首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
High-quality early care and education for low-income families: Toddlers’ cognitive and emotional functioning during the COVID-19 pandemic 为低收入家庭提供高质量的早期保育和教育:COVID-19大流行期间幼儿的认知和情绪功能。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12619
Lukas D. Lopez, Anabel Castillo, Elizabeth Frechette, Shinyoung Jeon, Sherri Castle, Diane Horm, Kyong-Ah Kwon

High-quality early care and education (ECE) programs are associated with positive outcomes, especially for children from low-income families. During the initial COVID-19 pandemic lockdown many of these families faced an abrupt halt to ECE. Here, we examined how toddlers from economically disadvantaged backgrounds enrolled in high-quality ECE programs in the United States during the 2020 pandemic (n = 48) fared on cognitive and socioemotional outcomes compared to a 2019 pre-pandemic cohort (n = 94) and a pandemic 2021 cohort (n = 132). Toddlers in the 2020 cohort scored significantly lower on executive function compared to toddlers in 2019 and 2021 cohorts. Toddlers in the 2020 cohort had higher ratings self-regulation compared to the pre-pandemic cohort, but not 2021 cohort. There were no differences on attachment ratings between cohorts. Findings suggest that the abrupt halt to ECE programs due to the COVID-19 pandemic impacted US toddlers’ cognitive and socioemotional abilities. This underscores the importance of continued high-quality ECE for infants and toddlers from low-income families during disruptive times. Further work is needed to investigate the long-term impacts of experiencing an abrupt halt to ECE due to COVID-19.

高质量的早期保育和教育(ECE)计划与积极的成果息息相关,尤其是对低收入家庭的儿童而言。在最初的 COVID-19 大流行封锁期间,许多这些家庭面临着幼教突然中断的情况。在此,我们研究了在 2020 年大流行期间,与 2019 年大流行前队列(n = 94)和 2021 年大流行队列(n = 132)相比,在美国优质幼教项目中就读的经济条件较差的幼儿(n = 48)在认知和社会情感方面的表现。与 2019 年和 2021 年队列中的幼儿相比,2020 年队列中的幼儿在执行功能方面的得分明显较低。与大流行前的组群相比,2020 年组群的幼儿在自我调节方面的评分较高,但 2021 年组群的幼儿则不然。不同组群之间的依恋评分没有差异。研究结果表明,COVID-19 大流行导致的幼教计划突然停止影响了美国幼儿的认知和社会情感能力。这强调了在混乱时期继续为低收入家庭的婴幼儿提供高质量幼儿教育的重要性。还需要进一步开展工作,研究因 COVID-19 而突然停止幼教所造成的长期影响。
{"title":"High-quality early care and education for low-income families: Toddlers’ cognitive and emotional functioning during the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Lukas D. Lopez,&nbsp;Anabel Castillo,&nbsp;Elizabeth Frechette,&nbsp;Shinyoung Jeon,&nbsp;Sherri Castle,&nbsp;Diane Horm,&nbsp;Kyong-Ah Kwon","doi":"10.1111/infa.12619","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12619","url":null,"abstract":"<p>High-quality early care and education (ECE) programs are associated with positive outcomes, especially for children from low-income families. During the initial COVID-19 pandemic lockdown many of these families faced an abrupt halt to ECE. Here, we examined how toddlers from economically disadvantaged backgrounds enrolled in high-quality ECE programs in the United States during the 2020 pandemic (<i>n</i> = 48) fared on cognitive and socioemotional outcomes compared to a 2019 pre-pandemic cohort (<i>n</i> = 94) and a pandemic 2021 cohort (<i>n</i> = 132). Toddlers in the 2020 cohort scored significantly lower on executive function compared to toddlers in 2019 and 2021 cohorts. Toddlers in the 2020 cohort had higher ratings self-regulation compared to the pre-pandemic cohort, but not 2021 cohort. There were no differences on attachment ratings between cohorts. Findings suggest that the abrupt halt to ECE programs due to the COVID-19 pandemic impacted US toddlers’ cognitive and socioemotional abilities. This underscores the importance of continued high-quality ECE for infants and toddlers from low-income families during disruptive times. Further work is needed to investigate the long-term impacts of experiencing an abrupt halt to ECE due to COVID-19.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"983-1001"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12619","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142037298","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants' individuation of human faces across race and identity 不同种族和身份的婴儿对人脸的识别。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12618
Charisse B. Pickron, Isabella C. Stallworthy, Erik W. Cheries

Young infants' face perception capabilities quickly tune to the features of their primary caregiver. The current study examines whether infants distinguish faces in a more conceptual manner using a manual-search, violation of expectation task that has previously been used to test kind-based individuation. We tested how well infants between 11 and 27 months of age individuated faces that varied by superordinate category (human vs. non-human in Experiment 1) subordinate category (individual identity in Experiment 2) or by race (White vs. Black, Experiments 1 & 2). We assessed individuation by quantifying the difference in infants' duration of reaching within an empty box between trials when the box was unexpectedly empty and expectedly empty. We found evidence of individuation across all ages and conditions, but with within-infant variation. On average, infants individuated face from non-face stimuli (Experiment 1), individual face identities (Experiment 2), and White versus Black faces (Experiments 1 & 2). These findings suggest that 1- to 2-year-old infants use distinct human face features to represent individuals across time and space. We discuss this evidence for race-based individuation and related complexities of face identity in terms of implications for conceptual development for faces in the first years of life.

年幼婴儿的面孔感知能力很快就会适应其主要照顾者的特征。本研究通过手动搜索、违反期望的任务来检验婴儿是否能以更概念化的方式区分人脸。我们测试了 11 到 27 个月大的婴儿对不同上位类别(实验 1 中的人类和非人类)、下位类别(实验 2 中的个人身份)或种族(实验 1 和 2 中的白人和黑人)的人脸的识别能力。我们通过量化婴儿在空盒子意外为空时和预期为空时伸手的持续时间差异来评估个体化。我们在所有年龄段和条件下都发现了个体化的证据,但在婴儿内部也存在差异。平均而言,婴儿能从非人脸刺激中识别出人脸(实验 1)、识别出个人人脸身份(实验 2)以及识别出白脸和黑脸(实验 1 和 2)。这些研究结果表明,1-2 岁的婴儿使用不同的人脸特征来代表不同时间和空间的个体。我们将从对生命最初几年人脸概念发展的影响的角度,讨论基于种族的个体化和相关的人脸身份复杂性的证据。
{"title":"Infants' individuation of human faces across race and identity","authors":"Charisse B. Pickron,&nbsp;Isabella C. Stallworthy,&nbsp;Erik W. Cheries","doi":"10.1111/infa.12618","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12618","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Young infants' face perception capabilities quickly tune to the features of their primary caregiver. The current study examines whether infants distinguish faces in a more conceptual manner using a manual-search, violation of expectation task that has previously been used to test kind-based individuation. We tested how well infants between 11 and 27 months of age individuated faces that varied by superordinate category (human vs. non-human in Experiment 1) subordinate category (individual identity in Experiment 2) or by race (White vs. Black, Experiments 1 &amp; 2). We assessed individuation by quantifying the difference in infants' duration of reaching within an empty box between trials when the box was unexpectedly empty and expectedly empty. We found evidence of individuation across all ages and conditions, but with within-infant variation. On average, infants individuated face from non-face stimuli (Experiment 1), individual face identities (Experiment 2), and White versus Black faces (Experiments 1 &amp; 2). These findings suggest that 1- to 2-year-old infants use distinct human face features to represent individuals across time and space. We discuss this evidence for race-based individuation and related complexities of face identity in terms of implications for conceptual development for faces in the first years of life.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"958-982"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12618","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142037299","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Processing of visual stimuli following infant directed speech: Attention-guiding effects of unfamiliar speech 婴儿引导性言语后的视觉刺激处理:陌生语言的注意力引导效应
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12611
Stefanie Peykarjou, Julia Wissner, Sabina Pauen

This study investigates attention modulation as a function of infant directed (ID) versus adult directed (AD) speech in seven-month-old infants using electroencephalographic measures. In three experiments, infants were presented with either ID speech or AD speech as stimuli, followed by highly variable images of inanimate objects as targets. In Experiment 1 (N = 18), images were preceded by ID or AD speech with semantic content (“Look here”). Contrary to hypothesis, targets preceded by AD speech elicited increased amplitude of the Negative central (Nc) component compared to targets preceded by ID speech, indicating increased attention. Experiment 2 (N = 23) explored whether ID versus AD speech influences attention allocation also without semantic content. The same targets were either preceded by human voice sounds without semantic content (“Uh-Ah”) following the prosody of either ID or AD speech register. No differences in attention allocation or object processing were observed. Experiment 3 (N = 18) contrasted ID speech with and without semantic content and found enhanced attention allocation following stimuli without semantic content, but increased object processing following stimuli with semantic content. Overall, the effects observed here are consistent with the idea that less familiar speech stimuli increase attention for subsequent objects. Semantic content of stimuli increased the depth of object processing in 7-month-olds.

本研究采用脑电图测量方法,研究了七个月大婴儿在婴儿引导(ID)和成人引导(AD)言语作用下的注意力调节。在三项实验中,婴儿先接受婴儿引导式语言或成人引导式语言的刺激,然后再接受高度可变的无生命物体图像作为目标。在实验 1(N = 18)中,图像之前先出现带有语义内容的 ID 或 AD 言语("看这里")。与假设相反的是,与 ID 讲话前的目标相比,AD 讲话前的目标引起的负中心(Nc)成分振幅增大,这表明注意力增加了。实验 2(N = 23)同样在没有语义内容的情况下,探讨了 ID 与 AD 语音是否会影响注意力分配。同样的目标物在 ID 或 AD 语域的前音之后,会出现没有语义内容的人声("啊-啊")。在注意力分配或目标处理方面没有观察到任何差异。实验 3(N = 18)对比了有语义内容和无语义内容的 ID 语音,发现无语义内容的刺激会增强注意力分配,但有语义内容的刺激会增强对象处理。总的来说,这里观察到的效果与不太熟悉的语音刺激会增加对后续对象的注意这一观点是一致的。刺激的语义内容增加了 7 个月大幼儿处理物体的深度。
{"title":"Processing of visual stimuli following infant directed speech: Attention-guiding effects of unfamiliar speech","authors":"Stefanie Peykarjou,&nbsp;Julia Wissner,&nbsp;Sabina Pauen","doi":"10.1111/infa.12611","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12611","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study investigates attention modulation as a function of infant directed (ID) versus adult directed (AD) speech in seven-month-old infants using electroencephalographic measures. In three experiments, infants were presented with either ID speech or AD speech as stimuli, followed by highly variable images of inanimate objects as targets. In Experiment 1 (<i>N</i> = 18), images were preceded by ID or AD speech with semantic content (“Look here”). Contrary to hypothesis, targets preceded by AD speech elicited increased amplitude of the Negative central (Nc) component compared to targets preceded by ID speech, indicating increased attention. Experiment 2 (<i>N</i> = 23) explored whether ID versus AD speech influences attention allocation also without semantic content. The same targets were either preceded by human voice sounds without semantic content (“Uh-Ah”) following the prosody of either ID or AD speech register. No differences in attention allocation or object processing were observed. Experiment 3 (<i>N</i> = 18) contrasted ID speech with and without semantic content and found enhanced attention allocation following stimuli without semantic content, but increased object processing following stimuli with semantic content. Overall, the effects observed here are consistent with the idea that less familiar speech stimuli increase attention for subsequent objects. Semantic content of stimuli increased the depth of object processing in 7-month-olds.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"789-810"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12611","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761731","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Contingency enables the formation of social expectations about an artificial agent 权变可以形成对人工智能的社会期望。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12614
Julia A. Venditti, Rachel Elkin, Rondeline M. Williams, Jennifer A. Schwade, Angela Narayan, Michael H. Goldstein

What environmental regularities support infant communicative learning from social interactions? We propose that infants allocate their attention toward and learn from external events that are contingent on their own behaviors. We tested the robustness of the influence of contingency on communicative learning by using a non-biological stimulus, a remote-controlled car, as the social partner. The car approached infants and produced a speech sound either contingently to infants' vocalizations or on a yoked schedule. Two additional groups had an unfamiliar human experimenter as their social partner in contingent and yoked control conditions. We assessed whether infants formed expectations about their partner's responsiveness to their vocalizations. Expectations made based on contingent responsiveness would support the role of contingency in promoting plasticity in early communicative learning. Infants across all conditions increased their vocalization rates when their partner paused in responding, suggesting that they expected their vocalizations to influence their partners' behavior. Infants vocalized significantly more to the social partner than their caregiver if they received contingent rather than yoked responses from the social partner, regardless of if the partner was a human or non-biological agent. Contingent responses to prelinguistic vocalizations facilitated the formation of expectations for interactivity of social partners.

是什么环境规律支持婴儿从社会互动中进行交流学习?我们提出,婴儿会将注意力分配给与自身行为相关的外部事件,并从中学习。我们使用非生物刺激物--一辆遥控汽车--作为社交伙伴,测试了偶然性对交流学习影响的稳健性。小汽车靠近婴儿,并根据婴儿的发声或按计划发出言语声音。另外两组婴儿的社会伙伴分别是一名陌生的人类实验者,他们分别处于或然控制和牵制控制条件下。我们评估了婴儿是否会对同伴是否会回应他们的发声形成预期。基于或然反应而产生的预期将支持或然性在促进早期交流学习的可塑性方面所起的作用。当伴侣暂停回应时,所有条件下的婴儿都会提高发声率,这表明他们期望自己的发声会影响伴侣的行为。如果婴儿从社交伙伴那里得到的是条件反应而不是牵制反应,那么他们对社交伙伴的发声明显多于对其看护人的发声,无论社交伙伴是人类还是非生物。对前语言发声的条件反应有助于形成对社会伙伴互动性的期望。
{"title":"Contingency enables the formation of social expectations about an artificial agent","authors":"Julia A. Venditti,&nbsp;Rachel Elkin,&nbsp;Rondeline M. Williams,&nbsp;Jennifer A. Schwade,&nbsp;Angela Narayan,&nbsp;Michael H. Goldstein","doi":"10.1111/infa.12614","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12614","url":null,"abstract":"<p>What environmental regularities support infant communicative learning from social interactions? We propose that infants allocate their attention toward and learn from external events that are contingent on their own behaviors. We tested the robustness of the influence of contingency on communicative learning by using a non-biological stimulus, a remote-controlled car, as the social partner. The car approached infants and produced a speech sound either contingently to infants' vocalizations or on a yoked schedule. Two additional groups had an unfamiliar human experimenter as their social partner in contingent and yoked control conditions. We assessed whether infants formed expectations about their partner's responsiveness to their vocalizations. Expectations made based on contingent responsiveness would support the role of contingency in promoting plasticity in early communicative learning. Infants across all conditions increased their vocalization rates when their partner paused in responding, suggesting that they expected their vocalizations to influence their partners' behavior. Infants vocalized significantly more to the social partner than their caregiver if they received contingent rather than yoked responses from the social partner, regardless of if the partner was a human or non-biological agent. Contingent responses to prelinguistic vocalizations facilitated the formation of expectations for interactivity of social partners.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761730","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Language environment and early language production in Slovenian infants: An exploratory study using daylong recordings 斯洛文尼亚婴儿的语言环境和早期语言生成:使用全天录音进行的探索性研究。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12615
Naja Ferjan Ramírez, Ljubica Marjanovič Umek, Urška Fekonja

Daylong recordings provide an ecologically valid option for analyzing language input, and have become a central method for studying child language development. However, the vast majority of this work has been conducted in North America. We harnessed a unique collection of daylong recordings from Slovenian infants (age: 16–30 months, N = 40, 18 girls), and focus our attention on manually annotated measures of parentese (infant-directed speech with a higher pitch, slower tempo, and exaggerated intonation), conversational turns, infant words, and word combinations. Measures from daylong recordings showed large variation, but were comparable to previous studies with North American samples. Infants heard almost twice as much speech and parentese from mothers compared to fathers, but there were no differences in language input to boys and girls. Positive associations were found between the social-interactional features of language input (parentese, turn-taking) and infants' concurrent language production. Measures of child speech from daylong recordings were positively correlated with measures obtained through the Slovenian MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory. These results support the notion that the social-interactional features of parental language input are the foundation of infants' language skills, even in an environment where infants spend much of their waking hours in childcare settings, as they do in Slovenia.

长达一天的录音为分析语言输入提供了一种生态学上有效的选择,并已成为研究儿童语言发展的核心方法。然而,绝大多数研究工作都是在北美进行的。我们从斯洛文尼亚婴儿(年龄:16-30 个月,N = 40,18 个女孩)中收集了独特的日间录音,并将注意力集中在人工标注的父母语(音调较高、节奏较慢、语调夸张的婴儿引导语)、会话转折、婴儿单词和单词组合的测量上。对全天录音的测量结果显示出很大的差异,但与之前对北美样本的研究结果相当。与父亲相比,婴儿从母亲那里听到的言语和家长用语几乎是父亲的两倍,但对男孩和女孩的语言输入没有差异。研究发现,语言输入的社交互动特征(家长语、轮流发言)与婴儿同时产生的语言之间存在正相关。通过日间录音获得的儿童语言测量结果与斯洛文尼亚麦克阿瑟-贝茨交际发展量表(Slovenian MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory)获得的测量结果呈正相关。这些结果支持这样一种观点,即父母语言输入的社会互动特征是婴儿语言技能的基础,即使像斯洛文尼亚那样,婴儿醒着的大部分时间都是在托儿所度过的。
{"title":"Language environment and early language production in Slovenian infants: An exploratory study using daylong recordings","authors":"Naja Ferjan Ramírez,&nbsp;Ljubica Marjanovič Umek,&nbsp;Urška Fekonja","doi":"10.1111/infa.12615","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12615","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Daylong recordings provide an ecologically valid option for analyzing language input, and have become a central method for studying child language development. However, the vast majority of this work has been conducted in North America. We harnessed a unique collection of daylong recordings from Slovenian infants (age: 16–30 months, <i>N</i> = 40, 18 girls), and focus our attention on manually annotated measures of parentese (infant-directed speech with a higher pitch, slower tempo, and exaggerated intonation), conversational turns, infant words, and word combinations. Measures from daylong recordings showed large variation, but were comparable to previous studies with North American samples. Infants heard almost twice as much speech and parentese from mothers compared to fathers, but there were no differences in language input to boys and girls. Positive associations were found between the social-interactional features of language input (parentese, turn-taking) and infants' concurrent language production. Measures of child speech from daylong recordings were positively correlated with measures obtained through the Slovenian MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory. These results support the notion that the social-interactional features of parental language input are the foundation of infants' language skills, even in an environment where infants spend much of their waking hours in childcare settings, as they do in Slovenia.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"811-837"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141753110","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
“My baby is ready to learn”—The role of infant pointing in redirecting maternal responses to be more informative "我的宝宝准备好学习了"--婴儿的指点在重新引导母亲做出更多信息反应中的作用。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12616
Edna Orr, Gabriela Kashy Rosenbaum

Caregivers may perceive pointing as an indication of infants' readiness to learn, thereby increasing their tendency to label objects regardless of the infant's gesture type and context. This was investigated in this study by tracking 35 infants at home at the ages of 11 and 13 months and observing their interactions with their mothers during object manipulation. We focused on four types of communicative gestures: typical giving gestures, gestures contingent on exploration, gestures contingent on play, and pointing. We analyzed maternal response tendencies, including affirmation, naming, discourse, and pretense. The results revealed that when infants reached the age of 13 months, they tripled their pointing production; in turn, the maternal response changed entirely, with naming becoming the preferred response to all types of gestures. Furthermore, when infants were 13 months old and offered an object contingent on play acts, mothers increased their pretense acts sevenfold. Based on the most informative responses to infants among those examined, we argue that an increase in the number of pointing gestures may gradually be associated with the establishment of the maternal perception that an infant is ready to learn and a subsequent increase in naming and pretense production by the mother.

照看者可能会认为婴儿用手指表示他们准备好学习,从而增加他们给物体贴标签的倾向,而不管婴儿的手势类型和背景如何。本研究通过跟踪 35 名 11 个月和 13 个月大的婴儿在家的情况,观察他们在操作物体时与母亲的互动,对此进行了研究。我们重点研究了四种类型的交流手势:典型的给予手势、探索手势、游戏手势和指向手势。我们分析了母亲的反应倾向,包括肯定、命名、话语和伪装。结果表明,当婴儿长到 13 个月大时,他们的指点动作增加了两倍;反过来,母性的反应也完全改变了,在所有类型的手势中,命名成为首选反应。此外,当婴儿长到 13 个月大并以游戏行为为条件提供物品时,母亲的假装行为增加了七倍。根据所研究的婴儿中信息量最大的反应,我们认为,指点手势数量的增加可能与母亲认为婴儿已准备好学习以及随后母亲命名和矫饰行为的增加有关。
{"title":"“My baby is ready to learn”—The role of infant pointing in redirecting maternal responses to be more informative","authors":"Edna Orr,&nbsp;Gabriela Kashy Rosenbaum","doi":"10.1111/infa.12616","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12616","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Caregivers may perceive pointing as an indication of infants' readiness to learn, thereby increasing their tendency to label objects regardless of the infant's gesture type and context. This was investigated in this study by tracking 35 infants at home at the ages of 11 and 13 months and observing their interactions with their mothers during object manipulation. We focused on four types of communicative gestures: typical giving gestures, gestures contingent on exploration, gestures contingent on play, and pointing. We analyzed maternal response tendencies, including affirmation, naming, discourse, and pretense. The results revealed that when infants reached the age of 13 months, they tripled their pointing production; in turn, the maternal response changed entirely, with naming becoming the preferred response to all types of gestures. Furthermore, when infants were 13 months old and offered an object contingent on play acts, mothers increased their pretense acts sevenfold. Based on the most informative responses to infants among those examined, we argue that an increase in the number of pointing gestures may gradually be associated with the establishment of the maternal perception that an infant is ready to learn and a subsequent increase in naming and pretense production by the mother.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"908-932"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141731512","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not too shy to point! Exploring the relationship between shyness and pointing in the second year 不害羞地指点!探索二年级学生害羞与指点之间的关系。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12610
Cristina Colonnesi, Eliala A. Salvadori, Frans J. Oort, Daniel S. Messinger

Infants' use of pointing gestures to direct and share attention develops during the first 2 years of life. Shyness, defined as an approach-avoidance motivational conflict during social interactions, may influence infants’ use of pointing. Recent research distinguished between positive (gaze and/or head aversions while smiling) and non-positive (gaze and/or head aversions without smiling) shyness, which are related to different social and cognitive skills. We investigated whether positive and non-positive shyness in 12-month-old (n = 38; 15 girls) and 15-month-old (n = 45; 15 girls) infants were associated with their production of pointing gestures. Infants' expressions of shyness were observed during a social-exposure task in which the infant entered the laboratory room in their parent's arms and was welcomed by an unfamiliar person who provided attention and compliments. Infants' pointing was measured with a pointing task involving three stimuli: pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral. Positive shyness was positively associated with overall pointing at 15 months, especially in combination with high levels of non-positive shyness. In addition, infants who displayed more non-positive shyness pointed more frequently to direct the attention of the social partner to an unpleasant (vs. neutral) stimulus at both ages. Results indicate that shyness influences the early use of pointing to emotionally charged stimuli.

婴儿使用指向手势来引导和分享注意力是在出生后的头 2 年中逐渐形成的。害羞被定义为社会交往中的接近-回避动机冲突,它可能会影响婴儿使用手指。最近的研究区分了积极害羞(微笑时目光和/或头部回避)和非积极害羞(不微笑时目光和/或头部回避),它们与不同的社交和认知技能有关。我们研究了 12 个月大(38 个;15 个女孩)和 15 个月大(45 个;15 个女孩)婴儿的积极害羞和非积极害羞是否与他们做出指点手势有关。在一项社会接触任务中,婴儿在父母的怀抱中进入实验室房间,并受到一名陌生人员的欢迎,该人员会给予关注和赞美,在此过程中,婴儿的害羞表现会被观察到。婴儿的指点能力是通过指点任务测量的,该任务包括三种刺激:令人愉快的、令人不快的和中性的。15 个月大时,积极害羞与婴儿的整体指点能力呈正相关,尤其是在非积极害羞水平较高的情况下。此外,在两个月龄时,表现出更多非积极害羞情绪的婴儿会更频繁地指向不愉快(相对于中性)刺激,以引导社交伙伴的注意力。研究结果表明,害羞会影响婴儿早期对情绪性刺激物使用手指。
{"title":"Not too shy to point! Exploring the relationship between shyness and pointing in the second year","authors":"Cristina Colonnesi,&nbsp;Eliala A. Salvadori,&nbsp;Frans J. Oort,&nbsp;Daniel S. Messinger","doi":"10.1111/infa.12610","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12610","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Infants' use of pointing gestures to direct and share attention develops during the first 2 years of life. Shyness, defined as an approach-avoidance motivational conflict during social interactions, may influence infants’ use of pointing. Recent research distinguished between positive (gaze and/or head aversions while smiling) and non-positive (gaze and/or head aversions without smiling) shyness, which are related to different social and cognitive skills. We investigated whether positive and non-positive shyness in 12-month-old (<i>n</i> = 38; 15 girls) and 15-month-old (<i>n</i> = 45; 15 girls) infants were associated with their production of pointing gestures. Infants' expressions of shyness were observed during a social-exposure task in which the infant entered the laboratory room in their parent's arms and was welcomed by an unfamiliar person who provided attention and compliments. Infants' pointing was measured with a pointing task involving three stimuli: pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral. Positive shyness was positively associated with overall pointing at 15 months, especially in combination with high levels of non-positive shyness. In addition, infants who displayed more non-positive shyness pointed more frequently to direct the attention of the social partner to an unpleasant (vs. neutral) stimulus at both ages. Results indicate that shyness influences the early use of pointing to emotionally charged stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"693-712"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12610","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141731513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddler's memory and media—Picture book reading and watching video content are associated with memory at 2 years of age 幼儿的记忆力与媒体--阅读图画书和观看视频内容与 2 岁幼儿的记忆力有关。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12609
Felix-Sebastian Koch, Annette Sundqvist, Ulrika Birberg Thornberg, Rachel Barr, Mikael Heimann

Memory develops across the course of the first years of life and is influenced by daily experiences, such as exposure to media like books and television. Memory as tapped by Deferred imitation (DI) requires that toddlers form a representation of the target actions that they can later use to reproduce the actions and in addition to measuring memory for real live events, it can also be used to measure memory for events viewed through media. Toddlers are frequently exposed to multiple forms of digital media in addition to more traditional forms of picture book reading. In a within-subjects design, memory was assessed with a DI task in 2-year-olds (n = 89) using the Frankfurt Imitation Test. Deferred imitation was assessed after live and video demonstrations. Parents completed a survey about children's media use. Picture book reading for less than 30 min a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated live. Watching video content for more than 1 h a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated on video. Results are interpreted in terms of individual differences in experiences of traditional and digital media and the development of symbolic understanding.

记忆力的发展贯穿于生命的最初几年,并受到日常经历的影响,如接触书籍和电视等媒体。延迟模仿(DI)的记忆要求学步儿童形成目标动作的表象,以便他们以后可以用来再现这些动作,除了测量对真实事件的记忆外,它还可以用来测量对通过媒体观看的事件的记忆。除了更传统的图画书阅读形式外,幼儿还经常接触多种形式的数字媒体。在受试者内设计中,我们使用法兰克福模仿测试对两岁幼儿(n = 89)的 DI 任务进行了记忆评估。在现场和视频演示后,对延迟模仿进行了评估。家长填写了一份有关儿童使用媒体情况的调查表。每天阅读图画书少于 30 分钟的儿童对现场演示动作的记忆得分较低。每天观看视频内容的时间超过 1 小时,则预示孩子对视频演示动作的记忆得分较低。研究从传统媒体和数字媒体体验的个体差异以及符号理解的发展角度对结果进行了解释。
{"title":"Toddler's memory and media—Picture book reading and watching video content are associated with memory at 2 years of age","authors":"Felix-Sebastian Koch,&nbsp;Annette Sundqvist,&nbsp;Ulrika Birberg Thornberg,&nbsp;Rachel Barr,&nbsp;Mikael Heimann","doi":"10.1111/infa.12609","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12609","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Memory develops across the course of the first years of life and is influenced by daily experiences, such as exposure to media like books and television. Memory as tapped by Deferred imitation (DI) requires that toddlers form a representation of the target actions that they can later use to reproduce the actions and in addition to measuring memory for real live events, it can also be used to measure memory for events viewed through media. Toddlers are frequently exposed to multiple forms of digital media in addition to more traditional forms of picture book reading. In a within-subjects design, memory was assessed with a DI task in 2-year-olds (<i>n</i> = 89) using the Frankfurt Imitation Test. Deferred imitation was assessed after live and video demonstrations. Parents completed a survey about children's media use. Picture book reading for less than 30 min a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated live. Watching video content for more than 1 h a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated on video. Results are interpreted in terms of individual differences in experiences of traditional and digital media and the development of symbolic understanding.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"729-749"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12609","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Co-regulation, relationship quality, and infant distress vocalizations observed during mother-infant interactions: Influences of maternal depression and different contexts 在母婴互动过程中观察到的共同调节、关系质量和婴儿苦恼发声:母亲抑郁和不同环境的影响。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12617
Elizabeth L. Leong, Dale M. Stack, Olivia K. Lazimbat, Samantha Bouchard, Tiffany M. Field

Mother-infant interactions are co-regulated and provide the foundation for mother-infant relationship quality. The implications of maternal depression and contextual demands (i.e., reinstating the interaction following maternal unavailability and vocalized infant distress) on observationally coded co-regulation in mother-infant dyads (n = 40) at 4-months was investigated. Associations among co-regulation patterns and mother-infant relationship quality was also examined. Dyads participated in Still-Face (SF) and Separation (SP) procedures, with periods of maternal emotional and physical unavailability. Co-regulation was captured using the Revised Relational Coding System. Relationship quality was examined using the Emotional Availability Scales. Dyads in the depressed group had significantly more unilateral exchanges than the non-depressed group following the SF and SP perturbations. The depressed group also had significantly more distress vocalizations during the SP perturbation than the non-depressed group. Co-regulation in the depressed group was less disrupted by the SF perturbation. Positive relationship quality dimensions (maternal sensitivity, structuring, and infant responsiveness) were associated with more symmetrical and less unilateral co-regulation regardless of the interaction period. There were also context-specific results pertaining to patterns of co-regulation and associated maternal hostility and infant responsiveness. Results highlight co-regulatory differences in depressed mothers and their infants and how these differences are exacerbated by contextual demands.

母婴互动是共同调节的,是母婴关系质量的基础。本研究调查了母亲抑郁和情境需求(即在母亲不在和婴儿发声求助后恢复互动)对母婴二人组(n = 40)4 个月时观察编码共同调节的影响。此外,还研究了共同调节模式与母婴关系质量之间的关联。母婴组合参加了静止面对(SF)和分离(SP)程序,在这两个程序中,母亲在情感和身体上都处于不在场状态。共同调节采用修订的关系编码系统(Revised Relational Coding System)。使用情感可得性量表对关系质量进行检测。与非抑郁组相比,抑郁组在受到 SF 和 SP 干扰后的单边交流明显增多。抑郁组在SP扰动时的苦恼发声也明显多于非抑郁组。抑郁组的共同调节能力受 SF 扰动的干扰较小。积极的关系质量维度(母性敏感性、结构化和婴儿反应性)与更对称和更少的单侧共同调节相关,而与交互作用期无关。在共同调节的模式以及相关的母亲敌意和婴儿反应性方面,也有针对具体情境的结果。研究结果突显了抑郁母亲及其婴儿的共同调节差异,以及这些差异如何因情境需求而加剧。
{"title":"Co-regulation, relationship quality, and infant distress vocalizations observed during mother-infant interactions: Influences of maternal depression and different contexts","authors":"Elizabeth L. Leong,&nbsp;Dale M. Stack,&nbsp;Olivia K. Lazimbat,&nbsp;Samantha Bouchard,&nbsp;Tiffany M. Field","doi":"10.1111/infa.12617","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12617","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Mother-infant interactions are co-regulated and provide the foundation for mother-infant relationship quality. The implications of maternal depression and contextual demands (i.e., reinstating the interaction following maternal unavailability and vocalized infant distress) on observationally coded co-regulation in mother-infant dyads (<i>n</i> = 40) at 4-months was investigated. Associations among co-regulation patterns and mother-infant relationship quality was also examined. Dyads participated in Still-Face (SF) and Separation (SP) procedures, with periods of maternal emotional and physical unavailability. Co-regulation was captured using the Revised Relational Coding System. Relationship quality was examined using the Emotional Availability Scales. Dyads in the depressed group had significantly more unilateral exchanges than the non-depressed group following the SF and SP perturbations. The depressed group also had significantly more distress vocalizations during the SP perturbation than the non-depressed group. Co-regulation in the depressed group was less disrupted by the SF perturbation. Positive relationship quality dimensions (maternal sensitivity, structuring, and infant responsiveness) were associated with more symmetrical and less unilateral co-regulation regardless of the interaction period. There were also context-specific results pertaining to patterns of co-regulation and associated maternal hostility and infant responsiveness. Results highlight co-regulatory differences in depressed mothers and their infants and how these differences are exacerbated by contextual demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"933-957"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12617","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Of the importance to reconsider individual variability in infant studies: Family traits do impact turn-taking perception in 6-month-olds 在婴儿研究中重新考虑个体差异的重要性:家庭特征确实会影响 6 个月大婴儿的轮流感知。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12612
Bastien Meunier, Stéphanie Barbu, Alice Rabiller, Nicolas Dollion, Alban Lemasson, Virginie Durier

Turn-taking is a universal pattern of human conversations characterized by a fast exchange of turns between speakers and an avoidance of overlaps. Language is embedded in this conversational skill acquired well before it during infancy, through everyday interactions with caregivers. The earliness of this skill and its link with language allows us to test whether social environment shapes early language development. We therefore study turn-taking perception of 6-month-old infants by measuring their gazes during video presentation of three different conversational situations where the turn is explicitly given, normally taken or taken with an overlap. We studied 51 infants to cover several family and infant characteristics: infants' sex, presence of siblings, and family socioeconomic status (SES). We found that infants looked more at the second speaker when she overlapped the first speaker than in the other situations, but not all infants were equally sensitive. Indeed, infants from high-SES families reacted differently to the three situations, while infants from the two lower SES categories did not. Also, only singletons reacted differently by looking more at the second speaker after the overlapping and turn-giving situations, and not after the turn-taking situation. Our results emphasize the importance of early social experiences on language development.

轮流说话是人类对话的一种普遍模式,其特点是说话者之间快速轮流交流,避免重叠。这种会话技能蕴含着语言,早在婴儿期就已通过与照看者的日常互动习得。这种技能的出现及其与语言的联系使我们能够检验社会环境是否会影响早期语言的发展。因此,我们对 6 个月大婴儿的轮流感知进行了研究,方法是测量他们在视频演示三种不同对话情境时的注视情况,即明确给出轮流、正常轮流或重叠轮流。我们对 51 名婴儿进行了研究,涵盖了若干家庭和婴儿特征:婴儿性别、是否有兄弟姐妹以及家庭社会经济地位(SES)。我们发现,与其他情况相比,当第二个说话者与第一个说话者重叠时,婴儿会更多地注视第二个说话者,但并非所有婴儿都同样敏感。事实上,来自高社会经济地位家庭的婴儿对三种情况的反应不同,而来自两种较低社会经济地位家庭的婴儿则没有。此外,只有单亲家庭的婴儿在重叠情境和轮流情境下会做出不同的反应,即更多地注视第二个说话者,而在轮流情境下则不会。我们的研究结果强调了早期社会经验对语言发展的重要性。
{"title":"Of the importance to reconsider individual variability in infant studies: Family traits do impact turn-taking perception in 6-month-olds","authors":"Bastien Meunier,&nbsp;Stéphanie Barbu,&nbsp;Alice Rabiller,&nbsp;Nicolas Dollion,&nbsp;Alban Lemasson,&nbsp;Virginie Durier","doi":"10.1111/infa.12612","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12612","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Turn-taking is a universal pattern of human conversations characterized by a fast exchange of turns between speakers and an avoidance of overlaps. Language is embedded in this conversational skill acquired well before it during infancy, through everyday interactions with caregivers. The earliness of this skill and its link with language allows us to test whether social environment shapes early language development. We therefore study turn-taking perception of 6-month-old infants by measuring their gazes during video presentation of three different conversational situations where the turn is explicitly given, normally taken or taken with an overlap. We studied 51 infants to cover several family and infant characteristics: infants' sex, presence of siblings, and family socioeconomic status (SES). We found that infants looked more at the second speaker when she overlapped the first speaker than in the other situations, but not all infants were equally sensitive. Indeed, infants from high-SES families reacted differently to the three situations, while infants from the two lower SES categories did not. Also, only singletons reacted differently by looking more at the second speaker after the overlapping and turn-giving situations, and not after the turn-taking situation. Our results emphasize the importance of early social experiences on language development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"894-907"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12612","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1