首页 > 最新文献

Laterality最新文献

英文 中文
Stress exposure, hand preference, and hand skill: A deep phenotyping approach. 压力暴露,手偏好和手技能:一种深度表型方法。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2023.2204551
Annakarina Mundorf, Stephan Getzmann, Patrick D Gajewski, Mauro F Larra, Edmund Wascher, Sebastian Ocklenburg

ABSTRACTStress exposure and reactivity may show differential associations with handedness, but shallow phenotyping may influence the current knowledge. Importantly, different handedness measures do not necessarily show high correlations with each other and should not be used interchangeably as they may reflect different dimensions of laterality. Here, data on handedness from 599 participants in the population-based, longitudinal Dortmund Vital Study was used to determine various asymmetry indices. Hand preference was assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) and the lateral preference inventory (LPI) measuring handedness, footedness, earedness, and eyedness. Hand performance was determined using the pegboard test. In addition, data on several dimensions of stress exposure and reactivity, including hair cortisol, and mental well-being was analysed to determine associations with handedness. All handedness measures correlated significantly with each other, with the strongest correlation between the EHI and the LPI handedness score. The EHI and LPI hand measures resulted in the highest effect sizes and most consistent correlations with stress or mental well-being. In contrast, the pegboard test only showed very little association with the stress and mental well-being measures. This highlights the importance of handedness phenotyping. Including preference measures is recommended to disentangle the link between handedness and mental health.

摘要压力暴露和反应性可能显示出与手性的不同关联,但浅层表型可能影响当前的知识。重要的是,不同的手性测量不一定显示出彼此之间的高度相关性,也不应该互换使用,因为它们可能反映了不同的侧性维度。在这里,来自599名参与者的以人群为基础的,纵向多特蒙德生命研究的数据被用来确定各种不对称指数。用爱丁堡利手性量表(EHI)和测量利手性、足性、耳性和眼性的横向偏好量表(LPI)来评估手性偏好。使用钉板测试确定了手性能。此外,还分析了压力暴露和反应性的几个维度的数据,包括毛发皮质醇和心理健康,以确定与惯用手的关系。所有的手性测量结果彼此之间都有显著的相关性,其中EHI和LPI的手性得分之间的相关性最强。EHI和LPI手部测量结果的效应值最高,与压力或心理健康的相关性也最一致。相比之下,钉板测试只显示出与压力和心理健康测量的很少关联。这突出了利手性表型的重要性。建议包括偏好测量,以理清利手与心理健康之间的联系。
{"title":"Stress exposure, hand preference, and hand skill: A deep phenotyping approach.","authors":"Annakarina Mundorf,&nbsp;Stephan Getzmann,&nbsp;Patrick D Gajewski,&nbsp;Mauro F Larra,&nbsp;Edmund Wascher,&nbsp;Sebastian Ocklenburg","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2023.2204551","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2023.2204551","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>ABSTRACT</b>Stress exposure and reactivity may show differential associations with handedness, but shallow phenotyping may influence the current knowledge. Importantly, different handedness measures do not necessarily show high correlations with each other and should not be used interchangeably as they may reflect different dimensions of laterality. Here, data on handedness from 599 participants in the population-based, longitudinal Dortmund Vital Study was used to determine various asymmetry indices. Hand preference was assessed with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) and the lateral preference inventory (LPI) measuring handedness, footedness, earedness, and eyedness. Hand performance was determined using the pegboard test. In addition, data on several dimensions of stress exposure and reactivity, including hair cortisol, and mental well-being was analysed to determine associations with handedness. All handedness measures correlated significantly with each other, with the strongest correlation between the EHI and the LPI handedness score. The EHI and LPI hand measures resulted in the highest effect sizes and most consistent correlations with stress or mental well-being. In contrast, the pegboard test only showed very little association with the stress and mental well-being measures. This highlights the importance of handedness phenotyping. Including preference measures is recommended to disentangle the link between handedness and mental health.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9603636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 2
The sinister story of a gauche deliverer and his corrie-fisted tribesmen: Ehud and the left-handed artillery. 邪恶的故事,一个笨拙的快递员和他的corrifist部落成员:Ehud和左手炮兵。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199965
Alex G Stewart, Alan R Millard

Bias against left-handers is well-documented and seen in the etymology of "left" and "right" in most languages. The subject of this study, Ehud, lived between the exodus of the Hebrew slaves from Egypt and the establishment of the Israelite kingdom (c1200-1000 BC), at the transition between the Late Bronze Age and Iron Age 1. His left-handedness was crucial to his deliverance of the proto-nation from tyranny, recorded in Judges in the Hebrew Bible. The description of Ehud as left-handed ('iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô) is used once more in the Hebrew Bible, also in Judges, to describe the artillery of Ehud's tribe. The words apparently mean "bound/restricted in the right hand", sometimes interpreted as "ambidextrous". This is unlikely: ambidexterity is uncommon. The artillery used the sling with either hand, but Ehud used his left (śεm'ōl) hand to draw his sword. śεm'ōl, used throughout the Hebrew Bible, means left, without bias or derogatory implications. We suggest that 'iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô was a right-handed bias towards left-handed people, but Ehud's left-handed victory was recognized as significant. Significant enough that (a) the language changed and the biased description was dropped in favour of simple description, and (b) the army changed with the development of left-handed slingers (artillery).

对左撇子的偏见是有案可查的,在大多数语言中“左”和“右”的词源中都可以看到。本研究的主题Ehud生活在希伯来奴隶从埃及流亡到以色列王国建立(公元前1200-1000年)之间,处于青铜时代晚期和铁器时代之间的过渡时期。《希伯来圣经》中的《士师记》记载,他的左撇子对他将原民族从暴政中解救出来至关重要。以笏是左撇子的描述(iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô)在希伯来圣经中再次被使用,也在士师记中,用来描述以笏部落的炮兵。这个词的意思显然是“右手被束缚/限制”,有时被解释为“双手灵巧的”。这是不可能的:双手灵巧的人并不多见。炮兵用两只手投掷,但以笏用他的左手(śεm'ōl)拔出剑。śεm'ōl,在整个希伯来圣经中使用,意思是左,没有偏见或贬义。我们认为iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô是右撇子对左撇子的偏见,但Ehud左撇子的胜利被认为是重要的。重要的是(a)语言改变了,有偏见的描述被简单的描述所取代,(b)军队随着左手投石器(火炮)的发展而改变。
{"title":"The sinister story of a gauche deliverer and his corrie-fisted tribesmen: Ehud and the left-handed artillery.","authors":"Alex G Stewart,&nbsp;Alan R Millard","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199965","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199965","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Bias against left-handers is well-documented and seen in the etymology of \"left\" and \"right\" in most languages. The subject of this study, Ehud, lived between the exodus of the Hebrew slaves from Egypt and the establishment of the Israelite kingdom (c1200-1000 BC), at the transition between the Late Bronze Age and Iron Age 1. His left-handedness was crucial to his deliverance of the proto-nation from tyranny, recorded in Judges in the Hebrew Bible. The description of Ehud as left-handed (<i>'iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô</i>) is used once more in the Hebrew Bible, also in Judges, to describe the artillery of Ehud's tribe. The words apparently mean \"bound/restricted in the right hand\", sometimes interpreted as \"ambidextrous\". This is unlikely: ambidexterity is uncommon. The artillery used the sling with either hand, but Ehud used his left (<i>śεm'ōl</i>) hand to draw his sword. <i>śεm'ōl</i>, used throughout the Hebrew Bible, means left, without bias or derogatory implications. We suggest that '<i>iṭṭēr yaḏ-yεmînô</i> was a right-handed bias towards left-handed people, but Ehud's left-handed victory was recognized as significant. Significant enough that (a) the language changed and the biased description was dropped in favour of simple description, and (b) the army changed with the development of left-handed slingers (artillery).</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9605519","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Dual-task decrements in mono-, bi- and multilingual participants: Evidence for multilingual advantage. 单语、双语和多语参与者的双重任务减少:多语优势的证据。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2023.2178061
Sameera M Sidat, Anastasia Giannakopoulou, Christopher J Hand, Joanne Ingram

Evidence suggests that language processing in bilinguals is less left-lateralized than in monolinguals. We explored dual-task decrement (DTD) for mono-, bi- and multilinguals in a verbal-motor dual-task paradigm. We expected monolinguals to show greater DTD than bilingual participants, who would show greater DTD than multilingual participants. Fifty right-handed participants (18 monolingual, 16 bilingual, 16 multilingual) completed verbal fluency and manual motor tasks in isolation and concurrently. Tasks were completed twice in isolation (left-handed, right-handed) and twice as dual-tasks (left-handed, right-handed); participants' motor-executing hands served proxy for hemispheric activation. Results supported the hypotheses. Completing dual-tasks incurred greater cost for manual motor tasks than for verbal fluency tasks. Negative cost of performing dual-tasks diminished as number of languages spoken increased; in fact, multilingual individuals demonstrated a dual-task advantage in both tasks when using the right hand, strongest in the verbal task. Dual-tasking had the greatest negative impact on verbal fluency of monolingual participants when the motor task was completed with the right hand; for bi- and multi-lingual participants, the greatest negative impact on verbal fluency was seen when the motor task was completed with the left hand. Results provide support for the bi-lateralization of language function in bi- and multilingual individuals.

有证据表明,双语者的语言处理不如单语者的左偏化。我们在语言-运动双任务范式中探讨了单语言、双语言和多语言的双任务减量(DTD)。我们期望单语参与者比双语参与者显示更大的DTD,而双语参与者比多语参与者显示更大的DTD。50名右撇子参与者(18名单语者,16名双语者,16名多语者)单独或同时完成了语言流畅性和手动运动任务。单独完成两次任务(左手和右手)和两次双重任务(左手和右手);参与者的运动执行手是半球激活的代理。结果支持这些假设。完成双任务时,体力运动任务比语言流畅任务的成本更高。执行双重任务的负成本随着使用语言数量的增加而减少;事实上,掌握多种语言的人在使用右手时在两项任务中都表现出双重优势,在口头任务中表现得最明显。当用右手完成运动任务时,双任务对单语参与者的语言流畅性有最大的负面影响;对于双语和多语的参与者来说,当用左手完成运动任务时,对语言流畅性的负面影响最大。研究结果为双语和多语个体的语言功能双侧化提供了支持。
{"title":"Dual-task decrements in mono-, bi- and multilingual participants: Evidence for multilingual advantage.","authors":"Sameera M Sidat,&nbsp;Anastasia Giannakopoulou,&nbsp;Christopher J Hand,&nbsp;Joanne Ingram","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2023.2178061","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2023.2178061","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Evidence suggests that language processing in bilinguals is less left-lateralized than in monolinguals. We explored dual-task decrement (DTD) for mono-, bi- and multilinguals in a verbal-motor dual-task paradigm. We expected monolinguals to show greater DTD than bilingual participants, who would show greater DTD than multilingual participants. Fifty right-handed participants (18 monolingual, 16 bilingual, 16 multilingual) completed verbal fluency and manual motor tasks in isolation and concurrently. Tasks were completed twice in isolation (left-handed, right-handed) and twice as dual-tasks (left-handed, right-handed); participants' motor-executing hands served proxy for hemispheric activation. Results supported the hypotheses. Completing dual-tasks incurred greater cost for manual motor tasks than for verbal fluency tasks. Negative cost of performing dual-tasks diminished as number of languages spoken increased; in fact, multilingual individuals demonstrated a dual-task advantage in both tasks when using the right hand, strongest in the verbal task. Dual-tasking had the greatest negative impact on verbal fluency of monolingual participants when the motor task was completed with the right hand; for bi- and multi-lingual participants, the greatest negative impact on verbal fluency was seen when the motor task was completed with the left hand. Results provide support for the bi-lateralization of language function in bi- and multilingual individuals.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9959237","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Laterality indices consensus initiative (LICI): A Delphi expert survey report on recommendations to record, assess, and report asymmetry in human behavioural and brain research. 侧性指数共识倡议(LICI):一份德尔菲专家调查报告,建议记录、评估和报告人类行为和大脑研究中的不对称性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199963
Guy Vingerhoets, Helena Verhelst, Robin Gerrits, Nicholas Badcock, Dorothy V M Bishop, David Carey, Jason Flindall, Gina Grimshaw, Lauren Julius Harris, Markus Hausmann, Marco Hirnstein, Lutz Jäncke, Marc Joliot, Karsten Specht, René Westerhausen

Laterality indices (LIs) quantify the left-right asymmetry of brain and behavioural variables and provide a measure that is statistically convenient and seemingly easy to interpret. Substantial variability in how structural and functional asymmetries are recorded, calculated, and reported, however, suggest little agreement on the conditions required for its valid assessment. The present study aimed for consensus on general aspects in this context of laterality research, and more specifically within a particular method or technique (i.e., dichotic listening, visual half-field technique, performance asymmetries, preference bias reports, electrophysiological recording, functional MRI, structural MRI, and functional transcranial Doppler sonography). Experts in laterality research were invited to participate in an online Delphi survey to evaluate consensus and stimulate discussion. In Round 0, 106 experts generated 453 statements on what they considered good practice in their field of expertise. Statements were organised into a 295-statement survey that the experts then were asked, in Round 1, to independently assess for importance and support, which further reduced the survey to 241 statements that were presented again to the experts in Round 2. Based on the Round 2 input, we present a set of critically reviewed key recommendations to record, assess, and report laterality research for various methods.

侧性指数(LIs)量化了大脑的左右不对称性和行为变量,并提供了一种统计上方便且看似易于解释的测量方法。然而,在如何记录、计算和报告结构和功能不对称方面的实质性变化表明,对其有效评估所需的条件几乎没有一致意见。本研究旨在就侧边性研究的一般方面达成共识,更具体地说,在特定的方法或技术(即,二分聆听,视觉半视野技术,表现不对称,偏好偏差报告,电生理记录,功能性MRI,结构MRI和功能性经颅多普勒超声)。横向研究方面的专家被邀请参加在线德尔菲调查,以评估共识并激发讨论。在第0轮中,106位专家就他们所认为的各自专业领域的良好实践提出了453项陈述。陈述被组织成一份295条陈述的调查,然后在第一轮中,专家们被要求独立评估重要性和支持度,这进一步将调查减少到241条陈述,再次提交给第二轮的专家。基于第二轮的输入,我们提出了一套经过严格审查的关键建议,以记录、评估和报告各种方法的横向研究。
{"title":"Laterality indices consensus initiative (LICI): A Delphi expert survey report on recommendations to record, assess, and report asymmetry in human behavioural and brain research.","authors":"Guy Vingerhoets,&nbsp;Helena Verhelst,&nbsp;Robin Gerrits,&nbsp;Nicholas Badcock,&nbsp;Dorothy V M Bishop,&nbsp;David Carey,&nbsp;Jason Flindall,&nbsp;Gina Grimshaw,&nbsp;Lauren Julius Harris,&nbsp;Markus Hausmann,&nbsp;Marco Hirnstein,&nbsp;Lutz Jäncke,&nbsp;Marc Joliot,&nbsp;Karsten Specht,&nbsp;René Westerhausen","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199963","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2023.2199963","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Laterality indices (LIs) quantify the left-right asymmetry of brain and behavioural variables and provide a measure that is statistically convenient and seemingly easy to interpret. Substantial variability in how structural and functional asymmetries are recorded, calculated, and reported, however, suggest little agreement on the conditions required for its valid assessment. The present study aimed for consensus on general aspects in this context of laterality research, and more specifically within a particular method or technique (i.e., dichotic listening, visual half-field technique, performance asymmetries, preference bias reports, electrophysiological recording, functional MRI, structural MRI, and functional transcranial Doppler sonography). Experts in laterality research were invited to participate in an online Delphi survey to evaluate consensus and stimulate discussion. In Round 0, 106 experts generated 453 statements on what they considered good practice in their field of expertise. Statements were organised into a 295-statement survey that the experts then were asked, in Round 1, to independently assess for importance and support, which further reduced the survey to 241 statements that were presented again to the experts in Round 2. Based on the Round 2 input, we present a set of critically reviewed key recommendations to record, assess, and report laterality research for various methods.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9606544","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 8
Compromised bilaterality in the small African pangolin (Phataginus tricuspis) - An expression of or compensation for developmental errors; asymmetry, elliptical Fourier analyses and modularity. 非洲小穿山甲(Phataginus tricuspis)的双边性受损-发育错误的表现或补偿;不对称,椭圆傅里叶分析和模块化。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-03-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2023.2181068
O Michael Samuel, Olumayowa Igado, A Joan Adekanmbi

We present a postnatal evaluation of skull developmental signaling in small African pangolin emphasizing structural, and cognitive trend in ontogeny for assessment of developmental instability, proper identification and classification, forty digitally processed skulls and foramen magnum from different geo-locations were assessed for asymmetry, foramen magnum shape and modularity using geometric and Elliptical Fourier analyses. Multivariate analysis of regression demonstrated low (p < 0.5) but directional fluctuating asymmetry (F1539 = 3.4045, F882 = 3.2665, dorsal and ventral views). Allometric trajectories followed rostrocaudal, caudolateral directions; Intercepts for shape/size predictions were parallel. Mahalanobis distances between centroids (2.42) were significant (p < 0.01). The variance-covariance matrix in ontogeny lies between 0.0017 and 0.56. Foramen magnum outline descriptors by incremental harmonics revealed first 4 effective PCs defined 96.98% of shape properties and 3.02% constituted finerdetails. 74.1% accuracy decline after size factor elimination. Modulation PCA of covariance matrix and asymmetry component was 88.38% and 7.48% (PC1 and 2), respectively, the variance % predicted 10.08%. The studied samples confirmed 'handedness' and fluctuating asymmetries. Foramen magnum shape assumed priority over size in ontogeny with profound asymmetry (from the 5th harmonic), suggestive of compensations to lateralization in neural integration modules.

我们对小型非洲穿山甲的颅骨发育信号进行了产后评估,强调结构和个体发育的认知趋势,以评估发育不稳定性,适当的识别和分类,使用几何和椭圆傅里叶分析对来自不同地理位置的40个经过数字处理的头骨和枕骨大孔的不对称性,枕骨大孔形状和模块性进行了评估。多因素回归分析显示低(p 1539 = 3.4045, F882 = 3.2665,背侧和腹侧视图)。异速生长轨迹遵循背侧、尾侧方向;形状/大小预测的截距是平行的。马氏鱼质心之间的距离(2.42)显著(p
{"title":"Compromised bilaterality in the small African pangolin (<i>Phataginus tricuspis</i>) - An expression of or compensation for developmental errors; asymmetry, elliptical Fourier analyses and modularity.","authors":"O Michael Samuel,&nbsp;Olumayowa Igado,&nbsp;A Joan Adekanmbi","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2023.2181068","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2023.2181068","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We present a postnatal evaluation of skull developmental signaling in small African pangolin emphasizing structural, and cognitive trend in ontogeny for assessment of developmental instability, proper identification and classification, forty digitally processed skulls and foramen magnum from different geo-locations were assessed for asymmetry, foramen magnum shape and modularity using geometric and Elliptical Fourier analyses. Multivariate analysis of regression demonstrated low (p < 0.5) but directional fluctuating asymmetry (F<sub>1539</sub> = 3.4045, F<sub>882</sub> = 3.2665, dorsal and ventral views). Allometric trajectories followed rostrocaudal, caudolateral directions; Intercepts for shape/size predictions were parallel. Mahalanobis distances between centroids (2.42) were significant (p < 0.01). The variance-covariance matrix in ontogeny lies between 0.0017 and 0.56. Foramen magnum outline descriptors by incremental harmonics revealed first 4 effective PCs defined 96.98% of shape properties and 3.02% constituted finerdetails. 74.1% accuracy decline after size factor elimination. Modulation PCA of covariance matrix and asymmetry component was 88.38% and 7.48% (PC1 and 2), respectively, the variance % predicted 10.08%. The studied samples confirmed 'handedness' and fluctuating asymmetries. Foramen magnum shape assumed priority over size in ontogeny with profound asymmetry (from the 5th harmonic), suggestive of compensations to lateralization in neural integration modules.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-03-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9595680","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A meta-analysis of the line bisection task in children. 儿童平分线任务的荟萃分析。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2147941
Danishta Kaul, Marietta Papadatou-Pastou, Gemma Learmonth

Meta-analyses have shown subtle, group-level asymmetries of spatial attention in adults favouring the left hemispace (pseudoneglect). However, no meta-analysis has synthesized data on children. We performed a random-effects meta-analysis of spatial biases in children aged ≤16 years. Databases (PsycINFO, Web of Science & Scopus) and pre-print servers (bioRxiv, medRxiv & PsyArXiv) were searched for studies involving typically developing children with a mean age of ≤16, who were tested using line bisection. Thirty-three datasets, from 31 studies, involving 2101 children, were included. No bias was identified overall, but there was a small leftward bias in a subgroup where all children were aged ≤16. Moderator analysis found symmetrical neglect, with right-handed actions resulting in right-biased bisections, and left-handed actions in left-biased bisections. Bisections were more leftward in studies with a higher percentage of boys relative to girls. Mean age, hand preference, and control group status did not moderate biases, and there was no difference between children aged ≤7 and ≥7 years, although the number of studies in each moderator analysis was small. There was no evidence of small study bias. We conclude that pseudoneglect may be present in children but is dependent on individual characteristics (sex) and/or task demands (hand used).Registration: Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/n68fz/).

荟萃分析显示,成年人的空间注意力存在微妙的群体不对称,倾向于左半球(假性忽视)。然而,还没有关于儿童的综合数据的荟萃分析。我们对16岁以下儿童的空间偏差进行了随机效应荟萃分析。检索数据库(PsycINFO, Web of Science & Scopus)和预印本服务器(bioRxiv, medRxiv和PsyArXiv)中涉及平均年龄≤16岁的典型发育儿童的研究,这些儿童采用线平分法进行测试。纳入了来自31项研究的33个数据集,涉及2101名儿童。总体上没有发现偏倚,但在所有儿童年龄≤16岁的亚组中存在轻微的偏左。调节分析发现对称忽略,右手动作导致右偏等分,左手动作导致左偏等分。在男孩比例高于女孩比例的研究中,平分线更偏左。平均年龄、手部偏好和对照组状态不存在中度偏倚,并且≤7岁和≥7岁的儿童之间没有差异,尽管每个调节分析的研究数量很少。没有证据表明存在小的研究偏差。我们的结论是,儿童可能存在假性忽视,但这取决于个体特征(性别)和/或任务需求(手使用)。注册:开放科学框架(https://osf.io/n68fz/)。
{"title":"A meta-analysis of the line bisection task in children.","authors":"Danishta Kaul,&nbsp;Marietta Papadatou-Pastou,&nbsp;Gemma Learmonth","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2022.2147941","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2022.2147941","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Meta-analyses have shown subtle, group-level asymmetries of spatial attention in adults favouring the left hemispace (pseudoneglect). However, no meta-analysis has synthesized data on children. We performed a random-effects meta-analysis of spatial biases in children aged ≤16 years. Databases (PsycINFO, Web of Science & Scopus) and pre-print servers (bioRxiv, medRxiv & PsyArXiv) were searched for studies involving typically developing children with a mean age of ≤16, who were tested using line bisection. Thirty-three datasets, from 31 studies, involving 2101 children, were included. No bias was identified overall, but there was a small leftward bias in a subgroup where all children were aged ≤16. Moderator analysis found symmetrical neglect, with right-handed actions resulting in right-biased bisections, and left-handed actions in left-biased bisections. Bisections were more leftward in studies with a higher percentage of boys relative to girls. Mean age, hand preference, and control group status did not moderate biases, and there was no difference between children aged ≤7 and ≥7 years, although the number of studies in each moderator analysis was small. There was no evidence of small study bias. We conclude that pseudoneglect may be present in children but is dependent on individual characteristics (sex) and/or task demands (hand used).Registration: Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/n68fz/).</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9486338","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Testing the relationship between lateralization on sequence-based motor tasks and language laterality using an online battery. 使用在线电池测试基于顺序的运动任务的偏侧性与语言偏侧性的关系。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2129668
Jack H Grant, Adam J Parker, Jessica C Hodgson, John M Hudson, Dorothy V M Bishop

ABSTRACTStudies have highlighted an association between motor laterality and speech production laterality. It is thought that common demands for sequential processing may underlie this association. However, most studies in this area have relied on relatively small samples and have infrequently explored the reliability of the tools used to assess lateralization. We, therefore, established the validity and reliability of an online battery measuring sequence-based motor laterality and language laterality before exploring the associations between laterality indices on language and motor tasks. The online battery was completed by 621 participants, 52 of whom returned to complete the battery a second time. The three motor tasks included in the battery showed good between-session reliability (r  .78) and were lateralized in concordance with hand preference. The novel measure of speech production laterality was left lateralized at population level as predicted, but reliability was less satisfactory (r = .62). We found no evidence of an association between sequence-based motor laterality and language laterality. Those with a left-hand preference were more strongly lateralized on motor tasks requiring midline crossing; this effect was not observed in right-handers. We conclude that there is little evidence of the co-lateralization of language and sequence-based motor skill on this battery.

摘要研究强调了运动侧性和言语产生侧性之间的联系。人们认为,对顺序处理的共同需求可能是这种关联的基础。然而,该领域的大多数研究都依赖于相对较小的样本,并且很少探索用于评估侧化的工具的可靠性。因此,我们在探索语言和运动任务的横向指数之间的关联之前,建立了基于序列的运动横向和语言横向在线电池测量的有效性和可靠性。621名参与者完成了在线测试,其中52人第二次返回完成测试。组内的三个运动任务表现出良好的会话间可靠性(r≥0.78),并且与手偏好相一致。正如预测的那样,新的言语产生偏侧性测量在总体水平上是偏侧的,但可靠性不太令人满意(r = 0.62)。我们没有发现基于序列的运动偏侧性和语言偏侧性之间存在关联的证据。那些偏爱左手的人在需要穿越中线的运动任务中表现出更强的偏侧性;这种效应在右撇子中没有观察到。我们的结论是,几乎没有证据表明语言和基于顺序的运动技能在这个电池上的共同侧化。
{"title":"Testing the relationship between lateralization on sequence-based motor tasks and language laterality using an online battery.","authors":"Jack H Grant,&nbsp;Adam J Parker,&nbsp;Jessica C Hodgson,&nbsp;John M Hudson,&nbsp;Dorothy V M Bishop","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2022.2129668","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2022.2129668","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p><b>ABSTRACT</b>Studies have highlighted an association between motor laterality and speech production laterality. It is thought that common demands for sequential processing may underlie this association. However, most studies in this area have relied on relatively small samples and have infrequently explored the reliability of the tools used to assess lateralization. We, therefore, established the validity and reliability of an online battery measuring sequence-based motor laterality and language laterality before exploring the associations between laterality indices on language and motor tasks. The online battery was completed by 621 participants, 52 of whom returned to complete the battery a second time. The three motor tasks included in the battery showed good between-session reliability (<i>r <i>≥</i></i> .78) and were lateralized in concordance with hand preference. The novel measure of speech production laterality was left lateralized at population level as predicted, but reliability was less satisfactory (<i>r </i>= .62). We found no evidence of an association between sequence-based motor laterality and language laterality. Those with a left-hand preference were more strongly lateralized on motor tasks requiring midline crossing; this effect was not observed in right-handers. We conclude that there is little evidence of the co-lateralization of language and sequence-based motor skill on this battery.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9381764","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Consistency of limb preference across unimanual feeding, bipedal locomotion, and social grooming in golden snub-nosed monkeys (Rhinopithecus roxellana). 金金丝猴(Rhinopithecus roxellana)单手进食、两足运动和社会梳理中肢体偏好的一致性。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2141251
Weiwei Fu, Jianghui Xu, Xiaowei Wang, Yongbo Li, Shujun He, Chengliang Wang, Yi Ren, Bin Yang, Tong Wu, Yan Wang, Baoguo Li

The golden snub-nosed monkey (Rhinopithecus roxellana) is a typical arboreal group-living Old World primate. While limb preference has been extensively studied in this species, limb preference consistency has not yet been explored. Here, based on 26 R. roxellana adults, we investigated whether individuals exhibit consistent motor biases in manual- (e.g., unimanual feeding and social grooming) and foot-related (e.g., bipedal locomotion) tasks and whether limb preference consistency is influenced by increased social interactions during social grooming. Results showed no consistency in the direction or strength of limb preference among tasks, except for lateral strength in handedness for unimanual feeding and footedness in the initiation of locomotion. Population-level foot preference was only found among right-handers. Marked lateral bias was found in unimanual feeding, indicating that it may be a sensitive behavioural measure for assessing manual preference, especially for provisioned populations. This study not only improves our understanding of the relationship between hand and foot preference in R. roxellana but also reveals potential differential hemispheric regulation of limb preference and the influence of increased social interaction on handedness consistency.

金金丝猴(Rhinopithecus roxellana)是一种典型的东半球群居灵长类动物。虽然该物种的肢体偏好已被广泛研究,但肢体偏好的一致性尚未得到探讨。本文以26只成虫为研究对象,研究了个体在手动(如单手进食和社交梳理)和足部相关(如双足运动)任务中是否表现出一致的运动偏好,以及在社交梳理过程中,肢体偏好的一致性是否受到社会互动增加的影响。结果显示,在不同的任务中,肢体偏好的方向和强度没有一致性,除了在单手进食时的侧向强度和在运动开始时的足性。人口水平的脚偏好只在右撇子中发现。在单人工喂养中发现了明显的侧偏,表明它可能是评估人工偏好的敏感行为措施,特别是对于供应的种群。本研究不仅提高了我们对小红花手足偏好关系的认识,而且揭示了不同半球对肢体偏好的潜在差异调节以及社会交往增加对利手性一致性的影响。
{"title":"Consistency of limb preference across unimanual feeding, bipedal locomotion, and social grooming in golden snub-nosed monkeys (<i>Rhinopithecus roxellana</i>).","authors":"Weiwei Fu,&nbsp;Jianghui Xu,&nbsp;Xiaowei Wang,&nbsp;Yongbo Li,&nbsp;Shujun He,&nbsp;Chengliang Wang,&nbsp;Yi Ren,&nbsp;Bin Yang,&nbsp;Tong Wu,&nbsp;Yan Wang,&nbsp;Baoguo Li","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2022.2141251","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2022.2141251","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The golden snub-nosed monkey (<i>Rhinopithecus roxellana</i>) is a typical arboreal group-living Old World primate. While limb preference has been extensively studied in this species, limb preference consistency has not yet been explored. Here, based on 26 <i>R. roxellana</i> adults, we investigated whether individuals exhibit consistent motor biases in manual- (e.g., unimanual feeding and social grooming) and foot-related (e.g., bipedal locomotion) tasks and whether limb preference consistency is influenced by increased social interactions during social grooming. Results showed no consistency in the direction or strength of limb preference among tasks, except for lateral strength in handedness for unimanual feeding and footedness in the initiation of locomotion. Population-level foot preference was only found among right-handers. Marked lateral bias was found in unimanual feeding, indicating that it may be a sensitive behavioural measure for assessing manual preference, especially for provisioned populations. This study not only improves our understanding of the relationship between hand and foot preference in <i>R. roxellana</i> but also reveals potential differential hemispheric regulation of limb preference and the influence of increased social interaction on handedness consistency.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10823799","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Announcement. 公告。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2140030
{"title":"Announcement.","authors":"","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2022.2140030","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2022.2140030","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10650623","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Righteous Adam, Sinister Eve. 正义的亚当,邪恶的夏娃。
IF 1.4 4区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2022-11-01 DOI: 10.1080/1357650X.2022.2151614
Sergio Della Sala, Robert D McIntosh

The symbolism of laterality in images implies that the virtuous figure is represented on the right of the scene whereas the sinful character is depicted on the left. In portraits of male and female characters this has reflected and reinforced stereotypes and inequalities down the ages. Given these premises, we hypothesized that the prototypical representations of Adam and Eve, as a man and a woman conflated with notions of virtue and vice, would show a non-random arrangement. We tested this hypothesis, sampling artistic depictions of the Garden of Eden, from the twelth century to the present day in three separately-collected series of 100, 99, and 142 images respectively. Eve is depicted to Adam's left significantly more often than chance (between 70% and 83%), particularly in pre-1600 artworks. We interpret this asymmetry as reflecting the perceived lesser status of women in relation to men, since the allegorical incipit of humankind. We also provide experimental evidence that this asymmetry, although pervasive, has not been internalized by modern viewers. Cognitive sciences account for this spatial asymmetry in terms of preference for figures placed within the left visual field of the observer, i.e., within the right space of the objective scene.

图像中侧面的象征意味着,善良的人物在场景的右边,而罪恶的人物在左边。在男性和女性角色的肖像中,这反映并强化了古往今来的刻板印象和不平等。鉴于这些前提,我们假设亚当和夏娃的原型代表,作为一个男人和一个女人,与美德和邪恶的概念相结合,将显示出一种非随机的安排。我们对这一假设进行了检验,从12世纪到现在,我们分别收集了100张、99张和142张图片,对伊甸园的艺术描绘进行了采样。夏娃被描绘在亚当左边的几率要高得多(在70%到83%之间),尤其是在1600年前的艺术品中。我们将这种不对称解释为反映了自人类寓言起源以来,女性相对于男性的地位较低。我们还提供了实验证据,证明这种不对称虽然普遍存在,但并没有被现代观众内化。认知科学对这种空间不对称的解释是,人们偏爱放置在观察者左侧视野内的人物,即在客观场景的右侧空间内。
{"title":"Righteous Adam, Sinister Eve.","authors":"Sergio Della Sala,&nbsp;Robert D McIntosh","doi":"10.1080/1357650X.2022.2151614","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2022.2151614","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The symbolism of laterality in images implies that the virtuous figure is represented on the right of the scene whereas the sinful character is depicted on the left. In portraits of male and female characters this has reflected and reinforced stereotypes and inequalities down the ages. Given these premises, we hypothesized that the prototypical representations of Adam and Eve, as a man and a woman conflated with notions of virtue and vice, would show a non-random arrangement. We tested this hypothesis, sampling artistic depictions of the Garden of Eden, from the twelth century to the present day in three separately-collected series of 100, 99, and 142 images respectively. Eve is depicted to Adam's left significantly more often than chance (between 70% and 83%), particularly in pre-1600 artworks. We interpret this asymmetry as reflecting the perceived lesser status of women in relation to men, since the allegorical incipit of humankind. We also provide experimental evidence that this asymmetry, although pervasive, has not been internalized by modern viewers. Cognitive sciences account for this spatial asymmetry in terms of preference for figures placed within the left visual field of the observer, i.e., within the right space of the objective scene.</p>","PeriodicalId":47387,"journal":{"name":"Laterality","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.4,"publicationDate":"2022-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10662954","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Laterality
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1