首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
Analyzing the effect of sibling number on input and output in the first 18 months 分析兄弟姐妹数量对头 18 个月投入和产出的影响。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-06 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12578
Catherine Laing, Elika Bergelson

Prior research suggests that across a wide range of cognitive, educational, and health-based measures, first-born children outperform their later-born peers. Expanding on this literature using naturalistic home-recorded data and parental vocabulary reports, we find that early language outcomes vary by number of siblings in a sample of 43 English-learning U.S. children from mid-to-high socioeconomic status homes. More specifically, we find that children in our sample with two or more—but not one—older siblings had smaller productive vocabularies at 18 months, and heard less input from caregivers across several measures than their peers with less than two siblings. We discuss implications regarding what infants experience and learn across a range of family sizes in infancy.

先前的研究表明,在认知、教育和健康等多方面的衡量标准中,头胎儿童的表现都优于晚生儿童。我们利用自然的家庭记录数据和父母的词汇报告对这些文献进行了扩展,发现在 43 个来自中高社会经济地位家庭的学习英语的美国儿童样本中,早期语言成果因兄弟姐妹的数量而异。更具体地说,我们发现在我们的样本中,有两个或两个以上(而不是一个)兄弟姐妹的儿童在 18 个月大时,其生产性词汇量较小,与兄弟姐妹少于两个的同龄儿童相比,他们从照顾者那里听到的输入较少。我们讨论了婴儿期在不同规模的家庭中经历和学习的意义。
{"title":"Analyzing the effect of sibling number on input and output in the first 18 months","authors":"Catherine Laing,&nbsp;Elika Bergelson","doi":"10.1111/infa.12578","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12578","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Prior research suggests that across a wide range of cognitive, educational, and health-based measures, first-born children outperform their later-born peers. Expanding on this literature using naturalistic home-recorded data and parental vocabulary reports, we find that early language outcomes vary by number of siblings in a sample of 43 English-learning U.S. children from mid-to-high socioeconomic status homes. More specifically, we find that children in our sample with two or more—but not one—older siblings had smaller productive vocabularies at 18 months, and heard less input from caregivers across several measures than their peers with less than two siblings. We discuss implications regarding what infants experience and learn across a range of family sizes in infancy.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"175-195"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12578","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139111289","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental trajectories of picture-based object representations during the first year of life 出生后第一年以图片为基础的物体表征的发展轨迹。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-06 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12581
Jeanne L. Shinskey

Experience with an object's photograph changes 9-month-olds’ preference for the referent object, confirming they can represent objects from pictures. However, picture-based representations appear weaker than object-based representations. The current study's first objective was to investigate age differences in object recognition memory after familiarization with objects' pictures. The second objective was to test whether age differences in object permanence sensitivity with picture-based representations match those found with object-based representations, whereby 7-month-olds search more for familiar hidden objects but 11-month-olds search more for novel ones. Six- and 11-month-olds were familiarized with an object's photo and tested on their representation of the real object by comparing their reaching for it versus a novel object. Objects were visible under conditions testing recognition memory and hidden under conditions testing object permanence. Like 9-month-olds, 6- and 11-month-olds preferred novelty with visible objects, showing early object recognition after picture familiarization, as well as developmental continuity. Unlike 9-month-olds, who switched to preferring familiarity with hidden objects, 6- and 11-month-olds switched to null preference. This pattern fails to match 7- and 11-month-olds’ hidden-object preferences after familiarization with real objects, revealing discontinuity in sensitivity to object permanence after picture familiarization, and suggesting that picture-based representations are weaker than object-based ones.

9 个月大的幼儿对物体照片的体验会改变他们对参照物的偏好,这证明他们可以通过图片来表征物体。然而,基于图片的表征似乎弱于基于物体的表征。本研究的第一个目的是调查熟悉物体图片后物体识别记忆的年龄差异。第二个目的是测试基于图片表征的物体永久性敏感性的年龄差异是否与基于物体表征的年龄差异一致,即 7 个月大的幼儿更多搜索熟悉的隐藏物体,而 11 个月大的幼儿更多搜索新奇的物体。我们让 6 个月和 11 个月大的幼儿熟悉一个物体的照片,并通过比较他们伸手寻找真实物体和新物体的情况,测试他们对真实物体的表征。在测试识别记忆的条件下,物体是可见的,而在测试物体永久性的条件下,物体是隐藏的。与 9 个月大的幼儿一样,6 个月和 11 个月大的幼儿更喜欢可见物体的新奇感,这表明他们在熟悉图片后能较早地识别物体,而且其发展具有连续性。与 9 个月幼儿转而喜欢熟悉的隐藏物体不同,6 个月和 11 个月幼儿转而不喜欢。这种模式与 7 个月和 11 个月幼儿在熟悉真实物体后对隐藏物体的偏好不一致,揭示了幼儿在熟悉图片后对物体永久性敏感性的不连续性,并表明以图片为基础的表征比以物体为基础的表征弱。
{"title":"Developmental trajectories of picture-based object representations during the first year of life","authors":"Jeanne L. Shinskey","doi":"10.1111/infa.12581","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12581","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Experience with an object's photograph changes 9-month-olds’ preference for the referent object, confirming they can represent objects from pictures. However, picture-based representations appear weaker than object-based representations. The current study's first objective was to investigate age differences in object recognition memory after familiarization with objects' pictures. The second objective was to test whether age differences in object permanence sensitivity with picture-based representations match those found with object-based representations, whereby 7-month-olds search more for familiar hidden objects but 11-month-olds search more for novel ones. Six- and 11-month-olds were familiarized with an object's photo and tested on their representation of the real object by comparing their reaching for it versus a novel object. Objects were visible under conditions testing recognition memory and hidden under conditions testing object permanence. Like 9-month-olds, 6- and 11-month-olds preferred novelty with visible objects, showing early object recognition after picture familiarization, as well as developmental continuity. Unlike 9-month-olds, who switched to preferring familiarity with hidden objects, 6- and 11-month-olds switched to null preference. This pattern fails to match 7- and 11-month-olds’ hidden-object preferences after familiarization with real objects, revealing discontinuity in sensitivity to object permanence after picture familiarization, and suggesting that picture-based representations are weaker than object-based ones.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"233-250"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12581","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139111290","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of local meaning in infants' fixations of natural scenes 局部意义在婴儿固定自然场景中的作用。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-06 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12582
Lisa M. Oakes, Taylor R. Hayes, Shannon M. Klotz, Katherine I. Pomaranski, John M. Henderson

As infants view visual scenes every day, they must shift their eye gaze and visual attention from location to location, sampling information to process and learn. Like adults, infants' gaze when viewing natural scenes (i.e., photographs of everyday scenes) is influenced by the physical features of the scene image and a general bias to look more centrally in a scene. However, it is unknown how infants' gaze while viewing such scenes is influenced by the semantic content of the scenes. Here, we tested the relative influence of local meaning, controlling for physical salience and center bias, on the eye gaze of 4- to 12-month-old infants (N = 92) as they viewed natural scenes. Overall, infants were more likely to fixate scene regions rated as higher in meaning, indicating that, like adults, the semantic content, or local meaning, of scenes influences where they look. More importantly, the effect of meaning on infant attention increased with age, providing the first evidence for an age-related increase in the impact of local meaning on infants' eye movements while viewing natural scenes.

婴儿每天都要观看视觉场景,他们必须将目光和视觉注意力从一个位置转移到另一个位置,抽取信息进行处理和学习。与成人一样,婴儿观看自然场景(即日常场景的照片)时的注视也会受到场景图像物理特征的影响,而且一般会偏向于看场景的中心位置。然而,婴儿观看此类场景时的注视如何受到场景语义内容的影响,目前尚不得而知。在此,我们测试了局部意义对 4 至 12 个月大婴儿(92 人)观看自然场景时眼睛注视的相对影响,同时控制了物理显著性和中心偏向。总体而言,婴儿更倾向于定格在意义较高的场景区域,这表明与成人一样,场景的语义内容或局部意义会影响他们的视线。更重要的是,意义对婴儿注意力的影响会随着年龄的增长而增加,这首次证明了在观看自然场景时,局部意义对婴儿眼球运动的影响会随着年龄的增长而增加。
{"title":"The role of local meaning in infants' fixations of natural scenes","authors":"Lisa M. Oakes,&nbsp;Taylor R. Hayes,&nbsp;Shannon M. Klotz,&nbsp;Katherine I. Pomaranski,&nbsp;John M. Henderson","doi":"10.1111/infa.12582","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12582","url":null,"abstract":"<p>As infants view visual scenes every day, they must shift their eye gaze and visual attention from location to location, sampling information to process and learn. Like adults, infants' gaze when viewing natural scenes (i.e., photographs of everyday scenes) is influenced by the physical features of the scene image and a general bias to look more centrally in a scene. However, it is unknown how infants' gaze while viewing such scenes is influenced by the semantic content of the scenes. Here, we tested the relative influence of <i>local meaning</i>, controlling for physical salience and center bias, on the eye gaze of 4- to 12-month-old infants (<i>N</i> = 92) as they viewed natural scenes. Overall, infants were more likely to fixate scene regions rated as higher in meaning, indicating that, like adults, the semantic content, or local meaning, of scenes influences where they look. More importantly, the effect of meaning on infant attention increased with age, providing the first evidence for an age-related increase in the impact of local meaning on infants' eye movements while viewing natural scenes.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"284-298"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139111291","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Touch and look: The role of affective touch in promoting infants' attention towards complex visual scenes 触摸与观察:情感触觉在促进婴儿关注复杂视觉场景中的作用。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-05 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12580
Laura Carnevali, Letizia Della Longa, Danica Dragovic, Teresa Farroni

In a complex social environment, stimuli from different sensory modalities need to be integrated to decode communicative meanings. From very early in life, infants have to combine a multitude of sensory features with social and affective attributes. Of all senses, touch constitutes a privileged channel to carry affective-motivational meanings and foster social connection. In the present study, we investigate whether sharing sensory stimulation that varies for its affective value differentially affects infants' attention towards visual stimuli. 6 to 11-month-old infants (N = 42) were familiarized with two characters respectively matched with tactile (affective or non-affective) and auditory stimulation; then repeatedly exposed to scenes where the two characters moved towards target objects. Our results showed a main effect of stimulation (sound vs. touch) on looking times during familiarization, with longer looking times when sound is provided. During scenes presentation, a main effect of the type of touch (affective vs. non affective) emerged, with longer looking times in infants that previously experienced affective touch, suggesting that this sensory experience may critically engage the self and modulate infant attention. Overall, these findings suggest that while sound acts as attention getter, affective touch supports sustained attention towards complex visual scenes beyond the stimulation period itself.

在复杂的社会环境中,来自不同感官模式的刺激需要加以整合,以解码交流意义。从生命的早期开始,婴儿就必须将多种感官特征与社会和情感属性结合起来。在所有感官中,触觉是传递情感-动机意义和促进社会联系的重要渠道。在本研究中,我们探讨了分享不同情感价值的感官刺激是否会对婴儿对视觉刺激的注意力产生不同影响。我们先让 6 到 11 个月大的婴儿(42 人)熟悉分别与触觉(情感或非情感)和听觉刺激相匹配的两个角色,然后让他们反复接触这两个角色向目标物体移动的场景。我们的研究结果表明,在熟悉过程中,刺激(声音与触摸)对观察时间有主效应,当提供声音时,观察时间更长。在场景呈现过程中,触觉类型(情感触觉与非情感触觉)出现了主效应,之前经历过情感触觉的婴儿看的时间更长,这表明这种感官体验可能会关键性地吸引自我并调节婴儿的注意力。总之,这些研究结果表明,声音能引起婴儿的注意,而情感触觉则能在刺激期结束后支持婴儿对复杂视觉场景的持续注意。
{"title":"Touch and look: The role of affective touch in promoting infants' attention towards complex visual scenes","authors":"Laura Carnevali,&nbsp;Letizia Della Longa,&nbsp;Danica Dragovic,&nbsp;Teresa Farroni","doi":"10.1111/infa.12580","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12580","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In a complex social environment, stimuli from different sensory modalities need to be integrated to decode communicative meanings. From very early in life, infants have to combine a multitude of sensory features with social and affective attributes. Of all senses, touch constitutes a privileged channel to carry affective-motivational meanings and foster social connection. In the present study, we investigate whether sharing sensory stimulation that varies for its affective value differentially affects infants' attention towards visual stimuli. 6 to 11-month-old infants (<i>N</i> = 42) were familiarized with two characters respectively matched with tactile (affective or non-affective) and auditory stimulation; then repeatedly exposed to scenes where the two characters moved towards target objects. Our results showed a main effect of stimulation (sound vs. touch) on looking times during familiarization, with longer looking times when sound is provided. During scenes presentation, a main effect of the type of touch (affective vs. non affective) emerged, with longer looking times in infants that previously experienced affective touch, suggesting that this sensory experience may critically engage the self and modulate infant attention. Overall, these findings suggest that while sound acts as attention getter, affective touch supports sustained attention towards complex visual scenes beyond the stimulation period itself.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"271-283"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12580","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139098967","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Less attention to emotional faces is associated with low empathy and prosociality in 12-to 20-month old infants 12至20个月大的婴儿对情绪化面孔的关注较少与移情能力和亲社会性较低有关。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-03 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12569
Meghan Rose Donohue, M. Catalina Camacho, Jordan E. Drake, Rebecca F. Schwarzlose, Rebecca G. Brady, Caroline P. Hoyniak, Laura Hennefield, Lauren S. Wakschlag, Cynthia E. Rogers, Deanna M. Barch, Joan Luby

The development of empathy and prosocial behavior begins in infancy and is likely supported by emotion processing skills. The current study explored whether early emerging deficits in emotion processing are associated with disruptions in the development of empathy and prosociality. We investigated this question in a large, diverse sample of 147, 11- to 20-month-old infants (42% female; 61% Black; 67% low socioeconomic status). Infants completed two observational tasks assessing prosocial helping and one task assessing empathy and prosocial comforting behavior. Infants also completed an eye-tracking task assessing engagement and disengagement with negative emotional faces. Infants who attended less to angry, sad, and fearful faces (i.e., by being slower to look at and/or quicker to look away from negative compared to neutral faces) engaged in fewer helping behaviors, and effect sizes were larger when examining infants' attention toward the eye regions of faces. Additionally, infants who were quicker to look away from the eye regions of angry faces, but not the whole face, displayed less empathy and comforting behaviors. Results suggest that as early as 12 months of age, infants' decreased attention toward negative emotional faces, particularly the eye regions, is associated with less empathy and prosociality during a developmental period in which these abilities are rapidly maturing.

移情和亲社会行为的发展始于婴儿期,并可能得到情绪处理技能的支持。本研究探讨了早期出现的情绪处理缺陷是否与移情和亲社会行为的发展中断有关。我们对 147 名 11 到 20 个月大的婴儿(42% 为女性;61% 为黑人;67% 为社会经济地位较低)进行了大规模、多样化的抽样调查。婴儿完成了两项评估亲社会帮助的观察任务和一项评估移情和亲社会安慰行为的任务。婴儿还完成了一项眼动跟踪任务,评估对负面情绪面孔的参与和不参与情况。较少关注愤怒、悲伤和恐惧面孔的婴儿(即与中性面孔相比,较少关注负面情绪面孔和/或较少关注负面情绪面孔和/或较快远离负面情绪面孔)参与的帮助行为较少,而且当研究婴儿对面孔眼睛区域的关注时,其效应大小更大。此外,较快从愤怒面孔的眼睛区域(而非整个面孔)移开视线的婴儿,其移情和安慰行为较少。研究结果表明,早在婴儿12个月大时,他们对负面情绪面孔(尤其是眼睛区域)的注意力就会下降,而在这些能力迅速成熟的发育阶段,他们的移情能力和亲社会性也会下降。
{"title":"Less attention to emotional faces is associated with low empathy and prosociality in 12-to 20-month old infants","authors":"Meghan Rose Donohue,&nbsp;M. Catalina Camacho,&nbsp;Jordan E. Drake,&nbsp;Rebecca F. Schwarzlose,&nbsp;Rebecca G. Brady,&nbsp;Caroline P. Hoyniak,&nbsp;Laura Hennefield,&nbsp;Lauren S. Wakschlag,&nbsp;Cynthia E. Rogers,&nbsp;Deanna M. Barch,&nbsp;Joan Luby","doi":"10.1111/infa.12569","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12569","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The development of empathy and prosocial behavior begins in infancy and is likely supported by emotion processing skills. The current study explored whether early emerging deficits in emotion processing are associated with disruptions in the development of empathy and prosociality. We investigated this question in a large, diverse sample of 147, 11- to 20-month-old infants (42% female; 61% Black; 67% low socioeconomic status). Infants completed two observational tasks assessing prosocial helping and one task assessing empathy and prosocial comforting behavior. Infants also completed an eye-tracking task assessing engagement and disengagement with negative emotional faces. Infants who attended less to angry, sad, and fearful faces (i.e., by being slower to look at and/or quicker to look away from negative compared to neutral faces) engaged in fewer helping behaviors, and effect sizes were larger when examining infants' attention toward the eye regions of faces. Additionally, infants who were quicker to look away from the eye regions of angry faces, but not the whole face, displayed less empathy and comforting behaviors. Results suggest that as early as 12 months of age, infants' decreased attention toward negative emotional faces, particularly the eye regions, is associated with less empathy and prosociality during a developmental period in which these abilities are rapidly maturing.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"113-136"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139088986","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Patterns of attention-sensitive communication contribute to 7–20-month-olds' emerging pragmatic skills 注意力敏感的交流模式有助于 7-20 个月大的幼儿逐渐掌握实用技能
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-01 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12577
Mawa Dafreville, Michèle Guidetti, Marie Bourjade

The present study aimed at investigating the ability of 7- to 20-month-old infants to display attention-sensitive communication using either canonical markers of language acquisition (e.g., pointing gestures, canonical babblings) or other signals based on the physical features actually perceived by the mother in everyday interaction (e.g., body movements, mouth sounds). We studied 30 French mother-infant dyads in naturalistic settings. We assessed the infants' attention-sensitive communication through unimodal and cross-modal adjustment, defined as the capacity of infants to address visually inattentive mothers by avoiding visual communication mismatches and/or favoring communication matches through audible-or-contact signals. Unimodal and cross-modal adjustments were tested for specific signals across spontaneous “conditions” of maternal visual attention (attentive/inattentive) from video footage filmed in the home. Both canonical markers of language development and signals belonging to an extended repertoire of communication were used by infants to adjust to their mother's visual attention. Gaze-coordinated signals were overall not significantly better adjusted to maternal attention than non-gaze-coordinated signals, except for specific silent-visual signals at certain ages. Overall, these results indicate that attention-sensitive communication is relevant to the development of early pragmatic skills and that the intentional use of signals may be more reliably approximated by this capacity than by gaze-coordination with signals.

本研究旨在调查 7 至 20 个月大的婴儿使用语言习得的标准标记(如指点手势、标准咿呀声)或其他基于母亲在日常互动中实际感知到的身体特征(如身体动作、口腔声音)来进行注意力敏感交流的能力。我们在自然环境中对 30 个法国母婴二人组进行了研究。我们通过单模态和跨模态调整来评估婴儿的注意力敏感性交流能力。单模态和跨模态调整的定义是婴儿通过避免视觉交流不匹配和/或通过声音或接触信号进行交流匹配来应对视觉注意力不集中的母亲的能力。根据在家中拍摄的视频片段,在母亲视觉注意力(专注/不专注)的自发 "条件 "下,对特定信号的单模态和跨模态调整进行了测试。婴儿使用语言发展的典型标记和属于扩展交流曲目的信号来适应母亲的视觉注意力。总体而言,与非凝视协调信号相比,凝视协调信号对母亲注意力的调节效果并不明显,但某些年龄段的特定无声视觉信号除外。总之,这些结果表明,对注意力敏感的交流与早期实用技能的发展有关,与凝视协调信号相比,这种能力更能可靠地近似信号的有意使用。
{"title":"Patterns of attention-sensitive communication contribute to 7–20-month-olds' emerging pragmatic skills","authors":"Mawa Dafreville,&nbsp;Michèle Guidetti,&nbsp;Marie Bourjade","doi":"10.1111/infa.12577","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12577","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The present study aimed at investigating the ability of 7- to 20-month-old infants to display <i>attention-sensitive communication</i> using either canonical markers of language acquisition (e.g., pointing gestures, canonical babblings) or other signals based on the physical features <i>actually perceived</i> by the mother in everyday interaction (e.g., body movements, mouth sounds). We studied 30 French mother-infant dyads in naturalistic settings. We assessed the infants' <i>attention-sensitive communication</i> through <i>unimodal</i> and <i>cross-modal</i> adjustment, defined as the capacity of infants to address visually inattentive mothers by avoiding visual communication mismatches and/or favoring communication matches through audible-or-contact signals. <i>Unimodal</i> and <i>cross-modal</i> adjustments were tested for specific signals across spontaneous “conditions” of maternal visual attention (attentive/inattentive) from video footage filmed in the home. Both <i>canonical markers</i> of language development and signals belonging to an <i>extended repertoire</i> of communication were used by infants to adjust to their mother's visual attention. Gaze-coordinated signals were overall not significantly better adjusted to maternal attention than non-gaze-coordinated signals, except for specific silent-visual signals at certain ages. Overall, these results indicate that attention-sensitive communication is relevant to the development of early pragmatic skills and that the intentional use of signals may be more reliably approximated by this capacity than by gaze-coordination with signals.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"216-232"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139068828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between parents' prosocial behavior and media use and young children's prosocial development: The mediating role of children's media use 父母的亲社会行为和媒体使用与幼儿的亲社会发展之间的纵向联系:儿童使用媒体的中介作用
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-30 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12576
Laura M. Padilla-Walker, Katey Workman, Anna Calley, Sarah Ashby, Hailey G. Holmgren, Corinne Archibald, Ashley M. Fraser, Sarah M. Coyne

Research has found that media is associated with children's prosocial behavior (PB) from an early age, and that parents play a key role in children's media use and behavior. However, few studies explore these relations as early as infancy while also controlling for well-established predictors of PB (e.g., empathic concern). Thus, the present study examined longitudinal associations between parents' PB and media use, and prosocial development during early childhood, mediated by children's own media use. Participants were 519 children (M age at Time 1 = 17.77 months) and parents who participated in three timepoints of an ongoing, longitudinal study. A longitudinal path model suggested that children's media use was still significantly associated with PB 1 year later after accounting for factors such as parents' PB, media use, and empathy. These findings have important implications for the early development of behaviors that serve as a foundation for social and moral development.

研究发现,媒体从小就与儿童的亲社会行为(PB)有关,而父母在儿童的媒体使用和行为中起着关键作用。然而,很少有研究从婴儿期就开始探讨这些关系,同时还控制了已被证实的亲社会行为预测因素(如移情关注)。因此,本研究考察了父母的亲子关系和媒体使用与幼儿期亲社会发展之间的纵向联系,并以儿童自身的媒体使用为中介。本研究的参与者为 519 名儿童(时间 1 时的平均年龄为 17.77 个月)和父母,他们参加了一项正在进行的纵向研究的三个时间点。纵向路径模型表明,在考虑了父母的 PB、媒体使用和移情等因素后,1 年后儿童的媒体使用仍与 PB 显著相关。这些发现对作为社会和道德发展基础的早期行为发展具有重要意义。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between parents' prosocial behavior and media use and young children's prosocial development: The mediating role of children's media use","authors":"Laura M. Padilla-Walker,&nbsp;Katey Workman,&nbsp;Anna Calley,&nbsp;Sarah Ashby,&nbsp;Hailey G. Holmgren,&nbsp;Corinne Archibald,&nbsp;Ashley M. Fraser,&nbsp;Sarah M. Coyne","doi":"10.1111/infa.12576","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12576","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research has found that media is associated with children's prosocial behavior (PB) from an early age, and that parents play a key role in children's media use and behavior. However, few studies explore these relations as early as infancy while also controlling for well-established predictors of PB (e.g., empathic concern). Thus, the present study examined longitudinal associations between parents' PB and media use, and prosocial development during early childhood, mediated by children's own media use. Participants were 519 children (M age at Time 1 = 17.77 months) and parents who participated in three timepoints of an ongoing, longitudinal study. A longitudinal path model suggested that children's media use was still significantly associated with PB 1 year later after accounting for factors such as parents' PB, media use, and empathy. These findings have important implications for the early development of behaviors that serve as a foundation for social and moral development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"95-112"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139068616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infant screen media and child development: A prospective community study 婴儿屏幕媒体与儿童发展:前瞻性社区研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-29 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12575
Ortal Slobodin, Orit E. Hetzroni, Moran Mandel, Sappir Saad Nuttman, Zainab Gawi Damashi, Eden Machluf, Michael Davidovitch

The current study examined longitudinal associations between early screen media exposure (assessed at 6, 12, and 24 months) and the child's motor and language/communication development at the ages of 24 and 36 months. We also aimed to study whether these associations varied by socioeconomic status (SES). Participants were 179 parent-infant dyads, recruited from well-baby clinic services during routine visits. Child development measures included standardized measures of developmental milestones as assessed by professionals and referral data to child developmental centers. Both measures were retrieved from the official health maintenance organization records by an expert in child development. Results indicated that screen exposure at 6 and 12 months was associated with a higher risk for language/communication deficits at 36 months in children with moderate or high SES but not in children with low SES. Our findings are consistent with existing literature demonstrating cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between early screen exposure and language development deficits. Given that media use practices and motivations vary among families from different backgrounds, further investigation of the interaction between SES and screen exposure is needed.

本研究考察了儿童早期接触屏幕媒体(在 6、12 和 24 个月时进行评估)与儿童在 24 和 36 个月时的运动和语言/沟通能力发展之间的纵向联系。我们还旨在研究这些关联是否因社会经济地位(SES)而异。研究对象是 179 个父母-婴儿二人组,他们都是在常规就诊期间从健康婴儿诊所服务机构招募的。儿童发育指标包括由专业人士评估的发育里程碑标准化指标和儿童发育中心的转介数据。这两项指标均由儿童发育专家从官方健康维护组织的记录中提取。结果表明,在中等或高社会经济地位的儿童中,6 个月和 12 个月时接触屏幕与 36 个月时出现语言/沟通障碍的较高风险有关,但与低社会经济地位的儿童无关。我们的研究结果与现有的文献一致,这些文献显示了早期屏幕接触与语言发展缺陷之间的横截面和纵向联系。鉴于不同背景的家庭使用媒体的习惯和动机各不相同,因此需要进一步研究社会经济地位与屏幕接触之间的相互作用。
{"title":"Infant screen media and child development: A prospective community study","authors":"Ortal Slobodin,&nbsp;Orit E. Hetzroni,&nbsp;Moran Mandel,&nbsp;Sappir Saad Nuttman,&nbsp;Zainab Gawi Damashi,&nbsp;Eden Machluf,&nbsp;Michael Davidovitch","doi":"10.1111/infa.12575","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12575","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The current study examined longitudinal associations between early screen media exposure (assessed at 6, 12, and 24 months) and the child's motor and language/communication development at the ages of 24 and 36 months. We also aimed to study whether these associations varied by socioeconomic status (SES). Participants were 179 parent-infant dyads, recruited from well-baby clinic services during routine visits. Child development measures included standardized measures of developmental milestones as assessed by professionals and referral data to child developmental centers. Both measures were retrieved from the official health maintenance organization records by an expert in child development. Results indicated that screen exposure at 6 and 12 months was associated with a higher risk for language/communication deficits at 36 months in children with moderate or high SES but not in children with low SES. Our findings are consistent with existing literature demonstrating cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between early screen exposure and language development deficits. Given that media use practices and motivations vary among families from different backgrounds, further investigation of the interaction between SES and screen exposure is needed.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"155-174"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12575","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139068198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Attachment security and problematic media use in infancy: A longitudinal study in the United States 婴儿期的依恋安全和问题媒体使用:美国的一项纵向研究
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12570
Jane Shawcroft, Sarah M. Coyne, Lisa Linder, Brandon N. Clifford, Brandon T. McDaniel

Media use during childhood has quickly become a norm across the United States and in other countries. One area still not well understood is the development of problematic (or maladaptive and disruptive) media use in children. This research examines the role of attachment security as a central component in the development of problematic media use over time in a sample of 248 parent-child dyads (9.50% African American, 20.66% Hispanic, 62.81% White, 2.07% Asian, 4.96% other ethnicities). We examined the relationship between attachment security and problematic media use one and 2 years later. We then constructed a mediation model examining parent responsiveness while jointly engaging in media use and during play as mediators between infant attachment security and problematic media use over time. Results suggest that while infant attachment security may be protective against developing problematic media use patterns, this relationship does not seem to be mediated by parent-child interactions while engaging in media or during play.

在美国和其他国家,儿童时期使用媒体已迅速成为一种常态。但人们对儿童问题性(或适应不良和破坏性)媒体使用的发展仍不甚了解。本研究以 248 个亲子家庭(9.50% 非裔美国人、20.66% 西班牙裔美国人、62.81% 白人、2.07% 亚洲人、4.96% 其他种族)为样本,探讨了依恋安全感作为问题性媒体使用发展过程中的核心要素所起的作用。我们研究了依恋安全与一年和两年后问题媒体使用之间的关系。然后,我们构建了一个中介模型,研究了父母在共同使用媒体和玩耍时的反应能力,以此作为婴儿依恋安全感与随着时间推移的问题媒体使用之间的中介。结果表明,虽然婴儿的依恋安全感可能对形成问题媒体使用模式具有保护作用,但这种关系似乎并不以亲子在使用媒体或游戏时的互动为中介。
{"title":"Attachment security and problematic media use in infancy: A longitudinal study in the United States","authors":"Jane Shawcroft,&nbsp;Sarah M. Coyne,&nbsp;Lisa Linder,&nbsp;Brandon N. Clifford,&nbsp;Brandon T. McDaniel","doi":"10.1111/infa.12570","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12570","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Media use during childhood has quickly become a norm across the United States and in other countries. One area still not well understood is the development of problematic (or maladaptive and disruptive) media use in children. This research examines the role of attachment security as a central component in the development of problematic media use over time in a sample of 248 parent-child dyads (9.50% African American, 20.66% Hispanic, 62.81% White, 2.07% Asian, 4.96% other ethnicities). We examined the relationship between attachment security and problematic media use one and 2 years later. We then constructed a mediation model examining parent responsiveness while jointly engaging in media use and during play as mediators between infant attachment security and problematic media use over time. Results suggest that while infant attachment security may be protective against developing problematic media use patterns, this relationship does not seem to be mediated by parent-child interactions while engaging in media or during play.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"137-154"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12570","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138716572","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Handedness as a major determinant of lateral bias in human functional cradling 手型是人类功能性摇篮侧向偏差的主要决定因素
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-15 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12572
Audrey L. H. van der Meer

Studies examining infant cradling have almost uniformly concluded with a general human left-side bias for cradling, indicating that people prefer to hold an infant to the left of their body. Explanations for the notion of the left-side cradling bias have traditionally been searched for in a variety of factors, for example, in terms of maternal heartbeat, genetic factors, in the form of an ear asymmetry where auditory information is perceived faster through the left ear, as a result of a right hemispheric functional specialization for perception of emotions and faces, and in identifying a motor bias of the infant, such as the tendency of newborn infants to lie with the face to the right when placed supine. Interestingly, handedness is generally considered an inadequate explanation for the lateralized cradling bias, despite it being an intuitively plausible one. In this brief review, I put forward the cradler's handedness as the most convincing and elegant determinant of the cradling bias. This explanation is consistent with a developmental cascades' framework where the cradling bias can be understood as the result of a multitude of factors across a range of levels and systems.

对婴儿摇篮的研究几乎一致得出结论,人类普遍存在左侧摇篮的偏好,这表明人们更喜欢将婴儿抱在自己身体的左侧。传统上,人们从多种因素中寻找左侧抱养偏好的解释,例如,母亲的心跳、遗传因素、耳朵不对称(即左耳更快地感知到听觉信息)、右半球对情绪和面孔感知的功能特化,以及确定婴儿的运动偏好,如新生儿仰卧时面部倾向于右侧。有趣的是,人们普遍认为 "手性 "不足以解释侧向摇篮抱的偏向,尽管它在直觉上是合理的。在这篇简短的综述中,我提出了摇篮者的手性是摇篮偏向最有说服力和最优雅的决定因素。这一解释与发展级联框架相一致,即摇篮偏向可被理解为一系列层次和系统中多种因素共同作用的结果。
{"title":"Handedness as a major determinant of lateral bias in human functional cradling","authors":"Audrey L. H. van der Meer","doi":"10.1111/infa.12572","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12572","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Studies examining infant cradling have almost uniformly concluded with a general human left-side bias for cradling, indicating that people prefer to hold an infant to the left of their body. Explanations for the notion of the left-side cradling bias have traditionally been searched for in a variety of factors, for example, in terms of maternal heartbeat, genetic factors, in the form of an ear asymmetry where auditory information is perceived faster through the left ear, as a result of a right hemispheric functional specialization for perception of emotions and faces, and in identifying a motor bias of the infant, such as the tendency of newborn infants to lie with the face to the right when placed supine. Interestingly, handedness is generally considered an inadequate explanation for the lateralized cradling bias, despite it being an intuitively plausible one. In this brief review, I put forward the cradler's handedness as the most convincing and elegant determinant of the cradling bias. This explanation is consistent with a developmental cascades' framework where the cradling bias can be understood as the result of a multitude of factors across a range of levels and systems.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 2","pages":"84-94"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-12-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12572","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138716397","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1