首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
How People React to the Termination of an Intimate Relationship: An Exploratory Mixed-Methods Study. 人们对亲密关系终止的反应:一项探索性混合方法研究。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241312231
Menelaos Apostolou, Isaias Taliadoros, Timo Juhani Lajunen

Intimate relationships frequently come to an end, and in the current research, we have endeavored to examine how individuals would potentially react in the scenario where their intimate partner decides to terminate a relationship they wish to continue. More specifically, employing open-ended questionnaires on a sample of 219 Greek-speaking participants, we identified 79 possible reactions. Subsequently, using close-ended questionnaires on a sample of 442 Greek-speaking participants, we categorized these reactions into 13 broad factors. Participants indicated that they were more likely to feel sadness, inquire of their departing partners why they wish to end the relationship, and attempt to divert their thoughts elsewhere to avoid dwelling on the end of the relationship. Men indicated a higher likelihood than women to seek revenge sex, although significant sex differences were not observed in other reactions. Furthermore, we classified these 13 factors into three broader domains. The highest-rated domain was "Accept and forget," followed by "Sadness and depression," and "Physical and psychological aggression." These findings could enable us to gain a better understanding of the process of relationship dissolution, and could potentially be employed to identify and prevent reactions that may have harmful repercussions for the individuals involved in the relationship.

亲密关系经常会结束,在当前的研究中,我们努力研究个人在亲密伴侣决定终止他们希望继续的关系时的潜在反应。更具体地说,我们对219名讲希腊语的参与者采用开放式问卷调查,确定了79种可能的反应。随后,我们对442名说希腊语的参与者使用封闭式问卷,将这些反应分为13个主要因素。参与者表示,他们更有可能感到悲伤,询问即将离开的伴侣为什么希望结束这段关系,并试图转移注意力,避免老是想着这段关系的结束。男性比女性更有可能寻求报复性性行为,尽管在其他反应中没有观察到明显的性别差异。此外,我们将这13个因素分为三个更广泛的领域。评分最高的领域是“接受和遗忘”,其次是“悲伤和抑郁”,以及“身体和心理攻击”。这些发现可以使我们更好地理解关系破裂的过程,并可能被用来识别和预防可能对关系中的个人产生有害影响的反应。
{"title":"How People React to the Termination of an Intimate Relationship: An Exploratory Mixed-Methods Study.","authors":"Menelaos Apostolou, Isaias Taliadoros, Timo Juhani Lajunen","doi":"10.1177/14747049241312231","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241312231","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Intimate relationships frequently come to an end, and in the current research, we have endeavored to examine how individuals would potentially react in the scenario where their intimate partner decides to terminate a relationship they wish to continue. More specifically, employing open-ended questionnaires on a sample of 219 Greek-speaking participants, we identified 79 possible reactions. Subsequently, using close-ended questionnaires on a sample of 442 Greek-speaking participants, we categorized these reactions into 13 broad factors. Participants indicated that they were more likely to feel sadness, inquire of their departing partners why they wish to end the relationship, and attempt to divert their thoughts elsewhere to avoid dwelling on the end of the relationship. Men indicated a higher likelihood than women to seek revenge sex, although significant sex differences were not observed in other reactions. Furthermore, we classified these 13 factors into three broader domains. The highest-rated domain was \"Accept and forget,\" followed by \"Sadness and depression,\" and \"Physical and psychological aggression.\" These findings could enable us to gain a better understanding of the process of relationship dissolution, and could potentially be employed to identify and prevent reactions that may have harmful repercussions for the individuals involved in the relationship.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"23 1","pages":"14747049241312231"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11726518/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142972670","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sex Differences in Perception of Economic and Dating Access.
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241310154
Rachel E Hall, Khandis Blake, Ho Fai Chan, Benno Torgler, Stephen Whyte

Mating and labor markets are fundamental drivers of societal dynamics. Yet, equity of access to these domains differs between the sexes due to numerous biological, economic, psychological, and socio-cultural factors. These inequalities and their impacts can accentuate perceptions, preferences, and behaviors of males and females in different ways. Utilizing a large cross-sectional sample of those currently engaged in the Australian mating market (n = 1072 online daters), we explored the impact of sex and individual differences on the perceptions of men's ease of access to a decent job in the labor market (opportunity), women's economic dependence on men (economic inequality), and relative reproductive opportunity (dating access) for both sexes. Our study identifies both sex differences and symmetries in socio-economic factors (such as education level and having offspring) correlating with the perceptions of both economic and dating market access for Australian online daters. Additionally, key resource endowment indicators (income and unemployment) also reflect differences in both sexes' perceptions of both access and gender equity. That said, our study finds that perceptions of access to both (economic and mating) markets shows far greater variation based on biological age (especially for women) than any other factor.

{"title":"Sex Differences in Perception of Economic and Dating Access.","authors":"Rachel E Hall, Khandis Blake, Ho Fai Chan, Benno Torgler, Stephen Whyte","doi":"10.1177/14747049241310154","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241310154","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Mating and labor markets are fundamental drivers of societal dynamics. Yet, equity of access to these domains differs between the sexes due to numerous biological, economic, psychological, and socio-cultural factors. These inequalities and their impacts can accentuate perceptions, preferences, and behaviors of males and females in different ways. Utilizing a large cross-sectional sample of those currently engaged in the Australian mating market (<i>n </i>= 1072 online daters), we explored the impact of sex and individual differences on the perceptions of men's ease of access to a decent job in the labor market (opportunity), women's economic dependence on men (economic inequality), and relative reproductive opportunity (dating access) for both sexes. Our study identifies both sex differences and symmetries in socio-economic factors (such as education level and having offspring) correlating with the perceptions of both economic and dating market access for Australian online daters. Additionally, key resource endowment indicators (income and unemployment) also reflect differences in both sexes' perceptions of both access and gender equity. That said, our study finds that perceptions of access to both (economic and mating) markets shows far greater variation based on biological age (especially for women) than any other factor.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"23 1","pages":"14747049241310154"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11808775/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143383636","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Pace of Life Is Faster for a Bored Person: Exploring the Relationship Between Trait Boredom and Fast Life History Strategy. 无聊的人的生活节奏更快:探索特质无聊与快速生活史策略之间的关系。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241310772
Garam Kim, Eunsoo Choi

Life history theory suggests that in harsh, unpredictable environments, individuals may benefit from adopting a fast life history strategy. This may involve experiencing boredom more frequently and intensely as an adaptive mechanism to seek novel stimuli, potentially increasing the number of sexual partners and offspring. This study explored the relationship between trait boredom-a chronic characteristic of feeling bored-and fast life history strategies. Our findings confirmed a positive association between boredom proneness and fast life history strategies at both individual and country levels. In Study 1, fast life history strategy was positively correlated with boredom proneness and mediated the relationship between perceived childhood support/resources and boredom proneness. In Study 2, we conducted a multi-level analysis using secondary data from 15 countries. The results showed that countries with higher boredom proneness scores showed more indicators of faster life history strategies. These results suggest that trait boredom may be a functional characteristic of fast life history strategists. This study is the first empirical investigation of trait boredom within a life history framework, highlighting trait boredom's functional role from evolutionary and ecological perspectives.

生活史理论认为,在严酷、不可预测的环境中,个体可能会从采用快速生活史策略中受益。这可能包括更频繁和强烈地体验无聊,作为一种寻求新刺激的适应机制,潜在地增加性伴侣和后代的数量。本研究探讨了无聊感这一慢性特征与快速生活史策略之间的关系。我们的研究结果证实,在个人和国家层面上,无聊倾向和快速生活史策略之间存在正相关。研究1中,快速生活史策略与无聊倾向正相关,并在童年支持/资源感知与无聊倾向之间起中介作用。在研究2中,我们使用来自15个国家的二手数据进行了多层次分析。结果显示,无聊倾向得分越高的国家,其生活史策略的指标越快。这些结果表明,无聊特质可能是快生活史战略家的一个功能特征。本研究首次在生命史框架下对特质无聊进行了实证研究,从进化和生态的角度强调了特质无聊的功能作用。
{"title":"Pace of Life Is Faster for a Bored Person: Exploring the Relationship Between Trait Boredom and Fast Life History Strategy.","authors":"Garam Kim, Eunsoo Choi","doi":"10.1177/14747049241310772","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241310772","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Life history theory suggests that in harsh, unpredictable environments, individuals may benefit from adopting a fast life history strategy. This may involve experiencing boredom more frequently and intensely as an adaptive mechanism to seek novel stimuli, potentially increasing the number of sexual partners and offspring. This study explored the relationship between trait boredom-a chronic characteristic of feeling bored-and fast life history strategies. Our findings confirmed a positive association between boredom proneness and fast life history strategies at both individual and country levels. In Study 1, fast life history strategy was positively correlated with boredom proneness and mediated the relationship between perceived childhood support/resources and boredom proneness. In Study 2, we conducted a multi-level analysis using secondary data from 15 countries. The results showed that countries with higher boredom proneness scores showed more indicators of faster life history strategies. These results suggest that trait boredom may be a functional characteristic of fast life history strategists. This study is the first empirical investigation of trait boredom within a life history framework, highlighting trait boredom's functional role from evolutionary and ecological perspectives.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"23 1","pages":"14747049241310772"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11748154/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014050","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Relation Between War, Starvation, and Fertility Ideals in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Life History Perspective. 撒哈拉以南非洲战争、饥饿与生育理想之间的关系:生命史视角》。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241274622
Matthias Borgstede, Annette Scheunpflug

In this article, we examine the relations between extreme environmental harshness during childhood and personal fertility ideals in African students. The study is informed by biological models of predictive adaptive responses (PAR) for individual reproductive schedules in the context of life history theory (LHT). Following theoretical models of external and internal environmental cues, we tested whether war and starvation during childhood differentially predict African students' personal fertility ideals in terms of their desired number of children and their desired age of first parenthood. The data were collected in eight different countries from sub-Saharan Africa with an overall sample size of N = 392. Standardized effect estimates were obtained using a Bayesian approach. The results suggest that war and starvation are predictive of the desired number of children, but not of the desired age of first parenthood. Moreover, the effect estimates varied considerably between females and males, indicating possible interactions between the two independent variables depending on the students' sex. Furthermore, we found a small negative correlation between the desired number of children and the desired age of first parenthood, providing only weak support for a clustering of the two variables on a slow-fast continuum. The results are discussed in light of current models of individual life histories in humans.

本文研究了非洲学生童年时期极端恶劣的环境与个人生育理想之间的关系。这项研究借鉴了生活史理论(LHT)背景下个体生殖计划预测性适应反应(PAR)的生物学模型。根据外部和内部环境线索的理论模型,我们测试了童年时期的战争和饥饿是否会对非洲学生的个人生育理想产生不同程度的影响,即他们希望生育的子女数量和希望首次生育的年龄。我们在撒哈拉以南非洲的八个不同国家收集了数据,样本总数为 392 人。采用贝叶斯方法获得了标准化效应估计值。结果表明,战争和饥饿可以预测理想的子女数量,但不能预测理想的首次生育年龄。此外,女性和男性之间的效应估计值差异很大,这表明这两个自变量之间可能会因学生的性别而产生相互作用。此外,我们还发现,期望子女数与期望首次生育年龄之间存在微小的负相关,这仅为这两个变量在 "慢-快 "连续统一体上的聚类提供了微弱的支持。我们将根据目前的人类个体生活史模型对这些结果进行讨论。
{"title":"The Relation Between War, Starvation, and Fertility Ideals in Sub-Saharan Africa: A Life History Perspective.","authors":"Matthias Borgstede, Annette Scheunpflug","doi":"10.1177/14747049241274622","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241274622","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In this article, we examine the relations between extreme environmental harshness during childhood and personal fertility ideals in African students. The study is informed by biological models of predictive adaptive responses (PAR) for individual reproductive schedules in the context of life history theory (LHT). Following theoretical models of external and internal environmental cues, we tested whether war and starvation during childhood differentially predict African students' personal fertility ideals in terms of their desired number of children and their desired age of first parenthood. The data were collected in eight different countries from sub-Saharan Africa with an overall sample size of <i>N </i>= 392. Standardized effect estimates were obtained using a Bayesian approach. The results suggest that war and starvation are predictive of the desired number of children, but not of the desired age of first parenthood. Moreover, the effect estimates varied considerably between females and males, indicating possible interactions between the two independent variables depending on the students' sex. Furthermore, we found a small negative correlation between the desired number of children and the desired age of first parenthood, providing only weak support for a clustering of the two variables on a slow-fast continuum. The results are discussed in light of current models of individual life histories in humans.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 4","pages":"14747049241274622"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11475108/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142401518","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Jealousy as Predicted by Allocation and Reception of Resources in an Economic Game. 通过经济博弈中的资源分配和接收预测嫉妒。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241289232
María Teresa Barbato, Ana María Fernández, Carlos Rodriguez-Sickert, José Antonio Muñoz, Pablo Polo, David Buss

Evidence is abundant that evolution by selection has produced sex differences in the design of adaptations to solve the problems surrounding reproduction. A prime example is the design of human jealousy, which research suggests is triggered by distinct evoking acts that are specific challenges for women and men in their exclusive reproductive bond. It follows that jealousy would be directed toward driving away interlopers who could potentially threaten the bond with the romantic partner or increase mate retention efforts in response to sex-specific threats. To explore this possibility, we use as a methodological innovation an economic game for the evocation of jealousy. With a modified dictator game, we showed men and women in a committed relationship, conditions in which the partner or an intrasexual rival allocates money to (investing condition), or obtains money from (receiving condition), the partner or an opposite sex third party that they recently met. A sample of 56 heterosexual couples (n = 112) participated in a laboratory setting. Our results show the different scenarios of this dictator game exerted the expected evocation of jealousy (controlling individual differences), with women being more jealous by the partner's allocation of resources to a rival, and men reporting slightly more jealousy by their partner receiving money from a rival. We discuss the implications of this method to advance the comprehension of the adaptive function of sex differences in jealousy, the use of economic games, and possible modifications to improve the similarity of the game to a real assessment of actual male jealousy.

大量证据表明,选择性进化在解决生殖问题的适应设计上产生了性别差异。一个典型的例子就是人类嫉妒的设计,研究表明,这种嫉妒是由不同的唤起行为引发的,而这些唤起行为是女性和男性在专一的生殖关系中面临的特定挑战。由此可见,嫉妒的目的是为了赶走那些可能威胁到与浪漫伴侣的关系的入侵者,或者是为了应对特定性别的威胁而增加配偶的保留率。为了探索这种可能性,我们在方法上进行了创新,利用经济游戏来唤起嫉妒。通过一个经过改良的独裁者游戏,我们向处于承诺关系中的男性和女性展示了以下条件:伴侣或性内对手向伴侣或最近认识的异性第三方分配金钱(投资条件),或从伴侣或最近认识的异性第三方那里获得金钱(接受条件)。56 对异性伴侣(n = 112)在实验室环境中参与了这项研究。我们的结果表明,这种独裁者游戏的不同情景都能产生预期的嫉妒(控制个体差异),女性对伴侣将资源分配给竞争对手的嫉妒程度更高,而男性对伴侣从竞争对手处获得金钱的嫉妒程度略高。我们讨论了这种方法的意义,以促进对嫉妒中性别差异的适应功能的理解、经济游戏的使用,以及为提高游戏与实际男性嫉妒的真实评估的相似性而可能进行的修改。
{"title":"Jealousy as Predicted by Allocation and Reception of Resources in an Economic Game.","authors":"María Teresa Barbato, Ana María Fernández, Carlos Rodriguez-Sickert, José Antonio Muñoz, Pablo Polo, David Buss","doi":"10.1177/14747049241289232","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241289232","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Evidence is abundant that evolution by selection has produced sex differences in the design of adaptations to solve the problems surrounding reproduction. A prime example is the design of human jealousy, which research suggests is triggered by distinct evoking acts that are specific challenges for women and men in their exclusive reproductive bond. It follows that jealousy would be directed toward driving away interlopers who could potentially threaten the bond with the romantic partner or increase mate retention efforts in response to sex-specific threats. To explore this possibility, we use as a methodological innovation an economic game for the evocation of jealousy. With a modified dictator game, we showed men and women in a committed relationship, conditions in which the partner or an intrasexual rival allocates money to (investing condition), or obtains money from (receiving condition), the partner or an opposite sex third party that they recently met. A sample of 56 heterosexual couples (<i>n</i> = 112) participated in a laboratory setting. Our results show the different scenarios of this dictator game exerted the expected evocation of jealousy (controlling individual differences), with women being more jealous by the partner's allocation of resources to a rival, and men reporting slightly more jealousy by their partner receiving money from a rival. We discuss the implications of this method to advance the comprehension of the adaptive function of sex differences in jealousy, the use of economic games, and possible modifications to improve the similarity of the game to a real assessment of actual male jealousy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 4","pages":"14747049241289232"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11528616/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142548284","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When to Blame Victims for Negligence: Noncooperators Are Deemed Responsible for Their Own Hardship. 何时将过失归咎于受害者?不合作者被认为应对自己的困难负责。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241297902
Pascal Boyer, Eric Chantland, Lou Safra

In four preregistered studies, we tested implications from a cooperation model that explains victim-blaming as a strategic move, as a way for people to avoid the costs of helping victims (who seem to be unpromising cooperation partners) without paying the reputational cost of being seen as ungenerous, reluctant cooperators. An implication of this perspective is that, if an individual is identified as a poor cooperation prospect to start with, people would be likely to blame that individual for his/her own misfortune, notably by suggesting that the victim was negligent. The four studies presented here support this interpretation, as participants attributed more negligence to an accident victim if that victim had been initially described as less prosocial, either because they denied benefits to others or because they created costs for others. These results are consistent with a familiar result, that people blame victims more if they feel (or want to be seen as) more socially distant from that victim. The present studies may offer a simple, cooperation-based account of this and other aspects of victim-blaming.

在四项预先登记的研究中,我们检验了一个合作模型的含义,该模型将指责受害者解释为一种策略性举动,是人们避免帮助受害者(他们似乎是不具合作前景的伙伴)的成本的一种方式,而无需付出被视为不慷慨、不情愿的合作者的声誉成本。这种观点的一个含义是,如果一个人一开始就被认定为合作前景不佳,那么人们很可能会把自己的不幸归咎于这个人,特别是暗示受害者疏忽大意。本文介绍的四项研究支持这一解释,因为如果事故受害者最初被描述为亲社会性较差,那么参与者就会将更多的过失归咎于该受害者,因为他们剥夺了他人的利益,或者因为他们给他人造成了损失。这些结果与我们熟悉的结果一致,即如果人们觉得(或希望被视为)自己与受害者的社会关系更疏远,他们就会更多地指责受害者。目前的研究可能提供了一种简单的、基于合作的方法来解释这种情况以及指责受害者的其他方面。
{"title":"When to Blame Victims for Negligence: Noncooperators Are Deemed Responsible for Their Own Hardship.","authors":"Pascal Boyer, Eric Chantland, Lou Safra","doi":"10.1177/14747049241297902","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241297902","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In four preregistered studies, we tested implications from a cooperation model that explains victim-blaming as a strategic move, as a way for people to avoid the costs of helping victims (who seem to be unpromising cooperation partners) without paying the reputational cost of being seen as ungenerous, reluctant cooperators. An implication of this perspective is that, if an individual is identified as a poor cooperation prospect to start with, people would be likely to blame that individual for his/her own misfortune, notably by suggesting that the victim was negligent. The four studies presented here support this interpretation, as participants attributed more negligence to an accident victim if that victim had been initially described as less prosocial, either because they denied benefits to others or because they created costs for others. These results are consistent with a familiar result, that people blame victims more if they feel (or want to be seen as) more socially distant from that victim. The present studies may offer a simple, cooperation-based account of this and other aspects of victim-blaming.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 4","pages":"14747049241297902"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11590129/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142717473","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Role of Intrasexual Competition and the Big 5 in the Perpetration of Digital Dating Abuse. 性内竞争和五巨头在实施数字约会虐待中的作用。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-10-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241288188
Manpal Singh Bhogal, Morgan Taylor

Recent research has examined digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens, finding people who report intrasexual competitiveness engage in digital dating abuse. Here, we replicated this finding and extended the literature by examining the role of the Big Five personality traits in the perpetration of digital dating abuse, which, to our knowledge, has not been examined in relation to digital dating abuse (n=280). This paper reports findings showing intrasexual competitiveness positively predicts the perpetration of digitaldating abuse; whereby high intrasexual competition is related to high levels of digital dating abuse. Agreeableness was a negative predictor of digital dating abuse; whereby high agreeableness was related to low perpetration of digital dating abuse. Our findings extend the literature exploring digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens.

最近的研究从进化论的角度对数字约会虐待进行了研究,发现那些报告性内竞争的人参与了数字约会虐待。在此,我们复制了这一发现,并通过研究大五人格特质在实施数字约会虐待中的作用扩展了这一文献,据我们所知,还没有人研究过大五人格特质与数字约会虐待的关系(n=280)。本文报告的研究结果表明,性内竞争性对实施数字约会虐待有积极的预测作用;性内竞争性高与数字约会虐待程度高有关。合意度是数字约会虐待的一个负面预测因子;高合意度与低数字约会虐待行为有关。我们的研究结果扩展了从进化角度探讨数字约会虐待的文献。
{"title":"The Role of Intrasexual Competition and the Big 5 in the Perpetration of Digital Dating Abuse.","authors":"Manpal Singh Bhogal, Morgan Taylor","doi":"10.1177/14747049241288188","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241288188","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent research has examined digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens, finding people who report intrasexual competitiveness engage in digital dating abuse. Here, we replicated this finding and extended the literature by examining the role of the Big Five personality traits in the perpetration of digital dating abuse, which, to our knowledge, has not been examined in relation to digital dating abuse (<i>n</i>=280). This paper reports findings showing intrasexual competitiveness positively predicts the perpetration of digitaldating abuse; whereby high intrasexual competition is related to high levels of digital dating abuse. Agreeableness was a negative predictor of digital dating abuse; whereby high agreeableness was related to low perpetration of digital dating abuse. Our findings extend the literature exploring digital dating abuse through an evolutionary lens.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 4","pages":"14747049241288188"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-10-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11468318/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142394250","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Kindness or Intelligence? Angry Men are Perceived as Less Intelligent by Their Female Romantic Partners. 善良还是聪明?愤怒的男人在女性浪漫伴侣眼中智力较低
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241275706
Jeremiasz Górniak, Marcin Zajenkowski, Kinga Szymaniak, Peter K Jonason

We were interested in how people in a romantic relationship would perceive the intelligence of their partners who have high or low trait anger. Specifically, we referred to the tension between compassion (low anger) and competence (high intelligence) in mate choice. Some evolutionary theories suggest that mating might be considered a bargaining process between these two higher-order attributes. Our study involved 148 heterosexual couples in romantic relationships. We measured the relationship between relationship satisfaction, trait anger, objective intelligence, self-assessed intelligence, and subjectively assessed partners' intelligence. We found that angrier men were less satisfied in their romantic relationship than those men who were less angry, and their partners were also less satisfied in the relationship. Additionally, women perceived angrier men as less intelligent, an effect that remained after controlling for men's objective intelligence. Lastly, we found that women's perception of their partner's intelligence mediated the link between men's anger and relationship satisfaction for both sexes. Our findings suggest that both anger and intelligence play important roles in romantic relationship functioning, consistent with evolutionary theories that emphasize the value of competence (i.e., intelligence) and compassion (i.e., low anger) in romantic partners. Furthermore, our study highlights the importance of women's perception of their partner's intelligence in determining the quality of the relationship.

我们对恋爱关系中的人如何看待特质愤怒程度高或低的伴侣的智力感兴趣。具体来说,我们提到了在择偶过程中同情心(低愤怒)和能力(高智商)之间的矛盾。一些进化理论认为,交配可能是这两种高阶属性之间的讨价还价过程。我们的研究涉及 148 对处于恋爱关系中的异性夫妇。我们测量了关系满意度、特质愤怒、客观智力、自我评估智力和伴侣主观评估智力之间的关系。我们发现,易怒男性在恋爱关系中的满意度低于易怒男性,他们的伴侣在恋爱关系中的满意度也较低。此外,女性认为易怒的男性智力较低,在控制了男性的客观智力后,这种影响依然存在。最后,我们发现女性对其伴侣智力的看法在男性愤怒与两性关系满意度之间起到了中介作用。我们的研究结果表明,愤怒和智力在恋爱关系中都扮演着重要角色,这与强调恋爱伴侣能力(即智力)和同情心(即低愤怒)价值的进化理论是一致的。此外,我们的研究还强调了女性对其伴侣智力的看法在决定恋爱关系质量方面的重要性。
{"title":"Kindness or Intelligence? Angry Men are Perceived as Less Intelligent by Their Female Romantic Partners.","authors":"Jeremiasz Górniak, Marcin Zajenkowski, Kinga Szymaniak, Peter K Jonason","doi":"10.1177/14747049241275706","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241275706","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>We were interested in how people in a romantic relationship would perceive the intelligence of their partners who have high or low trait anger. Specifically, we referred to the tension between compassion (low anger) and competence (high intelligence) in mate choice. Some evolutionary theories suggest that mating might be considered a bargaining process between these two higher-order attributes. Our study involved 148 heterosexual couples in romantic relationships. We measured the relationship between relationship satisfaction, trait anger, objective intelligence, self-assessed intelligence, and subjectively assessed partners' intelligence. We found that angrier men were less satisfied in their romantic relationship than those men who were less angry, and their partners were also less satisfied in the relationship. Additionally, women perceived angrier men as less intelligent, an effect that remained after controlling for men's objective intelligence. Lastly, we found that women's perception of their partner's intelligence mediated the link between men's anger and relationship satisfaction for both sexes. Our findings suggest that both anger and intelligence play important roles in romantic relationship functioning, consistent with evolutionary theories that emphasize the value of competence (i.e., intelligence) and compassion (i.e., low anger) in romantic partners. Furthermore, our study highlights the importance of women's perception of their partner's intelligence in determining the quality of the relationship.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241275706"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11406617/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142126965","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Red Backgrounds Enhance Dominance in Human Faces and Shapes. 红色背景增强了人脸和形状的主导性。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241284602
Na Chen, Yidie Yang, Maiko Kobayashi, Koyo Nakamura, Katsumi Watanabe

Red color signals dominance in both animals and humans. This study investigated whether a red background color influences the perception of dominance in human faces and geometric shapes. The facial stimuli consisted of computer-generated faces, quantitatively morphed into nine levels of dominance, ranging from less dominant to more dominant. This included East-Asian female faces in Experiment 1 and male faces in Experiment 2. The face stimuli were presented against three background colors: red, green, and gray. Participants were instructed to categorize the faces as either obedient or dominant by pressing the corresponding labeled keys. The results showed that faces were more likely to be perceived as dominant when presented against a red background than against green or gray backgrounds, for both female and male faces. Additionally, two questionnaire surveys showed that the perception of dominance also increased for shapes presented against a red background. However the effect of red diminished in the absence of the actual perception of the color red. These results suggest that the perception of dominance in both human faces and objects is enhanced by the presence of red, possibly due to evolutionary factors related to the perception of red.

红色在动物和人类中都是优势的信号。本研究调查了红色背景是否会影响人脸和几何图形的支配感知。面部刺激包括计算机生成的人脸,这些人脸被定量变形为九个等级的支配力,从支配力较弱到支配力较强不等。实验 1 中包括东亚女性面孔,实验 2 中包括男性面孔。人脸刺激以三种背景颜色呈现:红色、绿色和灰色。参与者被要求按下相应的标签键,将人脸分为顺从型和支配型。结果显示,与绿色或灰色背景相比,红色背景下的女性和男性面孔更容易被认为是支配型的。此外,两项问卷调查显示,在红色背景下呈现的形状也会增加支配感。然而,在没有实际感知红色的情况下,红色的效果会减弱。这些结果表明,人脸和物体的优势感都会因红色的存在而增强,这可能是由于与红色感知有关的进化因素所致。
{"title":"Red Backgrounds Enhance Dominance in Human Faces and Shapes.","authors":"Na Chen, Yidie Yang, Maiko Kobayashi, Koyo Nakamura, Katsumi Watanabe","doi":"10.1177/14747049241284602","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241284602","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Red color signals dominance in both animals and humans. This study investigated whether a red background color influences the perception of dominance in human faces and geometric shapes. The facial stimuli consisted of computer-generated faces, quantitatively morphed into nine levels of dominance, ranging from less dominant to more dominant. This included East-Asian female faces in Experiment 1 and male faces in Experiment 2. The face stimuli were presented against three background colors: red, green, and gray. Participants were instructed to categorize the faces as either obedient or dominant by pressing the corresponding labeled keys. The results showed that faces were more likely to be perceived as dominant when presented against a red background than against green or gray backgrounds, for both female and male faces. Additionally, two questionnaire surveys showed that the perception of dominance also increased for shapes presented against a red background. However the effect of red diminished in the absence of the actual perception of the color red. These results suggest that the perception of dominance in both human faces and objects is enhanced by the presence of red, possibly due to evolutionary factors related to the perception of red.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241284602"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11440532/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142336926","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
An Experimental Test of Jealousy's Evolved Function: Imagined Partner Infidelity Induces Jealousy, Which Predicts Positive Attitude Towards Mate Retention. 对嫉妒进化功能的实验测试:假想伴侣不忠会诱发妒忌,而妒忌会预示保留配偶的积极态度。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241267226
Steven Arnocky, Kayla Kubinec, Megan MacKinnon, Dwight Mazmanian

Jealousy may have evolved to motivate adaptive compensatory behavior in response to threats to a valued relationship. This suggests that jealousy follows a temporal sequence: A perceived relational threat induces state feelings of jealousy which in turn motivates compensatory behavior, such as mate retention effort. Yet to date, tests of this mediation model have been limited to cross-sectional data. This study is the first to experimentally test this theoretical model. Men and women (N = 222) who were currently in committed romantic relationships were primed with an imagined partner infidelity (versus control) scenario. Participants then completed measures of state jealousy and intended mate retention behavior. Results found that those primed with the infidelity threat scenario experienced an increase in state jealousy, which in turn predicted more intended benefit-provisioning and cost-inflicting mate retention. Findings suggest that jealousy mediated the relationship between infidelity threat and intended mate retention behavior, supporting the evolutionary account of state jealousy.

嫉妒的进化可能是为了激发适应性补偿行为,以应对有价值关系受到的威胁。这表明嫉妒是有时间顺序的:感知到的关系威胁会引起状态上的嫉妒情绪,进而激发补偿行为,如努力保留配偶。然而,迄今为止,对这一中介模型的测试仅限于横截面数据。本研究首次对这一理论模型进行了实验验证。研究人员向目前处于承诺的恋爱关系中的男性和女性(N = 222)提供了一个想象中伴侣不忠(与对照组相比)的情景。然后,参与者完成了状态嫉妒和意向配偶保留行为的测量。结果发现,那些受到不忠威胁情景引诱的人的状态嫉妒会增加,这反过来又预示着他们会更多地打算提供利益和付出代价来挽留伴侣。研究结果表明,嫉妒在不忠威胁与意向性配偶保留行为之间起到了中介作用,支持了状态嫉妒的进化论。
{"title":"An Experimental Test of Jealousy's Evolved Function: Imagined Partner Infidelity Induces Jealousy, Which Predicts Positive Attitude Towards Mate Retention.","authors":"Steven Arnocky, Kayla Kubinec, Megan MacKinnon, Dwight Mazmanian","doi":"10.1177/14747049241267226","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049241267226","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Jealousy may have evolved to motivate adaptive compensatory behavior in response to threats to a valued relationship. This suggests that jealousy follows a temporal sequence: A perceived relational threat induces state feelings of jealousy which in turn motivates compensatory behavior, such as mate retention effort. Yet to date, tests of this mediation model have been limited to cross-sectional data. This study is the first to experimentally test this theoretical model. Men and women (<i>N </i>= 222) who were currently in committed romantic relationships were primed with an imagined partner infidelity (versus control) scenario. Participants then completed measures of state jealousy and intended mate retention behavior. Results found that those primed with the infidelity threat scenario experienced an increase in state jealousy, which in turn predicted more intended benefit-provisioning and cost-inflicting mate retention. Findings suggest that jealousy mediated the relationship between infidelity threat and intended mate retention behavior, supporting the evolutionary account of state jealousy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241267226"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11363030/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142093977","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolutionary Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1