首页 > 最新文献

Laterality最新文献

英文 中文
Auditory perceptual ability affects dichotic listening performance in older adults. 听觉感知能力影响老年人的二分法听力表现。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-04 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2420408
Yang Li, Xiaohu Yang

Age-related changes pose challenges in speech processing for older adults. However, little is known about the role of auditory perceptual ability in their performance in dichotic listening tasks. The present study investigated how older adults' auditory perceptual abilities affected their correct rates and the right ear advantage (REA) in the dichotic listening tasks in two experiments. In Experiment 1, older adults' performance was assessed using dichotic listening tasks based on consonant-vowel (CV) words varying in consonants, vowels, and lexical tones, each presenting distinct auditory perceptual demands. It was found that older adults exhibited decreased correct rates as auditory perceptual demands increased. Moreover, differences in the REA were observed in older listeners, suggesting increased engagement of the hemisphere responsible for acoustic analysis in processing challenging dichotic stimuli. Experiment 2 examined how older individuals' acoustic processing abilities contributed to their dichotic listening performance. It was shown that older adults with acoustic processing abilities comparable to those of younger individuals demonstrated correct rates and REAs similar to those of younger cohorts. These results revealed the nonnegligible role of acoustic processing in the dichotic listening paradigm and the significance of considering listeners' auditory perceptual abilities in investigating language lateralisation using the dichotic listening paradigm.

与年龄有关的变化给老年人的语音处理带来了挑战。然而,人们对听觉感知能力在二分法听力任务中的作用知之甚少。本研究通过两个实验,探讨了老年人的听觉感知能力如何影响他们在二分听任务中的正确率和右耳优势(REA)。在实验 1 中,研究人员使用基于辅音-元音(CV)单词的二分法听力任务对老年人的表现进行了评估,这些单词的辅音、元音和词调各不相同,每个单词都提出了不同的听觉感知要求。研究发现,随着听觉知觉要求的提高,老年人的正确率也在下降。此外,在老年听者身上还观察到了 REA 的差异,这表明在处理具有挑战性的二分刺激时,负责声学分析的半球的参与程度有所提高。实验 2 考察了老年人的声学处理能力如何影响他们的二分法听力表现。结果表明,声学处理能力与年轻人相当的老年人表现出了与年轻人相似的正确率和REA。这些结果揭示了声学处理在二分听范式中不可忽视的作用,以及在使用二分听范式研究语言侧化时考虑听者听觉知觉能力的重要性。
{"title":"Auditory perceptual ability affects dichotic listening performance in older adults.","authors":"Yang Li, Xiaohu Yang","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2420408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2024.2420408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Age-related changes pose challenges in speech processing for older adults. However, little is known about the role of auditory perceptual ability in their performance in dichotic listening tasks. The present study investigated how older adults' auditory perceptual abilities affected their correct rates and the right ear advantage (REA) in the dichotic listening tasks in two experiments. In Experiment 1, older adults' performance was assessed using dichotic listening tasks based on consonant-vowel (CV) words varying in consonants, vowels, and lexical tones, each presenting distinct auditory perceptual demands. It was found that older adults exhibited decreased correct rates as auditory perceptual demands increased. Moreover, differences in the REA were observed in older listeners, suggesting increased engagement of the hemisphere responsible for acoustic analysis in processing challenging dichotic stimuli. Experiment 2 examined how older individuals' acoustic processing abilities contributed to their dichotic listening performance. It was shown that older adults with acoustic processing abilities comparable to those of younger individuals demonstrated correct rates and REAs similar to those of younger cohorts. These results revealed the nonnegligible role of acoustic processing in the dichotic listening paradigm and the significance of considering listeners' auditory perceptual abilities in investigating language lateralisation using the dichotic listening paradigm.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"1-33"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-11-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142577074","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of ocular dominance on choroidal structures. 眼球优势对脉络膜结构的影响。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-23 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2416388
Zeynep Özer Özcan, Sevim Ayça Seyyar, Kıvanç Güngör

This study aimed to compare the dominant and non-dominant eyes of healthy individuals in terms of CT and CVI values to assess the effect of ocular dominance on choroidal morphology. Three hundred eyes of 150 individuals were included in this study. All patients underwent routine ophthalmological examinations and hole-in-the-card tests to determine the dominant eye. EDI-OCT was used to obtain choroidal images. Choroidal thickness (CT) was measured at three points on the choroid including the subfoveal region (subfoveal choroidal thickness (SFCT)), 1500 μm nasal (NCT) and temporal (TCT) to the fovea. Choroidal vascularity index (CVI), total choroidal area (TCA), luminal area (LA), and stromal area (SA) were measured at fovea-centered choroidal area by image binarization via Image J software. While 93(%62) participants were right-eyed, 57(%38) participants were left-eyed. We could not detect any difference in SFCT, NCT, TCT, TCA, LA, SA, and CVI measurements between the dominant and the non-dominant eyes. (p > 0.05 for all). We could not detect any effect of ocular dominance on choroidal structures. These results also support the use of randomized ocular data regardless of which eye is dominant when investigating CVI and CT in scientific research.

本研究旨在比较健康人的优势眼和非优势眼的 CT 和 CVI 值,以评估眼球优势对脉络膜形态的影响。本研究共纳入了 150 名患者的 300 只眼睛。所有患者都接受了常规眼科检查和眼底检查,以确定优势眼。使用 EDI-OCT 获得脉络膜图像。脉络膜厚度(CT)在脉络膜上的三个点进行测量,包括眼窝下区域(眼窝下脉络膜厚度(SFCT))、距离眼窝1500微米的鼻侧(NCT)和颞侧(TCT)。脉络膜血管指数(CVI)、脉络膜总面积(TCA)、管腔面积(LA)和基质面积(SA)是在以眼窝为中心的脉络膜区域通过图像二值化软件进行测量的。93(62%)名参与者为右眼,57(38%)名参与者为左眼。我们没有发现优势眼与非优势眼在 SFCT、NCT、TCT、TCA、LA、SA 和 CVI 测量值上有任何差异。(所有数据的 p 均大于 0.05)。我们没有发现眼球优势对脉络膜结构有任何影响。这些结果也支持在科学研究中调查 CVI 和 CT 时使用随机眼部数据,而不管哪只眼睛是优势眼。
{"title":"The effect of ocular dominance on choroidal structures.","authors":"Zeynep Özer Özcan, Sevim Ayça Seyyar, Kıvanç Güngör","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2416388","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2024.2416388","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study aimed to compare the dominant and non-dominant eyes of healthy individuals in terms of CT and CVI values to assess the effect of ocular dominance on choroidal morphology. Three hundred eyes of 150 individuals were included in this study. All patients underwent routine ophthalmological examinations and hole-in-the-card tests to determine the dominant eye. EDI-OCT was used to obtain choroidal images. Choroidal thickness (CT) was measured at three points on the choroid including the subfoveal region (subfoveal choroidal thickness (SFCT)), 1500 μm nasal (NCT) and temporal (TCT) to the fovea. Choroidal vascularity index (CVI), total choroidal area (TCA), luminal area (LA), and stromal area (SA) were measured at fovea-centered choroidal area by image binarization via Image J software. While 93(%62) participants were right-eyed, 57(%38) participants were left-eyed. We could not detect any difference in SFCT, NCT, TCT, TCA, LA, SA, and CVI measurements between the dominant and the non-dominant eyes. (<i>p </i>> 0.05 for all). We could not detect any effect of ocular dominance on choroidal structures. These results also support the use of randomized ocular data regardless of which eye is dominant when investigating CVI and CT in scientific research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"1-13"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-10-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142510298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Artistic turns: laterality in paintings of kisses and embraces 艺术转折:亲吻和拥抱画作中的侧面性
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-10 DOI: 10.1080/1357650x.2024.2399352
Gianluca Malatesta, Giulia Prete, Anita D’Anselmo, Chiara Lucafò, Luca Tommasi
Many lateral biases exist in human behavior, often implicit and not deliberated. Romantic kissing and embracing received experimental attention in the last three decades. We investigated laterality...
人类行为中存在许多横向偏差,这些偏差往往是隐性的,没有经过深思熟虑。在过去的三十年里,浪漫的接吻和拥抱受到了实验的关注。我们研究了侧向性...
{"title":"Artistic turns: laterality in paintings of kisses and embraces","authors":"Gianluca Malatesta, Giulia Prete, Anita D’Anselmo, Chiara Lucafò, Luca Tommasi","doi":"10.1080/1357650x.2024.2399352","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650x.2024.2399352","url":null,"abstract":"Many lateral biases exist in human behavior, often implicit and not deliberated. Romantic kissing and embracing received experimental attention in the last three decades. We investigated laterality...","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":"65 1","pages":"1-20"},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2024-09-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142217005","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A task-dependent analysis of closed vs. open and fine vs. gross motor skills in handedness. 对封闭式与开放式、精细运动技能与粗大运动技能在手部灵活性方面的任务依赖性分析。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-18 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2391793
Alexandre J Marcori, Matheus G Gamberini, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Pedro H M Monteiro, Victor H A Okazaki

The traditional classifications of motor skills nature (open vs closed; fine vs gross) have not been considered in handedness investigations. Instead, previous research focused on comparing complex vs less complex motor behaviour, leaving a gap in the literature. We compared manual preference between different motor skill characteristics, namely: fine and closed (FC), gross and closed (GC) and gross and open (GO) tasks. The hand preference was assessed with the Global Lateral Preference Inventory in four hundred and forty participants (244 women) aged from 18 to 59 years old. By assessing the degree and direction of handedness in different motor skills, our results showed a stronger lateralization pattern for FC motor skills as compared to GC and GO, with GO also being less lateralized than GC. Our results expand those of previous investigations that used the motor skill complexity definitions by showing how handedness can also be modulated by the interaction between classic motor skills classifications. Future research should consider fine vs. gross and open vs. closed classifications when selecting tasks for analysis of asymmetries of preference.

传统的运动技能性质分类(开放式与封闭式;精细与粗略)在手性调查中并未被考虑。相反,以往的研究侧重于比较复杂与不太复杂的运动行为,这在文献中留下了空白。我们比较了不同运动技能特征之间的手部偏好,即:精细和封闭(FC)、粗大和封闭(GC)以及粗大和开放(GO)任务。我们使用全球侧向偏好量表对 44 名参与者(244 名女性)的手部偏好进行了评估,这些参与者的年龄在 18 至 59 岁之间。通过评估不同运动技能中手的偏向程度和方向,我们的结果显示,FC 运动技能的偏侧模式强于 GC 和 GO,GO 的偏侧程度也低于 GC。我们的研究结果拓展了之前使用运动技能复杂性定义的研究,显示了手性也可以通过经典运动技能分类之间的相互作用来调节。未来的研究在选择任务进行偏好不对称分析时,应考虑精细与粗略、开放与封闭的分类。
{"title":"A task-dependent analysis of closed vs. open and fine vs. gross motor skills in handedness.","authors":"Alexandre J Marcori, Matheus G Gamberini, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Pedro H M Monteiro, Victor H A Okazaki","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2391793","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2024.2391793","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The traditional classifications of motor skills nature (open vs closed; fine vs gross) have not been considered in handedness investigations. Instead, previous research focused on comparing complex vs less complex motor behaviour, leaving a gap in the literature. We compared manual preference between different motor skill characteristics, namely: fine and closed (FC), gross and closed (GC) and gross and open (GO) tasks. The hand preference was assessed with the Global Lateral Preference Inventory in four hundred and forty participants (244 women) aged from 18 to 59 years old. By assessing the degree and direction of handedness in different motor skills, our results showed a stronger lateralization pattern for FC motor skills as compared to GC and GO, with GO also being less lateralized than GC. Our results expand those of previous investigations that used the motor skill complexity definitions by showing how handedness can also be modulated by the interaction between classic motor skills classifications. Future research should consider fine vs. gross and open vs. closed classifications when selecting tasks for analysis of asymmetries of preference.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"1-16"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-08-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001055","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Fear is more right lateralized than happiness and anger: Evidence for the motivational hypothesis of emotional face perception? 与快乐和愤怒相比,恐惧的右侧化程度更高:情绪面孔感知动机假说的证据?
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2377633
Bridgette E Speranza, Aron T Hill, Michael Do, Peter H Donaldson, Peter G Enticott, Melissa Kirkovski

Facial emotion processing (FEP) tends to be right hemisphere lateralized. This right-hemispheric bias (RHB) for FEP varies within and between individuals. The aim of the present research was to examine evidence pertaining to the prominent theories of FEP hemispheric bias as measured by a half-emotional half-neutral (no emotion) chimeric faces task. FEP hemispheric bias was indexed using laterality quotients (LQs) calculated from a Chimeric Faces Task completed by 427 adults recruited from the general population aged 18-67 years. Participants indicated which of two identical (but mirrored) emotional-neutral chimeric faces were more emotive. While all investigated emotions (fear, anger, and happiness) were right lateralized, fear was significantly more right lateralized than anger and happiness. These results provide evidence for both the right hemisphere hypothesis and the motivational hypothesis of emotion perception.

面部情绪加工(FEP)倾向于右半球偏侧。面部情绪加工的右半球偏向(RHB)在个体内部和个体之间存在差异。本研究的目的是通过半情绪半中性(无情绪)嵌合面孔任务来检验与 FEP 半球偏向著名理论相关的证据。我们使用从普通人群中招募的 427 名 18-67 岁成年人完成的嵌合面孔任务计算出的侧向商数(LQs)来衡量 FEP 半球偏差。在两个完全相同(但有镜像)的情绪中性嵌合面孔中,受试者表示哪一个更具情绪性。虽然所有调查的情绪(恐惧、愤怒和快乐)都是右侧化的,但恐惧的右侧化程度明显高于愤怒和快乐。这些结果为情绪感知的右半球假说和动机假说提供了证据。
{"title":"Fear is more right lateralized than happiness and anger: Evidence for the motivational hypothesis of emotional face perception?","authors":"Bridgette E Speranza, Aron T Hill, Michael Do, Peter H Donaldson, Peter G Enticott, Melissa Kirkovski","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2377633","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2024.2377633","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Facial emotion processing (FEP) tends to be right hemisphere lateralized. This right-hemispheric bias (RHB) for FEP varies within and between individuals. The aim of the present research was to examine evidence pertaining to the prominent theories of FEP hemispheric bias as measured by a half-emotional half-neutral (no emotion) chimeric faces task. FEP hemispheric bias was indexed using laterality quotients (LQs) calculated from a Chimeric Faces Task completed by 427 adults recruited from the general population aged 18-67 years. Participants indicated which of two identical (but mirrored) emotional-neutral chimeric faces were more emotive. While all investigated emotions (fear, anger, and happiness) were right lateralized, fear was significantly more right lateralized than anger and happiness. These results provide evidence for both the right hemisphere hypothesis and the motivational hypothesis of emotion perception.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"1-15"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141634986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Footedness in merlins: Raptors perching in a cold climate. 梅花鹿的脚力:在寒冷气候中栖息的猛禽
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2374765
Ian G Warkentin

ABSTRACTPerching or standing on one foot is commonly reported in birds but the level of consistency in using one foot over the other has been less-well documented in most species, particularly birds of prey. For birds experiencing colder temperatures, unipedal perching has been attributed to limiting heat loss through unfeathered legs and feet; individuals should spend longer periods of time perched on one foot as temperatures decrease. Using radio tracking, I collected 486 hours of observations on nine overwintering, free-living merlins (Falco columbarius) in Saskatoon, Canada. Five merlins displayed clear preferences to perch on one foot, however the direction of preference was not consistent and four birds were ambidextrous. There was a curvilinear response in the proportion of time spent in unipedal posture versus temperature, with a peak of ∼22% of the time at moderate temperatures (-10 to -19°C), but lower values at warmer and colder temperatures; the main effect of the squared term for temperature was highly influential while individual foot preference had no impact on the use of unipedal perching. Although preferential use of one foot for perching was displayed by some individuals, thermoregulation may not be the primary driver of this behaviour at colder temperatures.

摘要单足栖息或站立在鸟类中很常见,但在大多数物种中,尤其是在猛禽中,单足栖息或站立的一致性较低。对于气温较低的鸟类来说,单足栖息可限制热量通过无羽毛的腿和脚散失;随着气温降低,鸟类单足栖息的时间会延长。通过无线电跟踪,我在加拿大萨斯卡通对九只越冬、自由生活的梅林(Falco columbarius)进行了486小时的观察。其中五只梅林明显偏好单脚栖息,但偏好方向并不一致,四只梅林为双脚栖息。单足栖息的时间比例与温度呈曲线关系,在温度适中(-10至-19°C)时,单足栖息的时间比例达到顶峰,为22%,但在温度较高和较低时,单足栖息的时间比例较低;温度平方项的主效应影响很大,而单足栖息的偏好对单足栖息的使用没有影响。虽然一些个体表现出优先使用单足栖息,但在较低温度下,体温调节可能不是这种行为的主要驱动因素。
{"title":"Footedness in merlins: Raptors perching in a cold climate.","authors":"Ian G Warkentin","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2374765","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2024.2374765","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>ABSTRACT</b>Perching or standing on one foot is commonly reported in birds but the level of consistency in using one foot over the other has been less-well documented in most species, particularly birds of prey. For birds experiencing colder temperatures, unipedal perching has been attributed to limiting heat loss through unfeathered legs and feet; individuals should spend longer periods of time perched on one foot as temperatures decrease. Using radio tracking, I collected 486 hours of observations on nine overwintering, free-living merlins (<i>Falco columbarius</i>) in Saskatoon, Canada. Five merlins displayed clear preferences to perch on one foot, however the direction of preference was not consistent and four birds were ambidextrous. There was a curvilinear response in the proportion of time spent in unipedal posture versus temperature, with a peak of ∼22% of the time at moderate temperatures (-10 to -19°C), but lower values at warmer and colder temperatures; the main effect of the squared term for temperature was highly influential while individual foot preference had no impact on the use of unipedal perching. Although preferential use of one foot for perching was displayed by some individuals, thermoregulation may not be the primary driver of this behaviour at colder temperatures.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"1-14"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-07-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141564748","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Phenotyping in clinical laterality research: a comparison of commonly used methods to determine mixed-handedness and ambidexterity. 临床侧向性研究中的表型分析:比较常用于确定混合手型和惯用手型的方法。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2370871
Annakarina Mundorf, Stephan Getzmann, Patrick D Gajewski, Mauro F Larra, Edmund Wascher, Erhan Genç, Sebastian Ocklenburg

An increased prevalence of mixed-handedness has been reported in several neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. Unfortunately, there is high between-study variability in the definition of mixed-handedness, leading to a major methodological problem in clinical laterality research and endangering replicability and comparability of research findings. Adding to this challenge is the fact that sometimes researchers use the concepts of mixed-handedness and ambidexterity interchangeably. Therefore, having a consensus on how to determine mixed-handedness and how to distinguish it from ambidexterity is crucial for clinical laterality research. To this end, hand preference and hand performance data from more than 600 participants from the Dortmund Vital Study (Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397), a population-based study in Germany, was analyzed to ascertain an optimal classification to determine mixed-handedness and ambidexterity. Using a combination of latent class analyses, effect size determination, and comparisons with the existing literature, we establish that an LQ cut-off criterion of +/-60 for mixed-handedness is optimal for future clinical laterality studies. Moreover, we show that mixed-handedness and ambidexterity are not identical and that the terms should not be used interchangeably. We further highlight the need for a consensus on how to mathematically determine ambidexterity as results of existing categorization schemes largely differ.Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT05155397.

据报道,在多种神经发育和精神疾病中,混用手的发病率都有所上升。遗憾的是,不同研究之间对 "惯用双手 "的定义存在很大差异,从而导致临床侧位研究中的一个主要方法问题,并危及研究结果的可复制性和可比性。研究人员有时会交替使用 "惯用双手 "和 "双手不协调 "这两个概念,这也加剧了这一挑战。因此,就如何判定惯用双手以及如何将其与手足不协调区分开来达成共识,对于临床侧位研究至关重要。为此,我们分析了德国一项基于人群的研究--多特蒙德生命力研究(试验注册:ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397)--中 600 多名参与者的手部偏好和手部表现数据,以确定确定惯用手和惯用手的最佳分类方法。通过综合运用潜类分析、效应大小测定以及与现有文献的比较,我们确定,对于未来的临床侧向性研究而言,混用手能力的 LQ 临界标准为 +/-60。此外,我们还表明,惯用手与惯用手并不相同,这两个术语不应互换使用。我们进一步强调,由于现有分类方案的结果大相径庭,因此有必要就如何用数学方法确定左右手灵活性达成共识:试验注册:ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397;https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT05155397。
{"title":"Phenotyping in clinical laterality research: a comparison of commonly used methods to determine mixed-handedness and ambidexterity.","authors":"Annakarina Mundorf, Stephan Getzmann, Patrick D Gajewski, Mauro F Larra, Edmund Wascher, Erhan Genç, Sebastian Ocklenburg","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2370871","DOIUrl":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2370871","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An increased prevalence of mixed-handedness has been reported in several neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders. Unfortunately, there is high between-study variability in the definition of mixed-handedness, leading to a major methodological problem in clinical laterality research and endangering replicability and comparability of research findings. Adding to this challenge is the fact that sometimes researchers use the concepts of mixed-handedness and ambidexterity interchangeably. Therefore, having a consensus on how to determine mixed-handedness and how to distinguish it from ambidexterity is crucial for clinical laterality research. To this end, hand preference and hand performance data from more than 600 participants from the Dortmund Vital Study (Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397), a population-based study in Germany, was analyzed to ascertain an optimal classification to determine mixed-handedness and ambidexterity. Using a combination of latent class analyses, effect size determination, and comparisons with the existing literature, we establish that an LQ cut-off criterion of +/-60 for mixed-handedness is optimal for future clinical laterality studies. Moreover, we show that mixed-handedness and ambidexterity are not identical and that the terms should not be used interchangeably. We further highlight the need for a consensus on how to mathematically determine ambidexterity as results of existing categorization schemes largely differ.<b>Trial registration:</b> ClinicalTrials.gov NCT05155397; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT05155397.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"331-349"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141538725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Opposite perceptual biases in analogous auditory and visual tasks are unique to consonant-vowel strings and are unlikely a consequence of repetition. 在类似的听觉和视觉任务中,相反的知觉偏差是辅音-元音字符串所独有的,不太可能是重复的结果。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-03 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2348832
Adam J Parker, Maria-Elisabeta Hontaru, Rachel Lin, Sophie Ollerenshaw, Rolando Bonandrini

Despite wide reporting of a right ear (RE) advantage on dichotic listening tasks and a right visual field (RVF) advantage on visual half-field tasks, we know very little about the relationship between these perceptual biases. Previous studies that have investigated perceptual asymmetries for analogous auditory and visual consonant-vowel tasks have indicated a serendipitous finding: a RE advantage and a left visual field (LVF) advantage with poor cross-modal correlations. In this study, we examined the possibility that this LVF advantage for visual processing of consonant-vowel strings may be a consequence of repetition by examining perceptual biases in analogous auditory and visual tasks for both consonant-vowel strings and words. We replicated opposite perceptual biases for consonant-vowel strings (RE and LVF advantages). This did not extend to word stimuli where we found RE and RVF advantages. Furthermore, these perceptual biases did not differ across the three experimental blocks. Thus, we can firmly conclude that this LVF advantage is unique to consonant-vowel strings and is not a consequence of the repetition of a relatively limited number of stimuli. Finally, a test of covariances indicated no cross-modal relationships between laterality indices suggesting that perceptual biases are dissociable within individuals and cluster on mode of presentation.

尽管有大量报道称,在二分听觉任务中右耳(RE)有优势,在视觉半视野任务中右视野(RVF)有优势,但我们对这些知觉偏差之间的关系知之甚少。以前的研究对类似的听觉和视觉辅音-元音任务的知觉不对称进行了调查,发现了一个偶然的现象:RE 优势和左视野(LVF)优势的跨模态相关性很差。在本研究中,我们通过考察类似听觉和视觉任务中对辅音-元音字符串和单词的知觉偏差,研究了视觉处理辅音-元音字符串的左视野优势可能是重复的结果。我们对辅音-元音字符串复制了相反的知觉偏差(RE 和 LVF 优势)。但这一现象并没有延伸到单词刺激上,我们在单词刺激上发现了 RE 和 RVF 优势。此外,这些知觉偏差在三个实验组中并无差异。因此,我们可以坚定地得出结论:LVF 优势是辅音-元音字符串所独有的,而不是重复相对有限数量的刺激的结果。最后,协方差检验表明,侧向性指数之间存在跨模态关系,这表明知觉偏差在个体内部是可分离的,并根据呈现模式而聚集。
{"title":"Opposite perceptual biases in analogous auditory and visual tasks are unique to consonant-vowel strings and are unlikely a consequence of repetition.","authors":"Adam J Parker, Maria-Elisabeta Hontaru, Rachel Lin, Sophie Ollerenshaw, Rolando Bonandrini","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2348832","DOIUrl":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2348832","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Despite wide reporting of a right ear (RE) advantage on dichotic listening tasks and a right visual field (RVF) advantage on visual half-field tasks, we know very little about the relationship between these perceptual biases. Previous studies that have investigated perceptual asymmetries for analogous auditory and visual consonant-vowel tasks have indicated a serendipitous finding: a RE advantage and a left visual field (LVF) advantage with poor cross-modal correlations. In this study, we examined the possibility that this LVF advantage for visual processing of consonant-vowel strings may be a consequence of repetition by examining perceptual biases in analogous auditory and visual tasks for both consonant-vowel strings and words. We replicated opposite perceptual biases for consonant-vowel strings (RE and LVF advantages). This did not extend to word stimuli where we found RE and RVF advantages. Furthermore, these perceptual biases did not differ across the three experimental blocks. Thus, we can firmly conclude that this LVF advantage is unique to consonant-vowel strings and is not a consequence of the repetition of a relatively limited number of stimuli. Finally, a test of covariances indicated no cross-modal relationships between laterality indices suggesting that perceptual biases are dissociable within individuals and cluster on mode of presentation.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"283-312"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140865452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Visual lateralization in the sky: Geese manifest visual lateralization when flying with pair mates. 天空中的视觉横向化鹅在与配对伙伴飞行时表现出视觉侧向性。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-09 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2368587
Elmira Zaynagutdinova, Andrea Kölzsch, Alexandra Sinelshikova, Michael Vorotkov, Gerhard J D M Müskens, Andrey Giljov, Karina Karenina

The brain's sensory lateralization involves the processing of information from the sensory organs primarily in one hemisphere. This can improve brain efficiency by reducing interference and duplication of neural circuits. For species that rely on successful interaction among family partners, such as geese, lateralization can be advantageous. However, at the group level, one-sided biases in sensory lateralization can make individuals predictable to competitors and predators. We investigated lateral preferences in the positioning of pair mates of Greater white-fronted geese Anser albifrons albifrons. Using GPS-GSM trackers, we monitored individual geese in flight throughout the year. Our findings indicate that geese exhibit individual lateral biases when viewing their mate in flight, but the direction of these biases varies among individuals. We suggest that these patterns of visual lateralization could be an adaptive trait for the species with long-term social monogamy, high levels of interspecies communication and competition, and high levels of predator and hunting pressure.

大脑的感觉偏侧涉及主要在一个半球处理来自感觉器官的信息。这可以减少神经回路的干扰和重复,从而提高大脑的效率。对于依赖家庭伙伴间成功互动的物种(如大雁)来说,侧向化是有利的。然而,在群体水平上,感觉侧向化的片面偏差会使个体容易被竞争对手和捕食者发现。我们研究了大白额雁(Anser albifrons albifrons)对配偶定位的侧向偏好。我们使用全球定位系统-全球移动通信系统(GPS-GSM)跟踪器监测了全年飞行中的大白额雁个体。我们的研究结果表明,大白额雁在观察飞行中的配偶时会表现出个体横向偏好,但这些偏好的方向因个体而异。我们认为,这种视觉侧向模式可能是该物种的一种适应性特征,因为该物种具有长期的社会一夫一妻制、高水平的种间交流和竞争以及高水平的捕食和狩猎压力。
{"title":"Visual lateralization in the sky: Geese manifest visual lateralization when flying with pair mates.","authors":"Elmira Zaynagutdinova, Andrea Kölzsch, Alexandra Sinelshikova, Michael Vorotkov, Gerhard J D M Müskens, Andrey Giljov, Karina Karenina","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2368587","DOIUrl":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2368587","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The brain's sensory lateralization involves the processing of information from the sensory organs primarily in one hemisphere. This can improve brain efficiency by reducing interference and duplication of neural circuits. For species that rely on successful interaction among family partners, such as geese, lateralization can be advantageous. However, at the group level, one-sided biases in sensory lateralization can make individuals predictable to competitors and predators. We investigated lateral preferences in the positioning of pair mates of Greater white-fronted geese <i>Anser albifrons albifrons</i>. Using GPS-GSM trackers, we monitored individual geese in flight throughout the year. Our findings indicate that geese exhibit individual lateral biases when viewing their mate in flight, but the direction of these biases varies among individuals. We suggest that these patterns of visual lateralization could be an adaptive trait for the species with long-term social monogamy, high levels of interspecies communication and competition, and high levels of predator and hunting pressure.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"313-330"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141560053","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hemispheric engagement during the processing of affective adjectives-an ERP divided visual field study. 情感形容词处理过程中的半球参与--ERP 分视野研究。
IF 0.9 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2024.2331278
Szczepan J Grzybowski, Miroslaw Wyczesany

The study looked into the hemispheres' involvement in emotional word encoding. It combined brain activity measures (ERPs) with behavioural data during the affective categorization task in the divided visual field presentation paradigm. Forty healthy right-handed student volunteers took part in the study, in which they viewed and evaluated 33 positive and 33 negative emotional adjectives presented to either the left or right visual field. We observed a marginally significant effect on the earlier time window (220-250 ms, the P2 component) with higher mean amplitudes evoked to the words presented to the right hemisphere, and then a strong effect on the 340-400 ms (the P3) with a reversed pattern (higher amplitudes for words presented to the left hemisphere). The latter effect was also visible in the error rates and RTs, with better overall performance for adjectives presented to the left hemisphere. There was also an effect on behavioural data of positive words only (higher error rates, shorter RTs). Thus, the study showed a particular "progression" pattern of hemispheric engagement: dependence of the initial stages of affective lexico-semantic processing on the right hemisphere, replaced by the left-hemispheric dominance for content evaluation and response programming stages.

该研究探讨了大脑半球参与情感词编码的情况。研究结合了大脑活动测量(ERPs)和在分割视野呈现范式中进行情感分类任务时的行为数据。40名健康的右撇子学生志愿者参加了这项研究,他们观看并评估了在左侧或右侧视野中呈现的33个正面和33个负面情绪形容词。我们观察到,在较早的时间窗口(220-250 毫秒,即 P2 部分)中,右侧大脑半球出现的词语会引起较高的平均振幅,这种效应略微明显;而在 340-400 毫秒(即 P3 部分)中,则出现了强烈的反向效应(左侧大脑半球出现的词语会引起较高的振幅)。后一种效应在错误率和反应时间上也很明显,在左半球出现的形容词总体表现更好。此外,只对正面词语的行为数据有影响(错误率更高,RT 更短)。因此,研究显示了半球参与的特殊 "渐进 "模式:情感词义处理的初始阶段依赖右半球,而在内容评估和反应编程阶段则由左半球主导。
{"title":"Hemispheric engagement during the processing of affective adjectives-an ERP divided visual field study.","authors":"Szczepan J Grzybowski, Miroslaw Wyczesany","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2331278","DOIUrl":"10.1080/1357650X.2024.2331278","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The study looked into the hemispheres' involvement in emotional word encoding. It combined brain activity measures (ERPs) with behavioural data during the affective categorization task in the divided visual field presentation paradigm. Forty healthy right-handed student volunteers took part in the study, in which they viewed and evaluated 33 positive and 33 negative emotional adjectives presented to either the left or right visual field. We observed a marginally significant effect on the earlier time window (220-250 ms, the P2 component) with higher mean amplitudes evoked to the words presented to the right hemisphere, and then a strong effect on the 340-400 ms (the P3) with a reversed pattern (higher amplitudes for words presented to the left hemisphere). The latter effect was also visible in the error rates and RTs, with better overall performance for adjectives presented to the left hemisphere. There was also an effect on behavioural data of positive words only (higher error rates, shorter RTs). Thus, the study showed a particular \"progression\" pattern of hemispheric engagement: dependence of the initial stages of affective lexico-semantic processing on the right hemisphere, replaced by the left-hemispheric dominance for content evaluation and response programming stages.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":" ","pages":"223-245"},"PeriodicalIF":0.9,"publicationDate":"2024-05-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177082","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Laterality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1