首页 > 最新文献

Evolutionary Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Women's Romantic Jealousy Predicts Risky Appearance Enhancement Effort. 女性的浪漫嫉妒预示着提升外表的努力。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231185782
Steven Arnocky, Megan MacKinnon, Sadie Clarke, Grant McPherson, Emily Kapitanchuk

Human appearance enhancement effort has recently been considered from an evolutionary perspective as an adaptive and sexually dimorphic strategy for effective female intrasexual and intersexual competition. Most writing and research on the topic to date has focused on appearance enhancement as a means of mate attraction, with relatively less research examining its role in mate retention. The present study considered whether romantic jealousy, as a negative emotion experienced in response to perceived threat to a desired relationship, predicts costly and/or risky appearance enhancement independent of the closely related emotion of envy. In a sample of 189 undergraduate women, results showed that romantic jealousy and dispositional envy were positively correlated with one another. Results further demonstrated that romantic jealousy predicted women's positive attitude toward cosmetic surgery, willingness to use a one-week free tanning membership, willingness to use a risky diet pill, and intent on spending a greater proportion of their income on appearance enhancement, but not intended use of facial cosmetics. Results held independent of participants' dispositional envy, suggesting that romantic jealousy is a unique predictor of women's efforts at enhancing their physical appearance, which could extend into costly and physically risky mate retention efforts.

最近,从进化的角度来看,人类外表提升的努力被认为是一种适应性和性别二态性的策略,用于有效的女性性内和性间竞争。迄今为止,大多数关于这一主题的写作和研究都集中在外表提升作为吸引配偶的一种手段,而研究其在保持配偶中的作用的研究相对较少。本研究考虑了浪漫嫉妒,作为一种对所需关系的感知威胁所产生的负面情绪,是否独立于密切相关的嫉妒情绪,预测了代价高昂和/或有风险的外表提升。在189名大学生女性的样本中,研究结果表明,浪漫嫉妒和性格嫉妒之间呈正相关。研究结果进一步表明,浪漫嫉妒可以预测女性对整容手术的积极态度,愿意使用一周免费晒黑会员资格,愿意使用有风险的减肥药,以及打算将更大比例的收入用于美容,但不打算使用面部化妆品。研究结果独立于参与者的倾向性嫉妒,表明浪漫嫉妒是女性努力提升外表的独特预测因素,这可能会延伸到代价高昂且身体风险大的伴侣保持努力中。
{"title":"Women's Romantic Jealousy Predicts Risky Appearance Enhancement Effort.","authors":"Steven Arnocky,&nbsp;Megan MacKinnon,&nbsp;Sadie Clarke,&nbsp;Grant McPherson,&nbsp;Emily Kapitanchuk","doi":"10.1177/14747049231185782","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231185782","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Human appearance enhancement effort has recently been considered from an evolutionary perspective as an adaptive and sexually dimorphic strategy for effective female intrasexual and intersexual competition. Most writing and research on the topic to date has focused on appearance enhancement as a means of mate attraction, with relatively less research examining its role in mate retention. The present study considered whether romantic jealousy, as a negative emotion experienced in response to perceived threat to a desired relationship, predicts costly and/or risky appearance enhancement independent of the closely related emotion of envy. In a sample of 189 undergraduate women, results showed that romantic jealousy and dispositional envy were positively correlated with one another. Results further demonstrated that romantic jealousy predicted women's positive attitude toward cosmetic surgery, willingness to use a one-week free tanning membership, willingness to use a risky diet pill, and intent on spending a greater proportion of their income on appearance enhancement, but not intended use of facial cosmetics. Results held independent of participants' dispositional envy, suggesting that romantic jealousy is a unique predictor of women's efforts at enhancing their physical appearance, which could extend into costly and physically risky mate retention efforts.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10387690/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10074712","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
The Effect of Asymmetric Intersexual Selection Power Perception on the Choice Deferral Behavior of Men and Women. 非对称性选择权知觉对男性和女性选择推迟行为的影响。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231193993
Cheng-Wen Jin, Rui Chen

Consumers always delay their choices, which can cause companies to suffer tremendous losses. One reason for such delay is a lack of confidence. Confidence in consumer decision-making can stem from many sources, including social power. In this research, we find that selection power with regard to choosing a romantic mate increases consumers' decision confidence and, in turn, decreases choice deferral. We define the concept of intersexual selection power (ISP), and propose certain factors that can induce individuals' asymmetric ISP. We conducted four studies to explore four factors that could influence consumers' ISP perceptions (sex, mating cues, sex ratio, and mate value) and the effects of such power perception on choice deferral. The results showed that individuals with high ISP perception have more decision confidence and a lower choice deferral rate than individuals with low ISP perception regardless of the way in which choice deferral is measured.

消费者总是拖延他们的选择,这可能会导致公司遭受巨大损失。造成这种拖延的一个原因是缺乏信心。对消费者决策的信心可以来源于许多方面,包括社会力量。在这项研究中,我们发现,选择浪漫伴侣的选择力会增加消费者的决策信心,进而减少选择延迟。我们定义了双性选择力的概念,并提出了导致个体不对称选择力的某些因素。我们进行了四项研究,探讨了可能影响消费者ISP感知的四个因素(性别、交配线索、性别比例和配偶价值),以及这种权力感知对选择延迟的影响。结果表明,无论衡量选择延迟的方式如何,ISP感知度高的个体比ISP感知度低的个体有更高的决策信心和更低的选择延迟率。
{"title":"The Effect of Asymmetric Intersexual Selection Power Perception on the Choice Deferral Behavior of Men and Women.","authors":"Cheng-Wen Jin,&nbsp;Rui Chen","doi":"10.1177/14747049231193993","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231193993","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Consumers always delay their choices, which can cause companies to suffer tremendous losses. One reason for such delay is a lack of confidence. Confidence in consumer decision-making can stem from many sources, including social power. In this research, we find that selection power with regard to choosing a romantic mate increases consumers' decision confidence and, in turn, decreases choice deferral. We define the concept of intersexual selection power (ISP), and propose certain factors that can induce individuals' asymmetric ISP. We conducted four studies to explore four factors that could influence consumers' ISP perceptions (sex, mating cues, sex ratio, and mate value) and the effects of such power perception on choice deferral. The results showed that individuals with high ISP perception have more decision confidence and a lower choice deferral rate than individuals with low ISP perception regardless of the way in which choice deferral is measured.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/57/71/10.1177_14747049231193993.PMC10437045.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10127215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Shame System Operates With High Precision. 羞耻系统运行精度高。
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231203394
Alexie Leroux, Sébastien Hétu, Daniel Sznycer

Previous research indicates that the anticipatory shame an individual feels at the prospect of taking a disgraceful action closely tracks the degree to which local audiences, and even foreign audiences, devalue those individuals who take that action. This supports the proposition that the shame system (a) defends the individual against the threat of being devalued, and (b) balances the competing demands of operating effectively yet efficiently. The stimuli events used in previous research were highly variable in their perceived disgracefulness, ranging in rated shame and audience devaluation from low (e.g., missing the target in a throwing game) to high (e.g., being discovered cheating on one's spouse). But how precise is the tracking of audience devaluation by the shame system? Would shame track devaluation for events that are similarly low (or high) in disgracefulness? To answer this question, we conducted a study with participants from the United States and India. Participants were assigned, between-subjects, to one of two conditions: shame or audience devaluation. Within-subjects, participants rated three low-variation sets of 25 scenarios each, adapted from Mu, Kitayama, Han, & Gelfand (2015), which convey (a) appropriateness (e.g., yelling at a rock concert), (b) mild disgracefulness (e.g., yelling on the metro), and (c) disgracefulness (e.g., yelling in the library), all presented un-blocked, in random order. Consistent with previous research, shame tracked audience devaluation across the high-variation superset of 75 scenarios, both within and between cultures. Critically, shame tracked devaluation also within each of the three sets. The shame system operates with high precision.

先前的研究表明,一个人对采取不光彩行为的前景感到的预期羞耻感,与当地观众,甚至外国观众对采取这种行为的人的贬低程度密切相关。这支持了这样一种观点,即羞耻制度(a)保护个人免受贬值的威胁,以及(b)平衡有效而高效地运作的竞争需求。先前研究中使用的刺激事件在其感知到的不光彩方面存在很大差异,从低(例如,在投掷游戏中错过目标)到高(例如,被发现对配偶不忠),其羞耻感和观众贬值程度各不相同。但羞耻感系统对观众贬值的追踪有多精确?羞耻感会追踪同样低(或高)不光彩事件的贬值吗?为了回答这个问题,我们对来自美国和印度的参与者进行了一项研究。受试者被分为两种情况:羞耻或观众贬值。在受试者中,参与者对三组低变化场景进行了评分,每组25个场景,改编自Mu、Kitayama、Han和Gelfand(2015),这些场景传达了(a)适当性(例如,在摇滚音乐会上大喊大叫)、(b)轻度不光彩(例如,地铁上大喊大叫)和(c)不光彩(如,在图书馆大喊大叫),所有场景都以随机顺序呈现,没有被屏蔽。与之前的研究一致,羞耻感追踪了文化内部和文化之间75种场景的高变异超集中的观众贬值。至关重要的是,羞耻感追踪的贬值也出现在三套货币中的每一套中。羞耻感系统运行精度高。
{"title":"The Shame System Operates With High Precision.","authors":"Alexie Leroux, Sébastien Hétu, Daniel Sznycer","doi":"10.1177/14747049231203394","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231203394","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous research indicates that the anticipatory shame an individual feels at the prospect of taking a disgraceful action closely tracks the degree to which local audiences, and even foreign audiences, devalue those individuals who take that action. This supports the proposition that the shame system (a) defends the individual against the threat of being devalued, and (b) balances the competing demands of operating effectively yet efficiently. The stimuli events used in previous research were highly variable in their perceived disgracefulness, ranging in rated shame and audience devaluation from low (e.g., missing the target in a throwing game) to high (e.g., being discovered cheating on one's spouse). But how precise is the tracking of audience devaluation by the shame system? Would shame track devaluation for events that are similarly low (or high) in disgracefulness? To answer this question, we conducted a study with participants from the United States and India. Participants were assigned, between-subjects, to one of two conditions: shame or audience devaluation. Within-subjects, participants rated three low-variation sets of 25 scenarios each, adapted from Mu, Kitayama, Han, & Gelfand (2015), which convey (a) appropriateness (e.g., yelling at a rock concert), (b) mild disgracefulness (e.g., yelling on the metro), and (c) disgracefulness (e.g., yelling in the library), all presented un-blocked, in random order. Consistent with previous research, shame tracked audience devaluation across the high-variation superset of 75 scenarios, both within and between cultures. Critically, shame tracked devaluation also within each of the three sets. The shame system operates with high precision.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10540588/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41148669","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No Support for Two Hypotheses About the Communicative Functions of Displaying Disgust: Evidence From Turkey, Norway, Germany, and Croatia. 关于厌恶的交际功能的两种假设不支持:来自土耳其、挪威、德国和克罗地亚的证据。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231179408
Laith Al-Shawaf, David M G Lewis, Maliki E Ghossainy, Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair, Igor Mikloušić, Sascha Schwarz, Kaitlyn P White

In recent years, researchers have discovered much about how disgust works, its neural basis, its relationship with immune function, its connection with mating, and some of its antecedents and consequents. Despite these advances in our understanding, an under-explored area is how disgust may be used to serve a communicative function, including how individuals might strategically downplay or exaggerate the disgust display in front of different audiences. Here, we generated two hypotheses about potential communicative functions of disgust, and tested these hypotheses in four countries (Turkey, Croatia, Germany, and Norway). We found no evidence in support of either hypothesis in any country. Discussion focuses on the likely falsity of the two central hypotheses, alternative interpretations of our findings, and directions for future research.

近年来,研究人员已经发现了很多关于厌恶是如何工作的,它的神经基础,它与免疫功能的关系,它与交配的联系,以及它的一些前因和结果。尽管我们的理解取得了这些进步,但一个未被探索的领域是厌恶如何被用来服务于交际功能,包括个人如何在不同的观众面前策略性地淡化或夸大厌恶的表现。在这里,我们提出了两个关于厌恶潜在交际功能的假设,并在四个国家(土耳其、克罗地亚、德国和挪威)对这些假设进行了测试。我们在任何国家都没有发现支持这两种假设的证据。讨论的重点是两个中心假设可能的错误,对我们的发现的其他解释,以及未来研究的方向。
{"title":"No Support for Two Hypotheses About the Communicative Functions of Displaying Disgust: Evidence From Turkey, Norway, Germany, and Croatia.","authors":"Laith Al-Shawaf,&nbsp;David M G Lewis,&nbsp;Maliki E Ghossainy,&nbsp;Leif Edward Ottesen Kennair,&nbsp;Igor Mikloušić,&nbsp;Sascha Schwarz,&nbsp;Kaitlyn P White","doi":"10.1177/14747049231179408","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049231179408","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In recent years, researchers have discovered much about how disgust works, its neural basis, its relationship with immune function, its connection with mating, and some of its antecedents and consequents. Despite these advances in our understanding, an under-explored area is how disgust may be used to serve a communicative function, including how individuals might strategically downplay or exaggerate the disgust display in front of different audiences. Here, we generated two hypotheses about potential communicative functions of disgust, and tested these hypotheses in four countries (Turkey, Croatia, Germany, and Norway). We found no evidence in support of either hypothesis in any country. Discussion focuses on the likely falsity of the two central hypotheses, alternative interpretations of our findings, and directions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/41/1c/10.1177_14747049231179408.PMC10355289.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10393510","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attractiveness Differentially Affects Direct Versus Indirect Face Evaluations in Two Cultures. 在两种文化中,吸引力对直接和间接面孔评价的影响是不同的。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231186119
Brittany S Cassidy, S Adil Saribay, Hüseyin Yüksel, Karel Kleisner

Although decades of research have identified facial features relating to people's evaluations of faces, specific features have largely been examined in isolation from each other. Recent work shows that considering the relative importance of these features in face evaluations is important to test theoretical assumptions of impression formation. Here, we examined how two facial features of evolutionary interest, facial attractiveness and facial-width-to-height ratio (FWHR), relate to evaluations of faces across two cultures. Because face evaluations are typically directly measured via self-reports, we also examined whether these features exert differential effects on both direct and indirect face evaluations. Evaluations of standardized photos naturally varying in facial attractiveness and FWHR were collected using the Affect Misattribution Procedure in the United States and Turkey. When their relative contributions were considered in the same model, facial attractiveness, but not FWHR, related to face evaluations across cultures. This positive attractiveness effect was stronger for direct versus indirect evaluations across cultures. These findings highlight the importance of considering the relative contributions of facial features to evaluations across cultures and suggest a culturally invariant role of attractiveness when intentionally evaluating faces.

尽管几十年的研究已经确定了面部特征与人们对面部的评价有关,但具体的特征在很大程度上是相互孤立的。最近的研究表明,考虑到这些特征在面部评估中的相对重要性,对于测试印象形成的理论假设很重要。在这里,我们研究了两种具有进化意义的面部特征——面部吸引力和面部宽高比(FWHR)——与两种文化中对面部的评价之间的关系。由于面部评价通常是通过自我报告直接测量的,我们也研究了这些特征是否对直接和间接的面部评价产生不同的影响。在美国和土耳其,使用影响错误归因程序对面部吸引力和FWHR自然变化的标准化照片进行评估。当在同一模型中考虑他们的相对贡献时,面部吸引力,而不是FWHR,与不同文化的面部评估有关。这种积极的吸引力效应在跨文化的直接评价中强于间接评价。这些发现强调了考虑面部特征对不同文化评估的相对贡献的重要性,并表明在有意评估面部时,吸引力的作用在文化上是不变的。
{"title":"Attractiveness Differentially Affects Direct Versus Indirect Face Evaluations in Two Cultures.","authors":"Brittany S Cassidy,&nbsp;S Adil Saribay,&nbsp;Hüseyin Yüksel,&nbsp;Karel Kleisner","doi":"10.1177/14747049231186119","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049231186119","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although decades of research have identified facial features relating to people's evaluations of faces, specific features have largely been examined in isolation from each other. Recent work shows that considering the relative importance of these features in face evaluations is important to test theoretical assumptions of impression formation. Here, we examined how two facial features of evolutionary interest, facial attractiveness and facial-width-to-height ratio (FWHR), relate to evaluations of faces across two cultures. Because face evaluations are typically directly measured via self-reports, we also examined whether these features exert differential effects on both direct and indirect face evaluations. Evaluations of standardized photos naturally varying in facial attractiveness and FWHR were collected using the Affect Misattribution Procedure in the United States and Turkey. When their relative contributions were considered in the same model, facial attractiveness, but not FWHR, related to face evaluations across cultures. This positive attractiveness effect was stronger for direct versus indirect evaluations across cultures. These findings highlight the importance of considering the relative contributions of facial features to evaluations across cultures and suggest a culturally invariant role of attractiveness when intentionally evaluating faces.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/ee/e5/10.1177_14747049231186119.PMC10355303.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10399896","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Environmental Harshness, Life History, and Crystallized Intelligence of Chinese Adolescents. 中国青少年的环境恶劣、生活史和智力结晶。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231190051
Yuan Yuan Liu, Hui Jing Lu, Nan Zhu, Lei Chang

The present study examined longitudinal relations between adverse environment (at the community and family level), life history (LH) profile (conceptualized as a suite of behavioral and physical traits with converging adaptive functions), and crystalized intelligence (mathematics and vocabulary test scores) using data on 1,185 Chinese adolescents obtained from the China Family Panel Studies survey. Multilevel structural equation modeling indicates that early familial environmental harshness was negatively associated with slow LH profiles and crystalized intelligence, slow LH profiles were positively associated with crystallized intelligence, and early community-level environmental harshness strengthened the positive association between slow LH and crystalized cognitive abilities. The results underscore the importance of the childhood environment in fostering individual LH and cognitive development.

本研究考察了不利环境(在社区和家庭层面)、生活史(LH)概况(概念化为一套具有趋同适应功能的行为和身体特征)、,并利用中国家庭小组研究调查中获得的1185名中国青少年的数据,对其智力(数学和词汇测试成绩)进行了分析。多层次结构方程模型表明,早期家庭环境恶劣与慢LH谱和结晶智力呈负相关,慢LH谱与结晶智力呈正相关,早期社区环境恶劣强化了慢LH与结晶认知能力的正相关。研究结果强调了儿童环境在促进个体LH和认知发展方面的重要性。
{"title":"Environmental Harshness, Life History, and Crystallized Intelligence of Chinese Adolescents.","authors":"Yuan Yuan Liu,&nbsp;Hui Jing Lu,&nbsp;Nan Zhu,&nbsp;Lei Chang","doi":"10.1177/14747049231190051","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231190051","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present study examined longitudinal relations between adverse environment (at the community and family level), life history (LH) profile (conceptualized as a suite of behavioral and physical traits with converging adaptive functions), and crystalized intelligence (mathematics and vocabulary test scores) using data on 1,185 Chinese adolescents obtained from the China Family Panel Studies survey. Multilevel structural equation modeling indicates that early familial environmental harshness was negatively associated with slow LH profiles and crystalized intelligence, slow LH profiles were positively associated with crystallized intelligence, and early community-level environmental harshness strengthened the positive association between slow LH and crystalized cognitive abilities. The results underscore the importance of the childhood environment in fostering individual LH and cognitive development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/bb/62/10.1177_14747049231190051.PMC10392227.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10394002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Women's Preferences for Masculinity in Male Faces Are Predicted by Material Scarcity, But Not Time or Psychological Scarcity. 女性对男性脸上男性气概的偏好是由物质稀缺性预测的,而不是时间或心理稀缺性。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-07-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231175073
Anthony J Lee, Nikita K J McGuire

Facial femininity in men is purportedly used as a cue by women as a signal of parental quality and willingness to provide resources. Accordingly, in contexts where choosing a partner that will provide resources is more beneficial (e.g., when resources are scarce), women have shown an increase preference for facial femininity in male faces. However, domains of scarcity often covary, and it is, therefore, unclear whether these contextual shifts in facial masculinity/femininity preferences are specific to material scarcity (as implied by previous theory), or due to an unrelated domain of scarcity (e.g., time or psychological scarcity). Here, a sample of 823 women completed the Perceived Scarcity Scale, which measures three separate domains of scarcity: material scarcity, time scarcity, and psychological scarcity. Participants also rated the attractiveness of 42 male faces, which were measured on objective sexual dimorphism and perceived masculinity. Consistent with theory, material scarcity, and not time or psychological scarcity, was associated with a decreased preference for objective sexual dimorphism (i.e., an increased preference for facial femininity). This study provides evidence that women use sexual dimorphism as a cue to material resource provisioning potential when assessing men as a mate.

据称,男性的面部女性气质被女性用作父母素质和提供资源意愿的信号。因此,在选择一个能提供资源的伴侣更有益的情况下(例如,在资源稀缺的情况下),女性越来越倾向于男性面部的女性气质。然而,稀缺领域往往是相互关联的,因此,尚不清楚面部男性气质/女性气质偏好的这些背景变化是由于物质稀缺(如先前理论所暗示的),还是由于不相关的稀缺领域(如时间或心理稀缺)。在这里,823名女性完成了感知稀缺性量表,该量表测量了三个不同的稀缺领域:物质稀缺、时间稀缺和心理稀缺。参与者还对42张男性面孔的吸引力进行了评分,这些面孔是根据客观的两性异形和感知的男性气质来衡量的。与理论一致的是,物质匮乏,而不是时间或心理匮乏,与对客观两性异形的偏好减少有关(即,对面部女性气质的偏好增加)。这项研究提供了证据,证明女性在评估男性作为伴侣时,使用性二态性作为物质资源供应潜力的线索。
{"title":"Women's Preferences for Masculinity in Male Faces Are Predicted by Material Scarcity, But Not Time or Psychological Scarcity.","authors":"Anthony J Lee,&nbsp;Nikita K J McGuire","doi":"10.1177/14747049231175073","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231175073","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Facial femininity in men is purportedly used as a cue by women as a signal of parental quality and willingness to provide resources. Accordingly, in contexts where choosing a partner that will provide resources is more beneficial (e.g., when resources are scarce), women have shown an increase preference for facial femininity in male faces. However, domains of scarcity often covary, and it is, therefore, unclear whether these contextual shifts in facial masculinity/femininity preferences are specific to material scarcity (as implied by previous theory), or due to an unrelated domain of scarcity (e.g., time or psychological scarcity). Here, a sample of 823 women completed the Perceived Scarcity Scale, which measures three separate domains of scarcity: material scarcity, time scarcity, and psychological scarcity. Participants also rated the attractiveness of 42 male faces, which were measured on objective sexual dimorphism and perceived masculinity. Consistent with theory, material scarcity, and not time or psychological scarcity, was associated with a decreased preference for objective sexual dimorphism (i.e., an increased preference for facial femininity). This study provides evidence that women use sexual dimorphism as a cue to material resource provisioning potential when assessing men as a mate.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-07-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10517608/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41153137","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
An Experiment Using a Sexual Strategies Explanation to Alleviate Internalized Homophobia Among Men Who Have Sex With Men in China. 用性策略解释缓解中国男男性行为者内在同性恋恐惧症的实验。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231179151
Qianhui Gao, Jan Antfolk, Pekka Santtila

Some men who have sex with men (MSM) having more casual sex and sexual partners is interpreted to be a sign of mental disorder and used to justify negative attitudes toward them by some. MSM may internalize this attitude causing internalized homophobia (IH). According to the sexual strategies theory, MSM having more casual sex is the result of differences between men's and women's sexual strategies and is unrelated to sexual orientation. We investigated whether this explanation would reduce IH and improve mental health in MSM. We recruited 255 Chinese MSM online using Wenjuanxing, Douban, Weibo, Tieba, and Blued and divided them using simple randomization into an experimental group (n = 77; sexual strategies explanation provided), an active control group (n = 99; minority stress explanation provided), and a control group (n = 79; no intervention) with pretest (N = 255); a 1 week post-test (n = 195); and a 1 month follow-up test (n = 170) of outcome measures. IH, mental distress (MD), sort-term mating orientation, and risky sexual behaviors were measured online. The data were analyzed by SPSS 28. IH was associated with MD while being married was associated with IH and sexual contact with women with both more IH and MD as were not being masculine and self-identifying as straight. The sexual strategies explanation reduced IH related to pathologizing sexual behaviors and high-risk sexual behaviors while the minority stress explanation reduced MD. Providing a sexual strategies explanation may be used to de-stigmatize casual sex among MSM.

一些与男性发生性关系的男性(MSM)有更多的随意性行为和性伴侣,这被解释为精神障碍的迹象,并被一些人用来证明对他们持消极态度的理由。男同性恋者可能内化了这种态度,导致内化同性恋恐惧症(IH)。根据性策略理论,男男性接触者有更多的随意性行为是男女性策略不同的结果,与性取向无关。我们调查了这种解释是否会减少艾滋病毒和改善男同性恋者的心理健康。我们在问卷星、豆瓣、微博、贴吧和Blued等网站上招募了255名中国MSM,采用简单随机分组的方法将他们分为实验组(n = 77;提供性策略解释),积极对照组(n = 99;提供少数民族应激解释)和对照组(n = 79;无干预)加预试(N = 255);试验后1周(n = 195);并进行1个月的随访测试(n = 170)。IH、精神痛苦(MD)、短期交配取向和危险性行为在网上进行了测量。数据采用SPSS 28进行分析。IH与MD相关,而结婚则与IH和与不男性化且自认为是异性恋的女性发生性接触相关。性策略解释降低了与病理性行为和高危性行为相关的IH,而少数民族压力解释降低了MD。提供性策略解释可以用来消除MSM中随意性行为的污名化。
{"title":"An Experiment Using a Sexual Strategies Explanation to Alleviate Internalized Homophobia Among Men Who Have Sex With Men in China.","authors":"Qianhui Gao,&nbsp;Jan Antfolk,&nbsp;Pekka Santtila","doi":"10.1177/14747049231179151","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049231179151","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Some men who have sex with men (MSM) having more casual sex and sexual partners is interpreted to be a sign of mental disorder and used to justify negative attitudes toward them by some. MSM may internalize this attitude causing internalized homophobia (IH). According to the sexual strategies theory, MSM having more casual sex is the result of differences between men's and women's sexual strategies and is unrelated to sexual orientation. We investigated whether this explanation would reduce IH and improve mental health in MSM. We recruited 255 Chinese MSM online using Wenjuanxing, Douban, Weibo, Tieba, and Blued and divided them using simple randomization into an experimental group (n = 77; sexual strategies explanation provided), an active control group (n = 99; minority stress explanation provided), and a control group (n = 79; no intervention) with pretest (N = 255); a 1 week post-test (n = 195); and a 1 month follow-up test (n = 170) of outcome measures. IH, mental distress (MD), sort-term mating orientation, and risky sexual behaviors were measured online. The data were analyzed by SPSS 28. IH was associated with MD while being married was associated with IH and sexual contact with women with both more IH and MD as were not being masculine and self-identifying as straight. The sexual strategies explanation reduced IH related to pathologizing sexual behaviors and high-risk sexual behaviors while the minority stress explanation reduced MD. Providing a sexual strategies explanation may be used to de-stigmatize casual sex among MSM.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/9f/29/10.1177_14747049231179151.PMC10355304.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10392991","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion. 在有利的不平等厌恶中没有包容性适应度或互惠利他主义的迹象。
IF 1.5 4区 心理学 Q3 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-04-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231173401
Jan Antfolk, Emmie Marklund, Irene Nylund, Annika Gunst

Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6-8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4-8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6-8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion.

有利的不公平厌恶(即对有利于自己的不公平作出消极反应的倾向)通常在6-8岁的儿童中发展。然而,我们对可能形成这种现象的选择压力知之甚少。利用从120名4-8岁芬兰儿童中收集的数据,我们测试了两种进化解释:互惠利他主义(即,当角色在未来可能逆转时,从分享中受益)和包容性适应(即,从与携带相同等位基因的生物亲属分享中受益)。我们首先成功地复制了之前的一个实验,表明6-8岁的孩子表现出有利的不平等厌恶,他们更愿意扔掉资源,而不是自己留着。这里,这种行为也出现在5岁的孩子身上。通过一个新颖的实验,我们要求孩子们在自己、兄弟姐妹、同龄人和陌生人之间分发五块橡皮。也就是说,只有扔掉一块橡皮擦,才有可能实现平均分配。我们没有发现有利的不平等厌恶是由包容性适应度或互惠利他主义形成的。未来的研究可以调查代价高昂的信号和对社会规范的遵守,以避免负面后果,作为有利的不平等厌恶的最终解释。
{"title":"No Signs of Inclusive Fitness or Reciprocal Altruism in Advantageous Inequity Aversion.","authors":"Jan Antfolk,&nbsp;Emmie Marklund,&nbsp;Irene Nylund,&nbsp;Annika Gunst","doi":"10.1177/14747049231173401","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1177/14747049231173401","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Advantageous inequity aversion (i.e., the tendency to respond negatively to unfairness that benefits oneself) usually develops in 6-8-year-olds. However, little is known about the selection pressures that might have shaped this phenomenon. Using data collected from 120 4-8-year-old Finnish children, we tested two evolutionary explanations for the development of advantageous inequity aversion: reciprocal altruism (i.e., benefiting from sharing when the roles are likely reversed in the future) and inclusive fitness (i.e., benefiting from sharing with biological relatives that carry the same alleles). We first successfully replicated a previous experiment, showing that 6-8-year-olds display advantageous inequity aversion by preferring to throw away a resource rather than keep it for themselves. Here, this behavior was also displayed in 5-year-olds. Using a novel experiment, we then asked children to distribute five erasers between themselves, a sibling, a peer, and a stranger. That is, an equal distribution was only possible if throwing away one eraser. We found no support for advantageous inequity aversion being shaped by either inclusive fitness or reciprocal altruism. Future studies could investigate costly signaling and adherence to social norms to avoid negative consequences as ultimate explanations for advantageous inequity aversion.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.5,"publicationDate":"2023-04-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10303545/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10008571","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sperm Competition Risk: The Connections That Partner Attractiveness and Infidelity Risk Have with Mate Retention Behaviors and Semen-Displacing Behaviors. 精子竞争风险:伴侣吸引力和不忠风险与配偶保留行为和精液置换行为之间的联系》(Sperm Competition Risk: The Connections That Partner Attractiveness and Infidelity Risk Have with Mate Retention Behaviors and Semen-Displacing Behaviors.
IF 1.1 4区 心理学 Q4 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-01-01 DOI: 10.1177/14747049231161075
Gavin Vance, Virgil Zeigler-Hill, Todd K Shackelford

The present studies investigated the relationships between men's perceived risk of experiencing sperm competition (i.e., when the ejaculates of two or more men simultaneously occupy the reproductive tract of a single woman), and their use of strategies to detect, prevent, and correct their partner's sexual infidelity. We investigated these associations using self-reports provided by men (Study 1, n = 113), partner-reports provided by women (Study 2, n = 136), and dyadic reports (Study 3, n = 103 couples). The results of these studies indicated that the attractiveness of women was consistently associated with men's use of benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviors (e.g., buying expensive gifts for one's partner, showing signs of physical affection) and semen-displacing behaviors (e.g., deeper copulatory thrusting, more thrusts during copulation), whereas the infidelity risk of women was often associated with men's use of cost-inflicting mate retention behaviors (e.g., threatening to end the relationship, monopolization of partner's free time). Discussion addresses the evolutionary implications of these results, including the possibility that men use both benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviors and semen-displacing behaviors when they perceive their partner to be more attractive, ostensibly as a way to mitigate their risk of sperm competition. Discussion also explores the extent to which these results extend those of previous studies concerning sperm competition risk.

本研究调查了男性对精子竞争(即两名或两名以上男性的射精同时占据一名女性的生殖道)风险的感知,以及他们在发现、预防和纠正伴侣性不忠时所使用的策略之间的关系。我们使用男性提供的自我报告(研究 1,n = 113)、女性提供的伴侣报告(研究 2,n = 136)和夫妻报告(研究 3,n = 103 对夫妻)对这些关联进行了调查。这些研究结果表明,女性的吸引力始终与男性的利益提供型配偶保留行为(例如,为伴侣购买昂贵的礼物、表现出身体上的爱意)和精液置换行为(例如、而女性的不忠风险则往往与男性使用的影响配偶保留成本的行为(如威胁结束关系、垄断伴侣的空闲时间)有关。)讨论涉及了这些结果对进化的影响,包括当男性认为其伴侣更有吸引力时,他们可能会同时使用提供利益的配偶保留行为和精液置换行为,表面上这是一种减轻精子竞争风险的方法。讨论还探讨了这些结果在多大程度上扩展了之前有关精子竞争风险的研究。
{"title":"Sperm Competition Risk: The Connections That Partner Attractiveness and Infidelity Risk Have with Mate Retention Behaviors and Semen-Displacing Behaviors.","authors":"Gavin Vance, Virgil Zeigler-Hill, Todd K Shackelford","doi":"10.1177/14747049231161075","DOIUrl":"10.1177/14747049231161075","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The present studies investigated the relationships between men's perceived risk of experiencing sperm competition (i.e., when the ejaculates of two or more men simultaneously occupy the reproductive tract of a single woman), and their use of strategies to detect, prevent, and correct their partner's sexual infidelity. We investigated these associations using self-reports provided by men (Study 1, <i>n </i>= 113), partner-reports provided by women (Study 2, <i>n </i>= 136), and dyadic reports (Study 3, <i>n </i>= 103 couples). The results of these studies indicated that the attractiveness of women was consistently associated with men's use of benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviors (e.g., buying expensive gifts for one's partner, showing signs of physical affection) and semen-displacing behaviors (e.g., deeper copulatory thrusting, more thrusts during copulation), whereas the infidelity risk of women was often associated with men's use of cost-inflicting mate retention behaviors (e.g., threatening to end the relationship, monopolization of partner's free time). Discussion addresses the evolutionary implications of these results, including the possibility that men use both benefit-provisioning mate retention behaviors and semen-displacing behaviors when they perceive their partner to be more attractive, ostensibly as a way to mitigate their risk of sperm competition. Discussion also explores the extent to which these results extend those of previous studies concerning sperm competition risk.</p>","PeriodicalId":47499,"journal":{"name":"Evolutionary Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":1.1,"publicationDate":"2023-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://ftp.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pub/pmc/oa_pdf/76/0a/10.1177_14747049231161075.PMC10303476.pdf","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10014476","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Evolutionary Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1