首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Sex Differences in the Etiology of Victimization in Adulthood. 成年后受害原因的性别差异。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241267950
Bridget Joyner-Carpanini, Kevin M Beaver

Until relatively recently, the study of victimization has been largely outside the purview of behavioral geneticists and evolutionary psychologists. Recent victimology research, however, has shown that genetic and evolutionary forces are connected to the risk of victimization. The current study expands on these findings by examining whether genetic influences differentially explain victimization in males and females. To do so, we use a sample of sibling pairs drawn from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; N = 4,244). The analyses revealed no significant quantitative sex differences in the etiology of adult victimization. However, the results of this study do highlight the importance of accounting for genetic factors when studying the etiology of specific types of adult victimization. We conclude by discussing the implications of the current study for future research.

直到最近,对受害情况的研究在很大程度上还不属于行为遗传学家和进化心理学家的研究范围。然而,最近的受害研究表明,遗传和进化的力量与受害风险有关。本研究在这些研究成果的基础上,进一步探讨了遗传因素对男性和女性受害情况的影响是否存在差异。为此,我们使用了从全国青少年到成人健康纵向研究(Add Health; N = 4,244 )中抽取的兄弟姐妹对样本。分析结果显示,在成人受害的病因学方面没有明显的定量性别差异。不过,这项研究的结果确实强调了在研究特定类型的成人受害病因时考虑遗传因素的重要性。最后,我们将讨论本研究对未来研究的影响。
{"title":"Sex Differences in the Etiology of Victimization in Adulthood.","authors":"Bridget Joyner-Carpanini, Kevin M Beaver","doi":"10.1177/14747049241267950","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241267950","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Until relatively recently, the study of victimization has been largely outside the purview of behavioral geneticists and evolutionary psychologists. Recent victimology research, however, has shown that genetic and evolutionary forces are connected to the risk of victimization. The current study expands on these findings by examining whether genetic influences differentially explain victimization in males and females. To do so, we use a sample of sibling pairs drawn from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; <i>N </i>= 4,244). The analyses revealed no significant quantitative sex differences in the etiology of adult victimization. However, the results of this study do highlight the importance of accounting for genetic factors when studying the etiology of specific types of adult victimization. We conclude by discussing the implications of the current study for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241267950"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11325314/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141976923","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Autoimmune Hashimoto's Disease and Feminization Level-Testing the Immunocompetence Hypothesis. 自身免疫性桥本氏病与女性化水平--检验免疫能力假说。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241259187
Malwina Goździk, Agnieszka Żelaźniewicz, Judyta Nowak-Kornicka, Katarzyna Pawłowska-Seredyńska, Wioleta Umławska, Bogusław Pawłowski

Morphological femininity depends mainly on estrogen levels at puberty and is perceived as a cue of a woman's biological condition. Due to the immunostimulant properties of estradiol, estradiol-dependent feminine traits are expected to be positively related to immunity. However, heightened immunity in women may increase the risk of autoimmune disease, thus the relationship between femininity and immune quality may be complex. This study aimed to assess the relationship between morphological femininity and both the occurrence and severity of Hashimoto thyroiditis (HT) in women of reproductive age. Moreover, 95 women with HT and 84 without HT (all between 20 and 37 years) participated in the study. Morphological femininity was assessed based on somatic measurements of sexually dimorphic traits (2D:4D ratio, WHR, breast size, facial sexual dimorphism). The occurrence and severity of HT were assessed by serum TPOAb levels. The results showed that only the 2D:4D ratio of the right hand was higher in the HT group, indicating higher femininity in these women. However, there was also a positive relationship between facial femininity and TPOAb level in women with HT, indicating a higher severity of the disease. The results suggest that prenatal and pubertal exposure to estrogens may increase the probability or severity of autoimmune diseases in adulthood, but the relationship is tentative.

形态上的女性特质主要取决于青春期的雌激素水平,并被视为女性生理状况的线索。由于雌二醇具有免疫刺激的特性,依赖雌二醇的女性特质预计与免疫力呈正相关。然而,女性免疫力的提高可能会增加患自身免疫性疾病的风险,因此女性特质与免疫质量之间的关系可能很复杂。本研究旨在评估育龄妇女的形态女性特质与桥本氏甲状腺炎(HT)的发生和严重程度之间的关系。此外,95名患有桥本氏甲状腺炎的女性和84名未患有桥本氏甲状腺炎的女性(年龄均在20至37岁之间)参与了这项研究。根据对性别二形特征(2D:4D 比率、WHR、乳房大小、面部性别二形性)的体格测量结果,对形态女性化进行了评估。通过血清 TPOAb 水平评估 HT 的发生和严重程度。结果显示,HT 组中只有右手的 2D:4D 比例较高,表明这些女性的女性化程度较高。然而,HT 女性患者的面部女性特质与 TPOAb 水平之间也存在正相关关系,这表明该疾病的严重程度更高。结果表明,产前和青春期暴露于雌激素可能会增加成年后患自身免疫性疾病的概率或严重程度,但这种关系是暂时的。
{"title":"Autoimmune Hashimoto's Disease and Feminization Level-Testing <i>the Immunocompetence Hypothesis</i>.","authors":"Malwina Goździk, Agnieszka Żelaźniewicz, Judyta Nowak-Kornicka, Katarzyna Pawłowska-Seredyńska, Wioleta Umławska, Bogusław Pawłowski","doi":"10.1177/14747049241259187","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241259187","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Morphological femininity depends mainly on estrogen levels at puberty and is perceived as a cue of a woman's biological condition. Due to the immunostimulant properties of estradiol, estradiol-dependent feminine traits are expected to be positively related to immunity. However, heightened immunity in women may increase the risk of autoimmune disease, thus the relationship between femininity and immune quality may be complex. This study aimed to assess the relationship between morphological femininity and both the occurrence and severity of Hashimoto thyroiditis (HT) in women of reproductive age. Moreover, 95 women with HT and 84 without HT (all between 20 and 37 years) participated in the study. Morphological femininity was assessed based on somatic measurements of sexually dimorphic traits (2D:4D ratio, WHR, breast size, facial sexual dimorphism). The occurrence and severity of HT were assessed by serum TPOAb levels. The results showed that only the 2D:4D ratio of the right hand was higher in the HT group, indicating higher femininity in these women. However, there was also a positive relationship between facial femininity and TPOAb level in women with HT, indicating a higher severity of the disease. The results suggest that prenatal and pubertal exposure to estrogens may increase the probability or severity of autoimmune diseases in adulthood, but the relationship is tentative.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241259187"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11378202/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142141413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Paternal Filicide in Sweden: Background, Risk Factors and the Cinderella Effect. 瑞典的弑父事件:背景、风险因素和灰姑娘效应。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241265623
Hans Temrin

An overrepresentation of stepchildren as victims of filicide has been explained as a consequence of 'discriminative parental solicitude'. The idea being that Darwinian selection has favoured parental love and concern only for biological children, and when such parental feelings are absent, as in stepparents, conflicts with a child could easier escalate to lethal violence. An alternative explanation for this overrepresentation of stepchildren is that risk factors for filicide, such as criminal behaviour and mental health problems, are more prevalent in stepparents. This study focused on paternal filicide in Sweden and investigated (i) if stepchildren are overrepresented as victims of filicide compared with biological children, (ii) if filicides are committed in a context that implies a 'conflict with the child victim' and (iii) if stepfathers and biological fathers differ in characteristics associated with filicide risk. The analyses showed that stepchildren were overrepresented as victims compared with children of fathers in families with two biological parents and this overrepresentation was even higher in young children. Children of single biological fathers and children of non-residential biological fathers were also overrepresented as victims of filicide. Less than 20 percent of the filicides were committed in the context of a 'conflict with the child' and in these cases only stepchildren were overrepresented as victims. In the population at large, both stepfathers and single biological fathers had higher rates of mental health problems, violent criminality and illegal possession of drugs compared with fathers in families with two biological parents.

继子女成为杀父仇母行为受害者的比例过高,被解释为 "歧视性父母关爱 "的结果。这种观点认为,达尔文的选择倾向于只对亲生子女给予父母的爱和关心,而当这种父母情感缺失时,比如继父母,与子女的冲突就很容易升级为致命的暴力。继子女比例过高的另一种解释是,继父母中的犯罪行为和心理健康问题等杀父杀母的风险因素更为普遍。本研究侧重于瑞典的父亲杀婴案,并调查了(i)继子女与亲生子女相比是否成为杀婴案受害者的比例过高,(ii)杀婴案是否在意味着 "与受害儿童发生冲突 "的情况下发生,以及(iii)继父与亲生父亲在与杀婴风险相关的特征方面是否存在差异。分析表明,与生父为双亲的家庭中的子女相比,继子女成为受害者的比例过高,这种比例过高的情况在幼儿中更为严重。单亲生父的子女和非寄宿生父的子女也是过失杀人的受害者。不到 20%的弑父案件是在 "与子女发生冲突 "的情况下发生的,在这些案件中,只有继子女的受害者比例过高。在整个人口中,继父和单亲父亲的精神健康问题、暴力犯罪和非法持有毒品的比例都高于双亲家庭中的父亲。
{"title":"Paternal Filicide in Sweden: Background, Risk Factors and the Cinderella Effect.","authors":"Hans Temrin","doi":"10.1177/14747049241265623","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241265623","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>An overrepresentation of stepchildren as victims of filicide has been explained as a consequence of 'discriminative parental solicitude'. The idea being that Darwinian selection has favoured parental love and concern only for biological children, and when such parental feelings are absent, as in stepparents, conflicts with a child could easier escalate to lethal violence. An alternative explanation for this overrepresentation of stepchildren is that risk factors for filicide, such as criminal behaviour and mental health problems, are more prevalent in stepparents. This study focused on paternal filicide in Sweden and investigated (i) if stepchildren are overrepresented as victims of filicide compared with biological children, (ii) if filicides are committed in a context that implies a 'conflict with the child victim' and (iii) if stepfathers and biological fathers differ in characteristics associated with filicide risk. The analyses showed that stepchildren were overrepresented as victims compared with children of fathers in families with two biological parents and this overrepresentation was even higher in young children. Children of single biological fathers and children of non-residential biological fathers were also overrepresented as victims of filicide. Less than 20 percent of the filicides were committed in the context of a 'conflict with the child' and in these cases only stepchildren were overrepresented as victims. In the population at large, both stepfathers and single biological fathers had higher rates of mental health problems, violent criminality and illegal possession of drugs compared with fathers in families with two biological parents.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 3","pages":"14747049241265623"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11359440/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142082184","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment. 国际跨时空教育成就 "畸配 "元分析》(International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment)。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241249072
Sebastian Jensen, Emil Kirkegaard

Previous studies have found a high degree of assortative mating for educational attainment (r = .56). However, this can be confounded by cohort effects or country effects, where certain nations may have more pronounced assortative mating than others. In addition, method variance regarding how educational attainment is measured may also result in heterogeneity of effect sizes. Effect sizes were gathered from various datasets and from academic literature, resulting in a large collection of effect sizes (k = 1498, n = 9,159,098), spanning 84 different countries. Assortative mating for educational attainment was stronger than what previous literature suggested (r = .66, [.64, .68]), largely due to the fact that assortative mating for educational attainment is stronger when latent methods are used. The strongest predictors of assortative mating for education between countries were individualism (r = -.61, p < .001) and HDI (r = -.56, p < .001). Assortative mating over time was found to vary by region. Capitalist Europe experienced an increase in assortative mating for education, while Communist Europe experienced a decrease. The United States had a non-linear trend in assortative mating for educational attainment, as it decreased from 1875 to 1926, increased from 1926 to 1945, decreased from 1945 to 1958, increased from 1958 to 1977, and decreased from 1977 onwards.

以往的研究发现,教育程度的同配程度很高(r = .56)。然而,这可能会受到队列效应或国家效应的影响,某些国家的同配现象可能比其他国家更为明显。此外,教育程度测量方法的差异也可能导致效应大小的异质性。我们从不同的数据集和学术文献中收集了效应大小,从而收集了大量效应大小(k = 1498,n = 9,159,098),涉及 84 个不同的国家。教育程度方面的同配现象比以往的文献所指出的要强(r = .66,[.64, .68]),这主要是由于在使用潜在方法时,教育程度方面的同配现象更强。国家间教育同配的最强预测因素是个人主义(r = -.61,p
{"title":"International Cross-Temporal Meta-Analysis of Assortative Mating for Educational Attainment.","authors":"Sebastian Jensen, Emil Kirkegaard","doi":"10.1177/14747049241249072","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241249072","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have found a high degree of assortative mating for educational attainment (r = .56). However, this can be confounded by cohort effects or country effects, where certain nations may have more pronounced assortative mating than others. In addition, method variance regarding how educational attainment is measured may also result in heterogeneity of effect sizes. Effect sizes were gathered from various datasets and from academic literature, resulting in a large collection of effect sizes (k = 1498, n = 9,159,098), spanning 84 different countries. Assortative mating for educational attainment was stronger than what previous literature suggested (r = .66, [.64, .68]), largely due to the fact that assortative mating for educational attainment is stronger when latent methods are used. The strongest predictors of assortative mating for education between countries were individualism (r = -.61, p < .001) and HDI (r = -.56, p < .001). Assortative mating over time was found to vary by region. Capitalist Europe experienced an increase in assortative mating for education, while Communist Europe experienced a decrease. The United States had a non-linear trend in assortative mating for educational attainment, as it decreased from 1875 to 1926, increased from 1926 to 1945, decreased from 1945 to 1958, increased from 1958 to 1977, and decreased from 1977 onwards.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241249072"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11146041/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141201117","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Re-evaluating the Role of Partnership-Related Perceptions in Women's Preferences for Men with Masculine Face Shapes. 重新评估与伴侣关系相关的观念在女性偏好男性阳刚脸型中的作用。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241262712
Junzhi Dong, Kathlyne Leger, Anthony J Lee, Yasaman Rafiee, Benedict C Jones, Victor K M Shiramizu

Many researchers have proposed that women perceive men with masculine face shapes to be less suitable as parents and long-term partners than men with feminine face shapes, causing women to find masculine men more attractive for short-term than long-term relationships. However, recent work shows that results obtained using the type of experimentally manipulated stimuli that were employed in studies presenting evidence for these claims are not necessarily observed when natural (i.e., unmanipulated) face stimuli were used to suggest that the evidence for these claims may need to be revaluated. Consequently, we tested for possible relationships between ratings of natural male faces for parenting- and relationship-related traits and shape masculinity (Study 1) and also tested whether women's preferences for shape masculinity were stronger when natural male faces were rated for short-term relationships than when natural male faces were rated for long-term relationships (Studies 2 and 3). We saw no evidence for either of these predictions, instead finding that men with more attractive faces were perceived to be better parents and better long-term partners. Thus, our findings do not support the widely held view that masculine men are more attractive for short-term relationships because they are perceived to be unlikely to invest time and effort in their romantic partners and offspring.

许多研究人员提出,女性认为男性脸型的男性比女性脸型的男性更不适合作为父母和长期伴侣,从而导致女性认为男性脸型的男性在短期关系中比长期关系中更有吸引力。然而,最近的研究表明,当使用自然的(即未被操纵的)脸部刺激时,使用实验操纵的刺激类型所得到的结果并不一定能证明这些说法,这表明这些说法的证据可能需要重新评估。因此,我们测试了自然男性面孔在养育子女和人际关系相关特质上的评分与形状男子气概之间可能存在的关系(研究 1),还测试了当自然男性面孔被评为短期关系时,女性对形状男子气概的偏好是否比自然男性面孔被评为长期关系时更强烈(研究 2 和 3)。我们没有发现上述任何一种预测的证据,相反,我们发现拥有更具吸引力面孔的男性被认为是更好的父母和更好的长期伴侣。因此,我们的研究结果并不支持人们普遍持有的观点,即男性化的男性在短期关系中更具吸引力,因为他们被认为不太可能在恋爱伴侣和后代身上投入时间和精力。
{"title":"Re-evaluating the Role of Partnership-Related Perceptions in Women's Preferences for Men with Masculine Face Shapes.","authors":"Junzhi Dong, Kathlyne Leger, Anthony J Lee, Yasaman Rafiee, Benedict C Jones, Victor K M Shiramizu","doi":"10.1177/14747049241262712","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241262712","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many researchers have proposed that women perceive men with masculine face shapes to be less suitable as parents and long-term partners than men with feminine face shapes, causing women to find masculine men more attractive for short-term than long-term relationships. However, recent work shows that results obtained using the type of experimentally manipulated stimuli that were employed in studies presenting evidence for these claims are not necessarily observed when natural (i.e., unmanipulated) face stimuli were used to suggest that the evidence for these claims may need to be revaluated. Consequently, we tested for possible relationships between ratings of natural male faces for parenting- and relationship-related traits and shape masculinity (Study 1) and also tested whether women's preferences for shape masculinity were stronger when natural male faces were rated for short-term relationships than when natural male faces were rated for long-term relationships (Studies 2 and 3). We saw no evidence for either of these predictions, instead finding that men with more attractive faces were perceived to be better parents and better long-term partners. Thus, our findings do not support the widely held view that masculine men are more attractive for short-term relationships because they are perceived to be unlikely to invest time and effort in their romantic partners and offspring.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241262712"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11184995/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141332278","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Deciding Who Is Worthy of Help: Effect of the Probability of Reciprocity on Individuals' Willingness to Help Others. 决定谁值得帮助:互惠概率对个人帮助他人意愿的影响。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241254725
Ryo Oda, Natsuki Hayashi

In order to explain helping strangers in need in terms of reciprocal altruism, it is necessary to ensure that the help is reciprocated and that the costs of helping are thus compensated. Competence and willingness to make sacrifices for the benefactor of the person being helped are important cues for ensuring a return on help because reciprocity would not be possible if the person being helped had neither the competence nor the inclination to give back in the future. In this study, we used vignettes and manipulated the cause of suffering strangers' difficulties and prosociality to investigate participants' compassion for and willingness to help the stranger. In Study 1, we measured willingness to help by using hypothetical helping behaviors that were designed to vary in cost. In Study 2, we measured willingness to help by using the checkbox method in which participants were asked to sequentially check 10 × 10 checkboxes on a webpage, which asked the participants to pay a small but real cost. In both studies, the controllability of the cause and the prosociality were found to independently affect compassion. These two factors also independently affected willingness to help, as measured by both the hypothetical questions and the checkbox method. We consequently discussed the reasons for the independent processing of the competence and behavioral tendency cues.

为了从互惠利他主义的角度解释帮助需要帮助的陌生人,有必要确保帮助得到回报,从而补偿帮助的成本。被帮助者的能力和为恩人做出牺牲的意愿是确保帮助得到回报的重要线索,因为如果被帮助者既没有能力也没有意愿在未来做出回报,互惠就不可能实现。在本研究中,我们使用了小故事,并操纵了陌生人遭受困难的原因和亲社会性,以调查参与者对陌生人的同情心和帮助陌生人的意愿。在研究 1 中,我们通过设计不同成本的假设帮助行为来测量帮助意愿。在研究 2 中,我们使用复选框法来测量受试者的帮助意愿,即要求受试者在网页上依次勾选 10 × 10 个复选框,这就要求受试者支付小额但真实的费用。在这两项研究中,我们都发现原因的可控性和亲社会性会独立影响同情心。这两个因素对帮助意愿的影响也是独立的,通过假设性问题和复选框方法都可以测量出这两个因素。因此,我们讨论了能力和行为倾向线索独立处理的原因。
{"title":"Deciding Who Is Worthy of Help: Effect of the Probability of Reciprocity on Individuals' Willingness to Help Others.","authors":"Ryo Oda, Natsuki Hayashi","doi":"10.1177/14747049241254725","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241254725","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In order to explain helping strangers in need in terms of reciprocal altruism, it is necessary to ensure that the help is reciprocated and that the costs of helping are thus compensated. Competence and willingness to make sacrifices for the benefactor of the person being helped are important cues for ensuring a return on help because reciprocity would not be possible if the person being helped had neither the competence nor the inclination to give back in the future. In this study, we used vignettes and manipulated the cause of suffering strangers' difficulties and prosociality to investigate participants' compassion for and willingness to help the stranger. In Study 1, we measured willingness to help by using hypothetical helping behaviors that were designed to vary in cost. In Study 2, we measured willingness to help by using the checkbox method in which participants were asked to sequentially check 10 × 10 checkboxes on a webpage, which asked the participants to pay a small but real cost. In both studies, the controllability of the cause and the prosociality were found to independently affect compassion. These two factors also independently affected willingness to help, as measured by both the hypothetical questions and the checkbox method. We consequently discussed the reasons for the independent processing of the competence and behavioral tendency cues.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241254725"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11138187/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141160480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Terrorism as Coalitional Predation: Explaining Definitional Ambiguities and Precautionary Responses. 作为联盟掠夺的恐怖主义:解释定义的模糊性和预防性反应。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241263995
Michael Moncrieff

Terrorism continues to be an enigmatic and contested concept, lacking a universally accepted definition despite extensive scholarly debate. Lay intuitions, however, demonstrate a notable convergence in identifying acts as "terrorism" when specific situational features are present, such as indiscriminate violence and out-group perpetration. These features elicit predictable and robust precautionary responses, raising the question: Is there a unified and parsimonious explanation for these phenomena? It is hypothesized that a situational template exists in the human mind, the coalitional predation template (CPT), which evolved not to detect modern-day terrorism, per se, but to identify and respond to situations of predatory coalitional conflict. The paper examines the potential cues and mechanisms that constitute the psychological systems activated by such threats, suggesting that matching the input cues of the CPT triggers well-documented precautionary responses to terrorism. However, this cue-based system may not align neatly with contemporary threats, leading to disproportionate responses to some threats while underestimating others. The model also posits that interpretations of violence can vary due to incomplete cues and the social position of the evaluator, leading to public disagreements and inconsistencies in defining terrorism. Consequently, arriving at an unambiguous and widely accepted definition of terrorism may not be possible. The model presented may account for a range of phenomena, including the inclination towards attributing mental illness to particular violent incidents and the uncanny surface similarities between terrorism and war crimes. The findings have significant implications for both the theoretical understanding of terrorism and practical policy responses.

恐怖主义仍然是一个神秘而有争议的概念,尽管学者们进行了广泛的辩论,但仍缺乏一个普遍接受的定义。然而,外行人的直觉表明,当出现特定的情境特征(如不分青红皂白的暴力和外群体犯罪)时,将行为认定为 "恐怖主义 "的观点明显趋同。这些特征引起了可预测的、强有力的预防性反应,从而提出了一个问题:这些现象是否有统一而简洁的解释?本文假设人类头脑中存在一种情境模板,即联盟掠夺模板(CPT),它的进化不是为了侦测现代恐怖主义本身,而是为了识别和应对掠夺性联盟冲突的情境。本文研究了构成被此类威胁激活的心理系统的潜在线索和机制,认为与联合捕食模板的输入线索相匹配,会触发有据可查的针对恐怖主义的预防性反应。然而,这种基于线索的系统可能与当代威胁并不完全一致,从而导致对某些威胁做出不相称的反应,同时低估了其他威胁。该模型还假定,由于线索不完整和评估者的社会地位不同,对暴力的解释也会不同,从而导致公众对恐怖主义定义的分歧和不一致。因此,可能无法对恐怖主义做出一个明确且广为接受的定义。所提出的模型可以解释一系列现象,包括将精神疾病归因于特定暴力事件的倾向,以及恐怖主义与战争罪行之间不可思议的表面相似性。研究结果对恐怖主义的理论认识和实际政策应对都具有重要意义。
{"title":"Terrorism as Coalitional Predation: Explaining Definitional Ambiguities and Precautionary Responses.","authors":"Michael Moncrieff","doi":"10.1177/14747049241263995","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241263995","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Terrorism continues to be an enigmatic and contested concept, lacking a universally accepted definition despite extensive scholarly debate. Lay intuitions, however, demonstrate a notable convergence in identifying acts as \"terrorism\" when specific situational features are present, such as indiscriminate violence and out-group perpetration. These features elicit predictable and robust precautionary responses, raising the question: Is there a unified and parsimonious explanation for these phenomena? It is hypothesized that a situational template exists in the human mind, the coalitional predation template (CPT), which evolved not to detect modern-day terrorism, <i>per se</i>, but to identify and respond to situations of predatory coalitional conflict. The paper examines the potential cues and mechanisms that constitute the psychological systems activated by such threats, suggesting that matching the input cues of the CPT triggers well-documented precautionary responses to terrorism. However, this cue-based system may not align neatly with contemporary threats, leading to disproportionate responses to some threats while underestimating others. The model also posits that interpretations of violence can vary due to incomplete cues and the social position of the evaluator, leading to public disagreements and inconsistencies in defining terrorism. Consequently, arriving at an unambiguous and widely accepted definition of terrorism may not be possible. The model presented may account for a range of phenomena, including the inclination towards attributing mental illness to particular violent incidents and the uncanny surface similarities between terrorism and war crimes. The findings have significant implications for both the theoretical understanding of terrorism and practical policy responses.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241263995"},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11273568/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761658","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reasoning More Efficiently with Primary Knowledge Despite Extraneous Cognitive Load. 尽管有外来认知负荷,但利用初级知识推理的效率更高
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241252694
Florence Lespiau, André Tricot

Geary's evolutionary approach in educational psychology differentiates between primary (low cognitive costs and motivational advantage) and secondary knowledge (high cognitive costs and no motivational benefit). Although these features have been well demonstrated in previous work, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. To investigate it, in a reasoning task, the present study varies (i) the content of the problems (primary knowledge vs. secondary; e.g., food vs. grammar rules), (ii) the intrinsic cognitive load (conflict or non-conflict syllogism, the former requiring more cognitive resources to be properly processed than the latter) and (iii) the extraneous cognitive load (via a Dot Memory Task with three modalities: low, medium and high cognitive load). Analyses assessed the influence of these variables on performance, problem solving speed and perceived cognitive load. Results confirmed the positive impact of primary knowledge on efficiency, particularly when intrinsic cognitive load was high. Surprisingly, the extraneous cognitive load did not influence the performance in secondary knowledge content but that in primary knowledge content: the higher the additional load was, the better the performance was, only for primary knowledge and especially for syllogisms with high intrinsic load. Findings support evolutionary theory as secondary knowledge would overload cognitive resources, preventing participants from allocating sufficient resources to solve problems. Primary knowledge would allow participants to process the additional load and to increase their performance despite this. This study also raises the hypothesis that a minimum cognitive load is necessary for participants to be invested in the task.

Geary 在教育心理学中提出的进化论方法将知识分为初级知识(认知成本低,动机有利)和次级知识(认知成本高,动机无益)。虽然这些特征在以往的研究中得到了很好的证明,但其背后的机制仍不清楚。为了研究这个问题,本研究在推理任务中改变了(i)问题的内容(主要知识与次要知识;例如,食物与语法规则),(ii)内在认知负荷(冲突或非冲突的三段论,前者比后者需要更多的认知资源来正确处理)和(iii)外在认知负荷(通过点记忆任务,有三种模式:低、中、高认知负荷)。分析评估了这些变量对成绩、解题速度和认知负荷的影响。结果证实了初级知识对效率的积极影响,尤其是在内在认知负荷较高的情况下。令人惊讶的是,外在认知负荷对次要知识内容的学习成绩没有影响,但对主要知识内容的学习成绩有影响:额外负担越高,学习成绩越好,这只适用于主要知识,尤其是内在负荷高的对偶句。研究结果支持进化理论,因为次要知识会使认知资源超载,使参与者无法分配足够的资源来解决问题。而初级知识可以让参与者处理额外的负荷,并提高他们的成绩。本研究还提出了一个假设,即最低认知负荷是参与者投入任务的必要条件。
{"title":"Reasoning More Efficiently with Primary Knowledge Despite Extraneous Cognitive Load.","authors":"Florence Lespiau, André Tricot","doi":"10.1177/14747049241252694","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241252694","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Geary's evolutionary approach in educational psychology differentiates between primary (low cognitive costs and motivational advantage) and secondary knowledge (high cognitive costs and no motivational benefit). Although these features have been well demonstrated in previous work, the underlying mechanisms remain unclear. To investigate it, in a reasoning task, the present study varies (i) the content of the problems (primary knowledge vs. secondary; e.g., food vs. grammar rules), (ii) the intrinsic cognitive load (conflict or non-conflict syllogism, the former requiring more cognitive resources to be properly processed than the latter) and (iii) the extraneous cognitive load (via a Dot Memory Task with three modalities: low, medium and high cognitive load). Analyses assessed the influence of these variables on performance, problem solving speed and perceived cognitive load. Results confirmed the positive impact of primary knowledge on efficiency, particularly when intrinsic cognitive load was high. Surprisingly, the extraneous cognitive load did not influence the performance in secondary knowledge content but that in primary knowledge content: the higher the additional load was, the better the performance was, only for primary knowledge and especially for syllogisms with high intrinsic load. Findings support evolutionary theory as secondary knowledge would overload cognitive resources, preventing participants from allocating sufficient resources to solve problems. Primary knowledge would allow participants to process the additional load and to increase their performance despite this. This study also raises the hypothesis that a minimum cognitive load is necessary for participants to be invested in the task.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241252694"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11155337/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The General Factor of Environmental Sensitivity: Relationships with the General Factor of Personality. 环境敏感性的一般因素:环境敏感性一般因子:与人格一般因子的关系。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241254727
Shuhei Iimura, Kosuke Yano

Environmental sensitivity is a meta-concept that describes individual differences in susceptibility to both positive and negative environmental influences and has been repeatedly reported to correlate with other established personality traits, including the Big Five. The purpose of this study was to examine the correlation between the general factor of environmental sensitivity (GFS) and the general factor of personality (GFP). A total of 1,046 adult participants (52% female; Mage = 45.15, SDage = 12.70) completed a self-report psychological questionnaire on an online form. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that GFS had a strong negative correlation with GFP (r = -.41, 95% CI [-.52, -.30], p < .001). Focusing on the relationship with the Big Five, individuals with higher environmental sensitivity were emotionally unstable and introverted. The trait of environmental sensitivity may be described not only in relation to the Big Five but also in relation to GFP, which is assumed to be an indicator of social effectiveness.

环境敏感性是一个元概念,它描述了个体对积极和消极环境影响的易感性差异,并多次被报道与其他既定的人格特质(包括五大人格特质)相关。本研究旨在考察环境敏感性一般因子(GFS)与人格一般因子(GFP)之间的相关性。共有 1,046 名成年参与者(52% 为女性;平均年龄 = 45.15 岁,最小年龄 = 12.70 岁)通过在线形式完成了一份自我报告心理问卷。确认性因素分析表明,GFS 与 GFP 有很强的负相关(r = -.41, 95% CI [-.52, -.30], p
{"title":"The General Factor of Environmental Sensitivity: Relationships with the General Factor of Personality.","authors":"Shuhei Iimura, Kosuke Yano","doi":"10.1177/14747049241254727","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241254727","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Environmental sensitivity is a meta-concept that describes individual differences in susceptibility to both positive and negative environmental influences and has been repeatedly reported to correlate with other established personality traits, including the Big Five. The purpose of this study was to examine the correlation between the general factor of environmental sensitivity (GFS) and the general factor of personality (GFP). A total of 1,046 adult participants (52% female; <i>M</i><sub>age </sub>= 45.15, <i>SD</i><sub>age </sub>= 12.70) completed a self-report psychological questionnaire on an online form. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that GFS had a strong negative correlation with GFP (<i>r </i>= -.41, 95% CI [-.52, -.30], <i>p </i>< .001). Focusing on the relationship with the Big Five, individuals with higher environmental sensitivity were emotionally unstable and introverted. The trait of environmental sensitivity may be described not only in relation to the Big Five but also in relation to GFP, which is assumed to be an indicator of social effectiveness.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241254727"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11119410/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082542","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
First They Scream, Then They Laugh: The Cognitive Intersections of Humor and Fear. 他们先是尖叫,然后大笑:幽默与恐惧的认知交集》。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049241258355
Marc Hye-Knudsen, Jens Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, Brian B Boutwell, Mathias Clasen

On the surface, fear and humor seem like polar opposite states of mind, yet throughout our lives they continually interact. In this paper, we synthesize neurobiological, psychological, and evolutionary research on fear and humor, arguing that the two are deeply connected. The evolutionary origins of humor reside in play, a medium through which animals benignly explore situations and practice strategies, such as fight or flight, which would normally be accompanied by fear. Cognitively, humor retains the structure of play. Adopting a view of humor as requiring two appraisals, a violation appraisal and a benign appraisal, we describe how fear-inducing stimuli can be rendered benignly humorous through contextual cues, psychological distance, reframing, and cognitive reappraisal. The antagonistic relationship between humor and fear in terms of their neurochemistry and physiological effects in turn makes humor ideal for managing fear in many circumstances. We review five real-world examples of humor and fear intersecting, presenting new data in support of our account along the way. Finally, we discuss the possible therapeutic relevance of the deep connection between humor and fear.

从表面上看,恐惧和幽默似乎是两种截然相反的心理状态,但在我们的生活中,它们却不断地相互作用。在本文中,我们综合了有关恐惧和幽默的神经生物学、心理学和进化论研究,认为两者之间有着深刻的联系。幽默的进化起源于游戏,动物通过游戏良性地探索情境和练习策略,如战斗或逃跑,而这些通常会伴随着恐惧。在认知上,幽默保留了游戏的结构。我们认为幽默需要两种评价,一种是侵犯性评价,另一种是良性评价。我们描述了如何通过情境提示、心理距离、重构和认知再评价,使诱发恐惧的刺激变得良性幽默。幽默与恐惧之间在神经化学和生理效应方面的拮抗关系反过来又使幽默在许多情况下成为控制恐惧的理想手段。我们回顾了幽默与恐惧交织在一起的五个真实案例,并提出了支持我们观点的新数据。最后,我们将讨论幽默与恐惧之间的深层联系可能带来的治疗意义。
{"title":"First They Scream, Then They Laugh: The Cognitive Intersections of Humor and Fear.","authors":"Marc Hye-Knudsen, Jens Kjeldgaard-Christiansen, Brian B Boutwell, Mathias Clasen","doi":"10.1177/14747049241258355","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049241258355","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>On the surface, fear and humor seem like polar opposite states of mind, yet throughout our lives they continually interact. In this paper, we synthesize neurobiological, psychological, and evolutionary research on fear and humor, arguing that the two are deeply connected. The evolutionary origins of humor reside in play, a medium through which animals benignly explore situations and practice strategies, such as fight or flight, which would normally be accompanied by fear. Cognitively, humor retains the structure of play. Adopting a view of humor as requiring two appraisals, a violation appraisal and a benign appraisal, we describe how fear-inducing stimuli can be rendered benignly humorous through contextual cues, psychological distance, reframing, and cognitive reappraisal. The antagonistic relationship between humor and fear in terms of their neurochemistry and physiological effects in turn makes humor ideal for managing fear in many circumstances. We review five real-world examples of humor and fear intersecting, presenting new data in support of our account along the way. Finally, we discuss the possible therapeutic relevance of the deep connection between humor and fear.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":"22 2","pages":"14747049241258355"},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11155347/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141262946","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolutionary Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1