比较晚发现,晚出现和早发现的阅读障碍
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-18 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1712
Elise H. de Bree, Madelon van den Boer, Boukje M. Toering, Peter F. de Jong

When dyslexia is diagnosed late, the question is whether this is due to late-emerging (LE) or late-identified (LI) problems. In a random selection of dyslexia-diagnosis case files we distinguished early-diagnosed (Grade 1–3, n = 116) and late-diagnosed (Grade 4–6) dyslexia. The late-diagnosed files were divided into LE (n = 54) and LI dyslexia (n = 45). The LE group consisted of children whose national-curriculum literacy outcomes did not warrant referral for dyslexia diagnosis in Grades 1–2; the LI group of children whose literacy outcomes did, but who were referred for diagnostic assessment after Grade 3. At the time of diagnosis, the percentage of poor performers on word-level literacy measures generally did not differ between the groups. Only the LE group contained fewer poor performers than the early-diagnosed and LI group on some word-reading measures. All groups showed similar distributions of phonological difficulties. There were no indications of compensation through vocabulary, memory or IQ in either late-diagnosed group. Our diagnosis-based study confirms and extends previous research-based studies on LE dyslexia. Moreover, it shows that LI dyslexia exists, which can be regarded as the existence of instructional casualties. The findings speak to issues of identification, diagnosis and compensation and call for further efforts to improve the early identification of dyslexia.

当阅读障碍诊断较晚时,问题是这是由于晚出现(LE)还是晚发现(LI)的问题。在随机选择的阅读障碍诊断病例档案中,我们区分了早期诊断(1-3级,n = 116)和晚期诊断(4-6级)的阅读障碍。晚期诊断文件分为LE (n = 54)和LI阅读障碍(n = 45)。LE组由国家课程扫盲结果不支持1-2年级阅读障碍诊断的儿童组成;LI组的孩子,他们的读写能力有所提高,但他们在三年级后被转介进行诊断性评估。在诊断时,在单词水平读写能力测试中表现不佳的百分比在两组之间通常没有差异。只有低水平组比早期诊断组和低水平组在一些单词阅读测试中表现不佳。所有组都表现出相似的语音困难分布。在任何一个晚期诊断组中,没有迹象表明在词汇、记忆或智商方面有补偿。我们以诊断为基础的研究证实并扩展了先前对LE阅读障碍的研究。此外,该研究还表明了LI阅读障碍的存在,这可以看作是教学伤亡的存在。研究结果涉及识别、诊断和补偿问题,并呼吁进一步努力提高对阅读障碍的早期识别。
{"title":"A stitch in time…: Comparing late-identified, late-emerging and early-identified dyslexia","authors":"Elise H. de Bree,&nbsp;Madelon van den Boer,&nbsp;Boukje M. Toering,&nbsp;Peter F. de Jong","doi":"10.1002/dys.1712","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1712","url":null,"abstract":"<p>When dyslexia is diagnosed late, the question is whether this is due to late-<i>emerging</i> (LE) or late-<i>identified</i> (LI) problems. In a random selection of dyslexia-diagnosis case files we distinguished early-diagnosed (Grade 1–3, <i>n</i> = 116) and late-diagnosed (Grade 4–6) dyslexia. The late-diagnosed files were divided into LE (<i>n</i> = 54) and LI dyslexia (<i>n</i> = 45). The LE group consisted of children whose national-curriculum literacy outcomes did not warrant referral for dyslexia diagnosis in Grades 1–2; the LI group of children whose literacy outcomes did, but who were referred for diagnostic assessment after Grade 3. At the time of diagnosis, the percentage of poor performers on word-level literacy measures generally did not differ between the groups. Only the LE group contained fewer poor performers than the early-diagnosed and LI group on some word-reading measures. All groups showed similar distributions of phonological difficulties. There were no indications of compensation through vocabulary, memory or IQ in either late-diagnosed group. Our diagnosis-based study confirms and extends previous research-based studies on LE dyslexia. Moreover, it shows that LI dyslexia exists, which can be regarded as the existence of instructional casualties. The findings speak to issues of identification, diagnosis and compensation and call for further efforts to improve the early identification of dyslexia.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"276-292"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1002/dys.1712","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"89927427","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Executive functions in Chinese kindergarten children with early reading problems 中文幼儿园儿童早期阅读问题的执行功能
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-18 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1714
Cathy Yui-Chi Fong, Connie Suk-Han Ho

Learning to read Chinese is a complex task that draws on a range of executive function (EF) skills since early development. However, no studies have examined EF as a potential contributing factor to early reading problems among Chinese children. The present longitudinal study identified 48 poor readers and 48 normal readers among a sample of 190 Chinese children at the end of kindergarten. Measures of EF skills (working memory, inhibition control, and cognitive flexibility) and reading outcomes (word reading, sentence reading fluency, and sentence reading comprehension) were administered to the children. The two groups were retrospectively compared on the EF measures after age and nonverbal IQ were considered. Poor readers were found to perform significantly worse than normal readers in all the examined EF skills. Correlation and regression results revealed a relatively different nature of the relationship between EF and reading in poor readers as compared with normal readers. Inhibition control predicted reading outcomes in poor readers only, while cognitive flexibility predicted reading outcomes in normal readers only. Working memory was significantly correlated to word reading in poor readers and to reading comprehension in normal readers. The results are discussed in terms of the special characteristics of the Chinese language.

学习阅读中文是一项复杂的任务,从早期发展开始就需要一系列执行功能(EF)技能。然而,目前还没有研究表明EF是中国儿童早期阅读问题的潜在影响因素。本研究以190名幼儿园毕业的中国儿童为样本,选取了48名阅读能力差的儿童和48名阅读能力正常的儿童。对儿童进行EF技能(工作记忆、抑制控制和认知灵活性)和阅读结果(单词阅读、句子阅读流畅性和句子阅读理解)的测量。回顾性比较两组在年龄和非语言智商后的EF测量结果。结果发现,阅读能力差的人在所有测试的英语阅读技能上的表现都明显低于正常读者。相关和回归结果显示,与正常读者相比,不良读者的英语阅读与阅读之间的关系具有相对不同的性质。抑制控制仅预测阅读能力差的读者的阅读结果,而认知灵活性仅预测正常读者的阅读结果。工作记忆对阅读能力差的读者的单词阅读和正常读者的阅读理解有显著的影响。本文从汉语的特点出发,对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Executive functions in Chinese kindergarten children with early reading problems","authors":"Cathy Yui-Chi Fong,&nbsp;Connie Suk-Han Ho","doi":"10.1002/dys.1714","DOIUrl":"10.1002/dys.1714","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Learning to read Chinese is a complex task that draws on a range of executive function (EF) skills since early development. However, no studies have examined EF as a potential contributing factor to early reading problems among Chinese children. The present longitudinal study identified 48 poor readers and 48 normal readers among a sample of 190 Chinese children at the end of kindergarten. Measures of EF skills (working memory, inhibition control, and cognitive flexibility) and reading outcomes (word reading, sentence reading fluency, and sentence reading comprehension) were administered to the children. The two groups were retrospectively compared on the EF measures after age and nonverbal IQ were considered. Poor readers were found to perform significantly worse than normal readers in all the examined EF skills. Correlation and regression results revealed a relatively different nature of the relationship between EF and reading in poor readers as compared with normal readers. Inhibition control predicted reading outcomes in poor readers only, while cognitive flexibility predicted reading outcomes in normal readers only. Working memory was significantly correlated to word reading in poor readers and to reading comprehension in normal readers. The results are discussed in terms of the special characteristics of the Chinese language.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 3","pages":"325-341"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"48875235","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Saudi public primary school teachers' knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia. 沙特公立小学教师对发展性阅读障碍的认识与信念。
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2021-12-07 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1705
Mohaned G Abed, Todd K Shackelford

This exploratory research investigates knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia (DD) among public primary school teachers in Saudi Arabia. We explored links between several teacher-related socio-demographic variables (e.g., gender, teaching experience, self-evaluation of teaching children with DD) and knowledge and beliefs about DD. Saudi public primary school teachers (n = 136) completed an online survey that included the knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia scale (KBDDS). The results indicated that KBDDS scores were significantly associated with teaching experience, DD training, and self-evaluation of teaching children with DD. We address limitations of the current research, note directions for future research, and discuss implications of these results for teacher training and professional development, with special attention to Saudi public primary education.

本研究旨在探讨沙国公立小学教师对发展性阅读障碍(DD)的认知与信念。我们探讨了几个与教师相关的社会人口变量(如性别、教学经验、DD患儿教学的自我评价)与DD知识和信念之间的联系。沙特公立小学教师(n = 136)完成了一项在线调查,其中包括关于发展性阅读障碍量表(KBDDS)的知识和信念。结果表明,KBDDS分数与教学经验、DD培训和DD教学儿童的自我评价显著相关。我们指出了当前研究的局限性,指出了未来研究的方向,并讨论了这些结果对教师培训和专业发展的影响,特别关注沙特的公立小学教育。
{"title":"Saudi public primary school teachers' knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia.","authors":"Mohaned G Abed,&nbsp;Todd K Shackelford","doi":"10.1002/dys.1705","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1705","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This exploratory research investigates knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia (DD) among public primary school teachers in Saudi Arabia. We explored links between several teacher-related socio-demographic variables (e.g., gender, teaching experience, self-evaluation of teaching children with DD) and knowledge and beliefs about DD. Saudi public primary school teachers (n = 136) completed an online survey that included the knowledge and beliefs about developmental dyslexia scale (KBDDS). The results indicated that KBDDS scores were significantly associated with teaching experience, DD training, and self-evaluation of teaching children with DD. We address limitations of the current research, note directions for future research, and discuss implications of these results for teacher training and professional development, with special attention to Saudi public primary education.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 2","pages":"244-251"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39701362","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
Neuropsychological intervention of dyslexia has a positive effect on aspects of psychological well-being in young adults - a randomized controlled study. 一项随机对照研究表明,阅读障碍的神经心理干预对年轻人的心理健康有积极的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2021-09-27 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1697
Johanna M Nukari, Marja R Laasonen, Eva P Arkkila, Marja-Leena Haapanen, Jari O Lipsanen, Erja T Poutiainen

Effectiveness of individual- and group-based neuropsychological intervention on aspects of psychological well-being of dyslexic adults was evaluated. Dyslexic young adults (n = 120) were randomly assigned into individual intervention, group intervention or wait-list control group. Both interventions focussed on cognitive strategy learning, supporting self-esteem, and using psychoeducation. In group format peer support was also utilized. Cognitive and behavioural strategies, mood states, quality of life and self-esteem were assessed via self-report questionnaires at baseline, after the intervention/wait-list control time at 5 months and 10 months. Results indicated that the neuropsychological interventions had a positive effect on self-evaluated cognitive and behavioural strategies, especially in increasing success expectations and to a lesser degree in diminishing task-avoidance and in group intervention in diminishing social pessimism. The interventions also improved cognition-related quality of life and, to a lesser degree, self-esteem. These results indicate that structured neuropsychological interventions can positively affect self-evaluated psychological well-being, especially on cognitive and behavioural strategies. Considering the secondary consequences of dyslexia, support among young adults is often needed beyond the cognitive and reading-based challenges dyslexia poses.

评估了个体和群体为基础的神经心理学干预对成人失读症心理健康方面的有效性。120名有阅读障碍的年轻人被随机分为个体干预组、群体干预组和等候名单对照组。两种干预措施都侧重于认知策略学习、支持自尊和使用心理教育。小组形式的同伴支持也得到利用。认知和行为策略、情绪状态、生活质量和自尊在干预后5个月和10个月通过自我报告问卷进行基线评估。结果表明,神经心理学干预对自我评估的认知和行为策略有积极的影响,特别是在提高成功期望方面,在减少任务回避和群体干预方面有较小程度的减少社会悲观主义。干预还改善了与认知相关的生活质量,并在较小程度上改善了自尊。这些结果表明,结构化神经心理学干预可以积极影响自我评估的心理健康,特别是在认知和行为策略方面。考虑到阅读障碍的继发性后果,年轻人往往需要超越阅读障碍带来的认知和阅读挑战的支持。
{"title":"Neuropsychological intervention of dyslexia has a positive effect on aspects of psychological well-being in young adults - a randomized controlled study.","authors":"Johanna M Nukari,&nbsp;Marja R Laasonen,&nbsp;Eva P Arkkila,&nbsp;Marja-Leena Haapanen,&nbsp;Jari O Lipsanen,&nbsp;Erja T Poutiainen","doi":"10.1002/dys.1697","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1697","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Effectiveness of individual- and group-based neuropsychological intervention on aspects of psychological well-being of dyslexic adults was evaluated. Dyslexic young adults (n = 120) were randomly assigned into individual intervention, group intervention or wait-list control group. Both interventions focussed on cognitive strategy learning, supporting self-esteem, and using psychoeducation. In group format peer support was also utilized. Cognitive and behavioural strategies, mood states, quality of life and self-esteem were assessed via self-report questionnaires at baseline, after the intervention/wait-list control time at 5 months and 10 months. Results indicated that the neuropsychological interventions had a positive effect on self-evaluated cognitive and behavioural strategies, especially in increasing success expectations and to a lesser degree in diminishing task-avoidance and in group intervention in diminishing social pessimism. The interventions also improved cognition-related quality of life and, to a lesser degree, self-esteem. These results indicate that structured neuropsychological interventions can positively affect self-evaluated psychological well-being, especially on cognitive and behavioural strategies. Considering the secondary consequences of dyslexia, support among young adults is often needed beyond the cognitive and reading-based challenges dyslexia poses.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 2","pages":"166-184"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39465219","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Children's vocabulary and friendships: A comparative study between children with and without Specific Learning Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. 儿童词汇与友谊:特殊学习障碍和注意缺陷多动障碍儿童与非特殊学习障碍儿童的比较研究。
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-09 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1709
Sofia Kouvava, Katerina Antonopoulou, Asimina M Ralli, Constantinos M Kokkinos, Katerina Maridaki-Kassotaki

Language skills are important in the formation and maintenance of friendships. Children with specific learning disorder (SLD) or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience difficulties with their relationships and have language-related problems. This study aims to examine how expressive and receptive vocabulary may relate to friendships of children with and without SLD or ADHD. Participants were 64 children with SLD, 64 children with ADHD, and 64 typically developing (TD) children, aged 8-12 years (Mage  = 9.77 years, SD = 1.22), attending Grades 3 to 6 in inclusive primary schools of Attica, Greece. The Greek versions of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test and the expressive vocabulary subscale of the WISC-III were administered along with the sociometric nominations of friends and the self-reports of best friendship duration. Results showed that children with SLD and ADHD reported best friendships of shorter duration and had significantly poorer receptive and expressive vocabulary. Children with ADHD had significantly fewer close and best friends than children with SLD, who in turn had significantly fewer close and best friends than the TD children. Children's vocabulary in all three groups was positively correlated with the duration of their best friendships and was found to moderately predict children's close friendships.

语言技能在友谊的形成和维持中很重要。患有特殊学习障碍(SLD)或注意力缺陷/多动障碍(ADHD)的儿童在人际关系方面遇到困难,并有语言相关问题。本研究旨在研究表达性词汇和接受性词汇与有或没有特殊障碍或注意力缺陷多动障碍儿童的友谊之间的关系。参与者为64名SLD儿童,64名ADHD儿童和64名典型发育(TD)儿童,年龄为8-12岁(Mage = 9.77岁,SD = 1.22),在希腊Attica的全纳小学就读3 - 6年级。采用希腊文皮博迪图片词汇测试和WISC-III表达性词汇量表,同时进行朋友社会计量提名和最佳友谊持续时间自我报告。结果显示,患有SLD和ADHD的儿童最好的友谊持续时间较短,接受和表达词汇量明显较差。ADHD儿童的亲密朋友和最好的朋友明显少于自闭症儿童,而自闭症儿童的亲密朋友和最好的朋友又明显少于自闭症儿童。三组儿童的词汇量与他们最好的友谊的持续时间呈正相关,并且可以适度预测孩子的亲密友谊。
{"title":"Children's vocabulary and friendships: A comparative study between children with and without Specific Learning Disorder and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder.","authors":"Sofia Kouvava,&nbsp;Katerina Antonopoulou,&nbsp;Asimina M Ralli,&nbsp;Constantinos M Kokkinos,&nbsp;Katerina Maridaki-Kassotaki","doi":"10.1002/dys.1709","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1709","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Language skills are important in the formation and maintenance of friendships. Children with specific learning disorder (SLD) or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) experience difficulties with their relationships and have language-related problems. This study aims to examine how expressive and receptive vocabulary may relate to friendships of children with and without SLD or ADHD. Participants were 64 children with SLD, 64 children with ADHD, and 64 typically developing (TD) children, aged 8-12 years (M<sub>age</sub>  = 9.77 years, SD = 1.22), attending Grades 3 to 6 in inclusive primary schools of Attica, Greece. The Greek versions of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test and the expressive vocabulary subscale of the WISC-III were administered along with the sociometric nominations of friends and the self-reports of best friendship duration. Results showed that children with SLD and ADHD reported best friendships of shorter duration and had significantly poorer receptive and expressive vocabulary. Children with ADHD had significantly fewer close and best friends than children with SLD, who in turn had significantly fewer close and best friends than the TD children. Children's vocabulary in all three groups was positively correlated with the duration of their best friendships and was found to moderately predict children's close friendships.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 2","pages":"149-165"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39902632","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stroop performance is related to reading profiles in Hebrew-speaking individuals with dyslexia and typical readers. Stroop表现与希伯来语阅读障碍患者和典型读者的阅读概况有关。
IF 2.2 3区 教育学 Q1 EDUCATION, SPECIAL Pub Date : 2022-05-01 Epub Date: 2022-02-07 DOI: 10.1002/dys.1708
Donia Abo-Elhija, Rola Farah, Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus

There is a debate in the literature regarding the level of contribution of executive functions (EF) to reading comprehension (RC), in the context of the simple view of reading (SVR) model. The current study aims to create sub-profiles of reading and cognitive abilities based on a measure traditionally used for evaluating EF, that is, the Stroop task, and specifically, Stroop time. Ninety-seven adults with and without reading difficulties performed reading and cognitive tasks, including the Stroop tests. Four groups were created based on Stroop performance time and a reading profile was created for each group. A mediation analysis was conducted to determine if reading accuracy and linguistic abilities predict RC mediated by Stroop time. Participants with a shorter Stroop time demonstrated better reading abilities, whereas those with longer Stroop time showed decreased reading performance. Stroop time was also negatively associated with better performance in additional cognitive abilities. A mediation analysis suggested that decoding ability and linguistic ability predict RC through EF. Our findings support the SVR model and the involvement of EF in reading proficiency and might be used for designing EF-based interventions for reading and RC difficulties.

在简单阅读观(SVR)模型的背景下,关于执行功能对阅读理解的贡献程度,文献中存在争议。目前的研究旨在基于传统上用于评估EF的测量方法,即Stroop任务,特别是Stroop时间,创建阅读和认知能力的子概况。97名有或没有阅读困难的成年人进行了包括Stroop测试在内的阅读和认知任务。根据Stroop表现时间创建了四组,并为每组创建了阅读档案。阅读准确性和语言能力对Stroop时间介导的RC是否有预测作用。Stroop时间较短的参与者表现出更好的阅读能力,而Stroop时间较长的参与者则表现出阅读能力下降。Stroop时间还与其他认知能力的更好表现呈负相关。译码能力和语言能力通过EF预测语用行为。我们的研究结果支持SVR模型和英语对阅读能力的影响,并可用于设计基于英语的阅读和阅读障碍干预措施。
{"title":"Stroop performance is related to reading profiles in Hebrew-speaking individuals with dyslexia and typical readers.","authors":"Donia Abo-Elhija,&nbsp;Rola Farah,&nbsp;Tzipi Horowitz-Kraus","doi":"10.1002/dys.1708","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1002/dys.1708","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>There is a debate in the literature regarding the level of contribution of executive functions (EF) to reading comprehension (RC), in the context of the simple view of reading (SVR) model. The current study aims to create sub-profiles of reading and cognitive abilities based on a measure traditionally used for evaluating EF, that is, the Stroop task, and specifically, Stroop time. Ninety-seven adults with and without reading difficulties performed reading and cognitive tasks, including the Stroop tests. Four groups were created based on Stroop performance time and a reading profile was created for each group. A mediation analysis was conducted to determine if reading accuracy and linguistic abilities predict RC mediated by Stroop time. Participants with a shorter Stroop time demonstrated better reading abilities, whereas those with longer Stroop time showed decreased reading performance. Stroop time was also negatively associated with better performance in additional cognitive abilities. A mediation analysis suggested that decoding ability and linguistic ability predict RC through EF. Our findings support the SVR model and the involvement of EF in reading proficiency and might be used for designing EF-based interventions for reading and RC difficulties.</p>","PeriodicalId":47222,"journal":{"name":"Dyslexia","volume":"28 2","pages":"212-227"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2022-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"39898803","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"教育学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Dyslexia
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1