首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
The Relation Between War, Starvation, and Fertility Ideals in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Life History Perspective. 撒哈拉以南非洲战争、饥饿与生育理想之间的关系:生命史视角》。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241274622
Matthias Borgstede, Annette Scheunpflug

In this article, we examine the relations between extreme environmental harshness during childhood and personal fertility ideals in African students. The study is informed by biological models of predictive adaptive responses (PAR) for individual reproductive schedules in the context of life history theory (LHT). Following theoretical models of external and internal environmental cues, we tested whether war and starvation during childhood differentially predict African students' personal fertility ideals in terms of their desired number of children and their desired age of first parenthood. The data were collected in eight different countries from sub-Saharan Africa with an overall sample size of N = 392. Standardized effect estimates were obtained using a Bayesian approach. The results suggest that war and starvation are predictive of the desired number of children, but not of the desired age of first parenthood. Moreover, the effect estimates varied considerably between females and males, indicating possible interactions between the two independent variables depending on the students' sex. Furthermore, we found a small negative correlation between the desired number of children and the desired age of first parenthood, providing only weak support for a clustering of the two variables on a slow-fast continuum. The results are discussed in light of current models of individual life histories in humans.

本文研究了非洲学生童年时期极端恶劣的环境与个人生育理想之间的关系。这项研究借鉴了生活史理论(LHT)背景下个体生殖计划预测性适应反应(PAR)的生物学模型。根据外部和内部环境线索的理论模型,我们测试了童年时期的战争和饥饿是否会对非洲学生的个人生育理想产生不同程度的影响,即他们希望生育的子女数量和希望首次生育的年龄。我们在撒哈拉以南非洲的八个不同国家收集了数据,样本总数为 392 人。采用贝叶斯方法获得了标准化效应估计值。结果表明,战争和饥饿可以预测理想的子女数量,但不能预测理想的首次生育年龄。此外,女性和男性之间的效应估计值差异很大,这表明这两个自变量之间可能会因学生的性别而产生相互作用。此外,我们还发现,期望子女数与期望首次生育年龄之间存在微小的负相关,这仅为这两个变量在 "慢-快 "连续统一体上的聚类提供了微弱的支持。我们将根据目前的人类个体生活史模型对这些结果进行讨论。
{"title":"The Relation Between War, Starvation, and Fertility Ideals in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Life History Perspective.","authors":"Matthias Borgstede, Annette Scheunpflug","doi":"10.1177/14747049241274622","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241274622","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this article, we examine the relations between extreme environmental harshness during childhood and personal fertility ideals in African students. The study is informed by biological models of predictive adaptive responses (PAR) for individual reproductive schedules in the context of life history theory (LHT). Following theoretical models of external and internal environmental cues, we tested whether war and starvation during childhood differentially predict African students' personal fertility ideals in terms of their desired number of children and their desired age of first parenthood. The data were collected in eight different countries from sub-Saharan Africa with an overall sample size of <i>N </i>= 392. Standardized effect estimates were obtained using a Bayesian approach. The results suggest that war and starvation are predictive of the desired number of children, but not of the desired age of first parenthood. Moreover, the effect estimates varied considerably between females and males, indicating possible interactions between the two independent variables depending on the students' sex. Furthermore, we found a small negative correlation between the desired number of children and the desired age of first parenthood, providing only weak support for a clustering of the two variables on a slow-fast continuum. The results are discussed in light of current models of individual life histories in humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11475108/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Jealousy as Predicted by Allocation and Reception of Resources in an Economic Game. 通过经济博弈中的资源分配和接收预测嫉妒。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241289232
María Teresa Barbato, Ana María Fernández, Carlos Rodriguez-Sickert, José Antonio Muñoz, Pablo Polo, David Buss

Evidence is abundant that evolution by selection has produced sex differences in the design of adaptations to solve the problems surrounding reproduction. A prime example is the design of human jealousy, which research suggests is triggered by distinct evoking acts that are specific challenges for women and men in their exclusive reproductive bond. It follows that jealousy would be directed toward driving away interlopers who could potentially threaten the bond with the romantic partner or increase mate retention efforts in response to sex-specific threats. To explore this possibility, we use as a methodological innovation an economic game for the evocation of jealousy. With a modified dictator game, we showed men and women in a committed relationship, conditions in which the partner or an intrasexual rival allocates money to (investing condition), or obtains money from (receiving condition), the partner or an opposite sex third party that they recently met. A sample of 56 heterosexual couples (n = 112) participated in a laboratory setting. Our results show the different scenarios of this dictator game exerted the expected evocation of jealousy (controlling individual differences), with women being more jealous by the partner's allocation of resources to a rival, and men reporting slightly more jealousy by their partner receiving money from a rival. We discuss the implications of this method to advance the comprehension of the adaptive function of sex differences in jealousy, the use of economic games, and possible modifications to improve the similarity of the game to a real assessment of actual male jealousy.

大量证据表明,选择性进化在解决生殖问题的适应设计上产生了性别差异。一个典型的例子就是人类嫉妒的设计,研究表明,这种嫉妒是由不同的唤起行为引发的,而这些唤起行为是女性和男性在专一的生殖关系中面临的特定挑战。由此可见,嫉妒的目的是为了赶走那些可能威胁到与浪漫伴侣的关系的入侵者,或者是为了应对特定性别的威胁而增加配偶的保留率。为了探索这种可能性,我们在方法上进行了创新,利用经济游戏来唤起嫉妒。通过一个经过改良的独裁者游戏,我们向处于承诺关系中的男性和女性展示了以下条件:伴侣或性内对手向伴侣或最近认识的异性第三方分配金钱(投资条件),或从伴侣或最近认识的异性第三方那里获得金钱(接受条件)。56 对异性伴侣(n = 112)在实验室环境中参与了这项研究。我们的结果表明,这种独裁者游戏的不同情景都能产生预期的嫉妒(控制个体差异),女性对伴侣将资源分配给竞争对手的嫉妒程度更高,而男性对伴侣从竞争对手处获得金钱的嫉妒程度略高。我们讨论了这种方法的意义,以促进对嫉妒中性别差异的适应功能的理解、经济游戏的使用,以及为提高游戏与实际男性嫉妒的真实评估的相似性而可能进行的修改。
{"title":"Jealousy as Predicted by Allocation and Reception of Resources in an Economic Game.","authors":"María Teresa Barbato, Ana María Fernández, Carlos Rodriguez-Sickert, José Antonio Muñoz, Pablo Polo, David Buss","doi":"10.1177/14747049241289232","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241289232","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Evidence is abundant that evolution by selection has produced sex differences in the design of adaptations to solve the problems surrounding reproduction. A prime example is the design of human jealousy, which research suggests is triggered by distinct evoking acts that are specific challenges for women and men in their exclusive reproductive bond. It follows that jealousy would be directed toward driving away interlopers who could potentially threaten the bond with the romantic partner or increase mate retention efforts in response to sex-specific threats. To explore this possibility, we use as a methodological innovation an economic game for the evocation of jealousy. With a modified dictator game, we showed men and women in a committed relationship, conditions in which the partner or an intrasexual rival allocates money to (investing condition), or obtains money from (receiving condition), the partner or an opposite sex third party that they recently met. A sample of 56 heterosexual couples (<i>n</i> = 112) participated in a laboratory setting. Our results show the different scenarios of this dictator game exerted the expected evocation of jealousy (controlling individual differences), with women being more jealous by the partner's allocation of resources to a rival, and men reporting slightly more jealousy by their partner receiving money from a rival. We discuss the implications of this method to advance the comprehension of the adaptive function of sex differences in jealousy, the use of economic games, and possible modifications to improve the similarity of the game to a real assessment of actual male jealousy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11528616/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142548284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Role of Intrasexual Competition and the Big 5 in the Perpetration of Digital Dating Abuse. 性内竞争和五巨头在实施数字约会虐待中的作用。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241288188
Manpal Singh Bhogal, Morgan Taylor

Recent research has examined digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens, finding people who report intrasexual competitiveness engage in digital dating abuse. Here, we replicated this finding and extended the literature by examining the role of the Big Five personality traits in the perpetration of digital dating abuse, which, to our knowledge, has not been examined in relation to digital dating abuse (n=280). This paper reports findings showing intrasexual competitiveness positively predicts the perpetration of digitaldating abuse; whereby high intrasexual competition is related to high levels of digital dating abuse. Agreeableness was a negative predictor of digital dating abuse; whereby high agreeableness was related to low perpetration of digital dating abuse. Our findings extend the literature exploring digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens.

最近的研究从进化论的角度对数字约会虐待进行了研究,发现那些报告性内竞争的人参与了数字约会虐待。在此,我们复制了这一发现,并通过研究大五人格特质在实施数字约会虐待中的作用扩展了这一文献,据我们所知,还没有人研究过大五人格特质与数字约会虐待的关系(n=280)。本文报告的研究结果表明,性内竞争性对实施数字约会虐待有积极的预测作用;性内竞争性高与数字约会虐待程度高有关。合意度是数字约会虐待的一个负面预测因子;高合意度与低数字约会虐待行为有关。我们的研究结果扩展了从进化角度探讨数字约会虐待的文献。
{"title":"The Role of Intrasexual Competition and the Big 5 in the Perpetration of Digital Dating Abuse.","authors":"Manpal Singh Bhogal, Morgan Taylor","doi":"10.1177/14747049241288188","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241288188","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research has examined digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens, finding people who report intrasexual competitiveness engage in digital dating abuse. Here, we replicated this finding and extended the literature by examining the role of the Big Five personality traits in the perpetration of digital dating abuse, which, to our knowledge, has not been examined in relation to digital dating abuse (<i>n</i>=280). This paper reports findings showing intrasexual competitiveness positively predicts the perpetration of digitaldating abuse; whereby high intrasexual competition is related to high levels of digital dating abuse. Agreeableness was a negative predictor of digital dating abuse; whereby high agreeableness was related to low perpetration of digital dating abuse. Our findings extend the literature exploring digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11468318/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Kindness or Intelligence? Angry Men are Perceived as Less Intelligent by Their Female Romantic Partners. 善良还是聪明?愤怒的男人在女性浪漫伴侣眼中智力较低
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241275706
Jeremiasz Górniak, Marcin Zajenkowski, Kinga Szymaniak, Peter K Jonason

We were interested in how people in a romantic relationship would perceive the intelligence of their partners who have high or low trait anger. Specifically, we referred to the tension between compassion (low anger) and competence (high intelligence) in mate choice. Some evolutionary theories suggest that mating might be considered a bargaining process between these two higher-order attributes. Our study involved 148 heterosexual couples in romantic relationships. We measured the relationship between relationship satisfaction, trait anger, objective intelligence, self-assessed intelligence, and subjectively assessed partners' intelligence. We found that angrier men were less satisfied in their romantic relationship than those men who were less angry, and their partners were also less satisfied in the relationship. Additionally, women perceived angrier men as less intelligent, an effect that remained after controlling for men's objective intelligence. Lastly, we found that women's perception of their partner's intelligence mediated the link between men's anger and relationship satisfaction for both sexes. Our findings suggest that both anger and intelligence play important roles in romantic relationship functioning, consistent with evolutionary theories that emphasize the value of competence (i.e., intelligence) and compassion (i.e., low anger) in romantic partners. Furthermore, our study highlights the importance of women's perception of their partner's intelligence in determining the quality of the relationship.

我们对恋爱关系中的人如何看待特质愤怒程度高或低的伴侣的智力感兴趣。具体来说,我们提到了在择偶过程中同情心(低愤怒)和能力(高智商)之间的矛盾。一些进化理论认为,交配可能是这两种高阶属性之间的讨价还价过程。我们的研究涉及 148 对处于恋爱关系中的异性夫妇。我们测量了关系满意度、特质愤怒、客观智力、自我评估智力和伴侣主观评估智力之间的关系。我们发现,易怒男性在恋爱关系中的满意度低于易怒男性,他们的伴侣在恋爱关系中的满意度也较低。此外,女性认为易怒的男性智力较低,在控制了男性的客观智力后,这种影响依然存在。最后,我们发现女性对其伴侣智力的看法在男性愤怒与两性关系满意度之间起到了中介作用。我们的研究结果表明,愤怒和智力在恋爱关系中都扮演着重要角色,这与强调恋爱伴侣能力(即智力)和同情心(即低愤怒)价值的进化理论是一致的。此外,我们的研究还强调了女性对其伴侣智力的看法在决定恋爱关系质量方面的重要性。
{"title":"Kindness or Intelligence? Angry Men are Perceived as Less Intelligent by Their Female Romantic Partners.","authors":"Jeremiasz Górniak, Marcin Zajenkowski, Kinga Szymaniak, Peter K Jonason","doi":"10.1177/14747049241275706","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241275706","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We were interested in how people in a romantic relationship would perceive the intelligence of their partners who have high or low trait anger. Specifically, we referred to the tension between compassion (low anger) and competence (high intelligence) in mate choice. Some evolutionary theories suggest that mating might be considered a bargaining process between these two higher-order attributes. Our study involved 148 heterosexual couples in romantic relationships. We measured the relationship between relationship satisfaction, trait anger, objective intelligence, self-assessed intelligence, and subjectively assessed partners' intelligence. We found that angrier men were less satisfied in their romantic relationship than those men who were less angry, and their partners were also less satisfied in the relationship. Additionally, women perceived angrier men as less intelligent, an effect that remained after controlling for men's objective intelligence. Lastly, we found that women's perception of their partner's intelligence mediated the link between men's anger and relationship satisfaction for both sexes. Our findings suggest that both anger and intelligence play important roles in romantic relationship functioning, consistent with evolutionary theories that emphasize the value of competence (i.e., intelligence) and compassion (i.e., low anger) in romantic partners. Furthermore, our study highlights the importance of women's perception of their partner's intelligence in determining the quality of the relationship.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11406617/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142126965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Experimental Test of Jealousy's Evolved Function: Imagined Partner Infidelity Induces Jealousy, Which Predicts Positive Attitude Towards Mate Retention. 对嫉妒进化功能的实验测试:假想伴侣不忠会诱发妒忌,而妒忌会预示保留配偶的积极态度。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241267226
Steven Arnocky, Kayla Kubinec, Megan MacKinnon, Dwight Mazmanian

Jealousy may have evolved to motivate adaptive compensatory behavior in response to threats to a valued relationship. This suggests that jealousy follows a temporal sequence: A perceived relational threat induces state feelings of jealousy which in turn motivates compensatory behavior, such as mate retention effort. Yet to date, tests of this mediation model have been limited to cross-sectional data. This study is the first to experimentally test this theoretical model. Men and women (N = 222) who were currently in committed romantic relationships were primed with an imagined partner infidelity (versus control) scenario. Participants then completed measures of state jealousy and intended mate retention behavior. Results found that those primed with the infidelity threat scenario experienced an increase in state jealousy, which in turn predicted more intended benefit-provisioning and cost-inflicting mate retention. Findings suggest that jealousy mediated the relationship between infidelity threat and intended mate retention behavior, supporting the evolutionary account of state jealousy.

嫉妒的进化可能是为了激发适应性补偿行为,以应对有价值关系受到的威胁。这表明嫉妒是有时间顺序的:感知到的关系威胁会引起状态上的嫉妒情绪,进而激发补偿行为,如努力保留配偶。然而,迄今为止,对这一中介模型的测试仅限于横截面数据。本研究首次对这一理论模型进行了实验验证。研究人员向目前处于承诺的恋爱关系中的男性和女性(N = 222)提供了一个想象中伴侣不忠(与对照组相比)的情景。然后,参与者完成了状态嫉妒和意向配偶保留行为的测量。结果发现,那些受到不忠威胁情景引诱的人的状态嫉妒会增加,这反过来又预示着他们会更多地打算提供利益和付出代价来挽留伴侣。研究结果表明,嫉妒在不忠威胁与意向性配偶保留行为之间起到了中介作用,支持了状态嫉妒的进化论。
{"title":"An Experimental Test of Jealousy's Evolved Function: Imagined Partner Infidelity Induces Jealousy, Which Predicts Positive Attitude Towards Mate Retention.","authors":"Steven Arnocky, Kayla Kubinec, Megan MacKinnon, Dwight Mazmanian","doi":"10.1177/14747049241267226","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049241267226","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Jealousy may have evolved to motivate adaptive compensatory behavior in response to threats to a valued relationship. This suggests that jealousy follows a temporal sequence: A perceived relational threat induces state feelings of jealousy which in turn motivates compensatory behavior, such as mate retention effort. Yet to date, tests of this mediation model have been limited to cross-sectional data. This study is the first to experimentally test this theoretical model. Men and women (<i>N </i>= 222) who were currently in committed romantic relationships were primed with an imagined partner infidelity (versus control) scenario. Participants then completed measures of state jealousy and intended mate retention behavior. Results found that those primed with the infidelity threat scenario experienced an increase in state jealousy, which in turn predicted more intended benefit-provisioning and cost-inflicting mate retention. Findings suggest that jealousy mediated the relationship between infidelity threat and intended mate retention behavior, supporting the evolutionary account of state jealousy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363030/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142093977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Red Backgrounds Enhance Dominance in Human Faces and Shapes. 红色背景增强了人脸和形状的主导性。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241284602
Na Chen, Yidie Yang, Maiko Kobayashi, Koyo Nakamura, Katsumi Watanabe

Red color signals dominance in both animals and humans. This study investigated whether a red background color influences the perception of dominance in human faces and geometric shapes. The facial stimuli consisted of computer-generated faces, quantitatively morphed into nine levels of dominance, ranging from less dominant to more dominant. This included East-Asian female faces in Experiment 1 and male faces in Experiment 2. The face stimuli were presented against three background colors: red, green, and gray. Participants were instructed to categorize the faces as either obedient or dominant by pressing the corresponding labeled keys. The results showed that faces were more likely to be perceived as dominant when presented against a red background than against green or gray backgrounds, for both female and male faces. Additionally, two questionnaire surveys showed that the perception of dominance also increased for shapes presented against a red background. However the effect of red diminished in the absence of the actual perception of the color red. These results suggest that the perception of dominance in both human faces and objects is enhanced by the presence of red, possibly due to evolutionary factors related to the perception of red.

红色在动物和人类中都是优势的信号。本研究调查了红色背景是否会影响人脸和几何图形的支配感知。面部刺激包括计算机生成的人脸,这些人脸被定量变形为九个等级的支配力,从支配力较弱到支配力较强不等。实验 1 中包括东亚女性面孔,实验 2 中包括男性面孔。人脸刺激以三种背景颜色呈现:红色、绿色和灰色。参与者被要求按下相应的标签键,将人脸分为顺从型和支配型。结果显示,与绿色或灰色背景相比,红色背景下的女性和男性面孔更容易被认为是支配型的。此外,两项问卷调查显示,在红色背景下呈现的形状也会增加支配感。然而,在没有实际感知红色的情况下,红色的效果会减弱。这些结果表明,人脸和物体的优势感都会因红色的存在而增强,这可能是由于与红色感知有关的进化因素所致。
{"title":"Red Backgrounds Enhance Dominance in Human Faces and Shapes.","authors":"Na Chen, Yidie Yang, Maiko Kobayashi, Koyo Nakamura, Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1177/14747049241284602","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241284602","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Red color signals dominance in both animals and humans. This study investigated whether a red background color influences the perception of dominance in human faces and geometric shapes. The facial stimuli consisted of computer-generated faces, quantitatively morphed into nine levels of dominance, ranging from less dominant to more dominant. This included East-Asian female faces in Experiment 1 and male faces in Experiment 2. The face stimuli were presented against three background colors: red, green, and gray. Participants were instructed to categorize the faces as either obedient or dominant by pressing the corresponding labeled keys. The results showed that faces were more likely to be perceived as dominant when presented against a red background than against green or gray backgrounds, for both female and male faces. Additionally, two questionnaire surveys showed that the perception of dominance also increased for shapes presented against a red background. However the effect of red diminished in the absence of the actual perception of the color red. These results suggest that the perception of dominance in both human faces and objects is enhanced by the presence of red, possibly due to evolutionary factors related to the perception of red.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11440532/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142336926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex Differences in the Etiology of Victimization in Adulthood. 成年后受害原因的性别差异。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241267950
Bridget Joyner-Carpanini, Kevin M Beaver

Until relatively recently, the study of victimization has been largely outside the purview of behavioral geneticists and evolutionary psychologists. Recent victimology research, however, has shown that genetic and evolutionary forces are connected to the risk of victimization. The current study expands on these findings by examining whether genetic influences differentially explain victimization in males and females. To do so, we use a sample of sibling pairs drawn from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; N = 4,244). The analyses revealed no significant quantitative sex differences in the etiology of adult victimization. However, the results of this study do highlight the importance of accounting for genetic factors when studying the etiology of specific types of adult victimization. We conclude by discussing the implications of the current study for future research.

直到最近,对受害情况的研究在很大程度上还不属于行为遗传学家和进化心理学家的研究范围。然而,最近的受害研究表明,遗传和进化的力量与受害风险有关。本研究在这些研究成果的基础上,进一步探讨了遗传因素对男性和女性受害情况的影响是否存在差异。为此,我们使用了从全国青少年到成人健康纵向研究(Add Health; N = 4,244 )中抽取的兄弟姐妹对样本。分析结果显示,在成人受害的病因学方面没有明显的定量性别差异。不过,这项研究的结果确实强调了在研究特定类型的成人受害病因时考虑遗传因素的重要性。最后,我们将讨论本研究对未来研究的影响。
{"title":"Sex Differences in the Etiology of Victimization in Adulthood.","authors":"Bridget Joyner-Carpanini, Kevin M Beaver","doi":"10.1177/14747049241267950","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241267950","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Until relatively recently, the study of victimization has been largely outside the purview of behavioral geneticists and evolutionary psychologists. Recent victimology research, however, has shown that genetic and evolutionary forces are connected to the risk of victimization. The current study expands on these findings by examining whether genetic influences differentially explain victimization in males and females. To do so, we use a sample of sibling pairs drawn from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; <i>N </i>= 4,244). The analyses revealed no significant quantitative sex differences in the etiology of adult victimization. However, the results of this study do highlight the importance of accounting for genetic factors when studying the etiology of specific types of adult victimization. We conclude by discussing the implications of the current study for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11325314/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Autoimmune Hashimoto's Disease and Feminization Level-Testing the Immunocompetence Hypothesis. 自身免疫性桥本氏病与女性化水平--检验免疫能力假说。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241259187
Malwina Goździk, Agnieszka Żelaźniewicz, Judyta Nowak-Kornicka, Katarzyna Pawłowska-Seredyńska, Wioleta Umławska, Bogusław Pawłowski

Morphological femininity depends mainly on estrogen levels at puberty and is perceived as a cue of a woman's biological condition. Due to the immunostimulant properties of estradiol, estradiol-dependent feminine traits are expected to be positively related to immunity. However, heightened immunity in women may increase the risk of autoimmune disease, thus the relationship between femininity and immune quality may be complex. This study aimed to assess the relationship between morphological femininity and both the occurrence and severity of Hashimoto thyroiditis (HT) in women of reproductive age. Moreover, 95 women with HT and 84 without HT (all between 20 and 37 years) participated in the study. Morphological femininity was assessed based on somatic measurements of sexually dimorphic traits (2D:4D ratio, WHR, breast size, facial sexual dimorphism). The occurrence and severity of HT were assessed by serum TPOAb levels. The results showed that only the 2D:4D ratio of the right hand was higher in the HT group, indicating higher femininity in these women. However, there was also a positive relationship between facial femininity and TPOAb level in women with HT, indicating a higher severity of the disease. The results suggest that prenatal and pubertal exposure to estrogens may increase the probability or severity of autoimmune diseases in adulthood, but the relationship is tentative.

形态上的女性特质主要取决于青春期的雌激素水平,并被视为女性生理状况的线索。由于雌二醇具有免疫刺激的特性,依赖雌二醇的女性特质预计与免疫力呈正相关。然而,女性免疫力的提高可能会增加患自身免疫性疾病的风险,因此女性特质与免疫质量之间的关系可能很复杂。本研究旨在评估育龄妇女的形态女性特质与桥本氏甲状腺炎(HT)的发生和严重程度之间的关系。此外,95名患有桥本氏甲状腺炎的女性和84名未患有桥本氏甲状腺炎的女性(年龄均在20至37岁之间)参与了这项研究。根据对性别二形特征(2D:4D 比率、WHR、乳房大小、面部性别二形性)的体格测量结果,对形态女性化进行了评估。通过血清 TPOAb 水平评估 HT 的发生和严重程度。结果显示,HT 组中只有右手的 2D:4D 比例较高,表明这些女性的女性化程度较高。然而,HT 女性患者的面部女性特质与 TPOAb 水平之间也存在正相关关系,这表明该疾病的严重程度更高。结果表明,产前和青春期暴露于雌激素可能会增加成年后患自身免疫性疾病的概率或严重程度,但这种关系是暂时的。
{"title":"Autoimmune Hashimoto's Disease and Feminization Level-Testing <i>the Immunocompetence Hypothesis</i>.","authors":"Malwina Goździk, Agnieszka Żelaźniewicz, Judyta Nowak-Kornicka, Katarzyna Pawłowska-Seredyńska, Wioleta Umławska, Bogusław Pawłowski","doi":"10.1177/14747049241259187","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241259187","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Morphological femininity depends mainly on estrogen levels at puberty and is perceived as a cue of a woman's biological condition. Due to the immunostimulant properties of estradiol, estradiol-dependent feminine traits are expected to be positively related to immunity. However, heightened immunity in women may increase the risk of autoimmune disease, thus the relationship between femininity and immune quality may be complex. This study aimed to assess the relationship between morphological femininity and both the occurrence and severity of Hashimoto thyroiditis (HT) in women of reproductive age. Moreover, 95 women with HT and 84 without HT (all between 20 and 37 years) participated in the study. Morphological femininity was assessed based on somatic measurements of sexually dimorphic traits (2D:4D ratio, WHR, breast size, facial sexual dimorphism). The occurrence and severity of HT were assessed by serum TPOAb levels. The results showed that only the 2D:4D ratio of the right hand was higher in the HT group, indicating higher femininity in these women. However, there was also a positive relationship between facial femininity and TPOAb level in women with HT, indicating a higher severity of the disease. The results suggest that prenatal and pubertal exposure to estrogens may increase the probability or severity of autoimmune diseases in adulthood, but the relationship is tentative.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11378202/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Paternal Filicide in Sweden: Background, Risk Factors and the Cinderella Effect. 瑞典的弑父事件:背景、风险因素和灰姑娘效应。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241265623
Hans Temrin

An overrepresentation of stepchildren as victims of filicide has been explained as a consequence of 'discriminative parental solicitude'. The idea being that Darwinian selection has favoured parental love and concern only for biological children, and when such parental feelings are absent, as in stepparents, conflicts with a child could easier escalate to lethal violence. An alternative explanation for this overrepresentation of stepchildren is that risk factors for filicide, such as criminal behaviour and mental health problems, are more prevalent in stepparents. This study focused on paternal filicide in Sweden and investigated (i) if stepchildren are overrepresented as victims of filicide compared with biological children, (ii) if filicides are committed in a context that implies a 'conflict with the child victim' and (iii) if stepfathers and biological fathers differ in characteristics associated with filicide risk. The analyses showed that stepchildren were overrepresented as victims compared with children of fathers in families with two biological parents and this overrepresentation was even higher in young children. Children of single biological fathers and children of non-residential biological fathers were also overrepresented as victims of filicide. Less than 20 percent of the filicides were committed in the context of a 'conflict with the child' and in these cases only stepchildren were overrepresented as victims. In the population at large, both stepfathers and single biological fathers had higher rates of mental health problems, violent criminality and illegal possession of drugs compared with fathers in families with two biological parents.

继子女成为杀父仇母行为受害者的比例过高,被解释为 "歧视性父母关爱 "的结果。这种观点认为,达尔文的选择倾向于只对亲生子女给予父母的爱和关心,而当这种父母情感缺失时,比如继父母,与子女的冲突就很容易升级为致命的暴力。继子女比例过高的另一种解释是,继父母中的犯罪行为和心理健康问题等杀父杀母的风险因素更为普遍。本研究侧重于瑞典的父亲杀婴案,并调查了(i)继子女与亲生子女相比是否成为杀婴案受害者的比例过高,(ii)杀婴案是否在意味着 "与受害儿童发生冲突 "的情况下发生,以及(iii)继父与亲生父亲在与杀婴风险相关的特征方面是否存在差异。分析表明,与生父为双亲的家庭中的子女相比,继子女成为受害者的比例过高,这种比例过高的情况在幼儿中更为严重。单亲生父的子女和非寄宿生父的子女也是过失杀人的受害者。不到 20%的弑父案件是在 "与子女发生冲突 "的情况下发生的,在这些案件中,只有继子女的受害者比例过高。在整个人口中,继父和单亲父亲的精神健康问题、暴力犯罪和非法持有毒品的比例都高于双亲家庭中的父亲。
{"title":"Paternal Filicide in Sweden: Background, Risk Factors and the Cinderella Effect.","authors":"Hans Temrin","doi":"10.1177/14747049241265623","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241265623","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An overrepresentation of stepchildren as victims of filicide has been explained as a consequence of 'discriminative parental solicitude'. The idea being that Darwinian selection has favoured parental love and concern only for biological children, and when such parental feelings are absent, as in stepparents, conflicts with a child could easier escalate to lethal violence. An alternative explanation for this overrepresentation of stepchildren is that risk factors for filicide, such as criminal behaviour and mental health problems, are more prevalent in stepparents. This study focused on paternal filicide in Sweden and investigated (i) if stepchildren are overrepresented as victims of filicide compared with biological children, (ii) if filicides are committed in a context that implies a 'conflict with the child victim' and (iii) if stepfathers and biological fathers differ in characteristics associated with filicide risk. The analyses showed that stepchildren were overrepresented as victims compared with children of fathers in families with two biological parents and this overrepresentation was even higher in young children. Children of single biological fathers and children of non-residential biological fathers were also overrepresented as victims of filicide. Less than 20 percent of the filicides were committed in the context of a 'conflict with the child' and in these cases only stepchildren were overrepresented as victims. In the population at large, both stepfathers and single biological fathers had higher rates of mental health problems, violent criminality and illegal possession of drugs compared with fathers in families with two biological parents.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11359440/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142082184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment. 国际跨时空教育成就 "畸配 "元分析》(International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment)。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241249072
Sebastian Jensen, Emil Kirkegaard

Previous studies have found a high degree of assortative mating for educational attainment (r = .56). However, this can be confounded by cohort effects or country effects, where certain nations may have more pronounced assortative mating than others. In addition, method variance regarding how educational attainment is measured may also result in heterogeneity of effect sizes. Effect sizes were gathered from various datasets and from academic literature, resulting in a large collection of effect sizes (k = 1498, n = 9,159,098), spanning 84 different countries. Assortative mating for educational attainment was stronger than what previous literature suggested (r = .66, [.64, .68]), largely due to the fact that assortative mating for educational attainment is stronger when latent methods are used. The strongest predictors of assortative mating for education between countries were individualism (r = -.61, p < .001) and HDI (r = -.56, p < .001). Assortative mating over time was found to vary by region. Capitalist Europe experienced an increase in assortative mating for education, while Communist Europe experienced a decrease. The United States had a non-linear trend in assortative mating for educational attainment, as it decreased from 1875 to 1926, increased from 1926 to 1945, decreased from 1945 to 1958, increased from 1958 to 1977, and decreased from 1977 onwards.

以往的研究发现,教育程度的同配程度很高(r = .56)。然而,这可能会受到队列效应或国家效应的影响,某些国家的同配现象可能比其他国家更为明显。此外,教育程度测量方法的差异也可能导致效应大小的异质性。我们从不同的数据集和学术文献中收集了效应大小,从而收集了大量效应大小(k = 1498,n = 9,159,098),涉及 84 个不同的国家。教育程度方面的同配现象比以往的文献所指出的要强(r = .66,[.64, .68]),这主要是由于在使用潜在方法时,教育程度方面的同配现象更强。国家间教育同配的最强预测因素是个人主义(r = -.61,p
{"title":"International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment.","authors":"Sebastian Jensen, Emil Kirkegaard","doi":"10.1177/14747049241249072","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241249072","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have found a high degree of assortative mating for educational attainment (r = .56). However, this can be confounded by cohort effects or country effects, where certain nations may have more pronounced assortative mating than others. In addition, method variance regarding how educational attainment is measured may also result in heterogeneity of effect sizes. Effect sizes were gathered from various datasets and from academic literature, resulting in a large collection of effect sizes (k = 1498, n = 9,159,098), spanning 84 different countries. Assortative mating for educational attainment was stronger than what previous literature suggested (r = .66, [.64, .68]), largely due to the fact that assortative mating for educational attainment is stronger when latent methods are used. The strongest predictors of assortative mating for education between countries were individualism (r = -.61, p < .001) and HDI (r = -.56, p < .001). Assortative mating over time was found to vary by region. Capitalist Europe experienced an increase in assortative mating for education, while Communist Europe experienced a decrease. The United States had a non-linear trend in assortative mating for educational attainment, as it decreased from 1875 to 1926, increased from 1926 to 1945, decreased from 1945 to 1958, increased from 1958 to 1977, and decreased from 1977 onwards.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11146041/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolutionary Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1