首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
“My baby is ready to learn”—The role of infant pointing in redirecting maternal responses to be more informative "我的宝宝准备好学习了"--婴儿的指点在重新引导母亲做出更多信息反应中的作用。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12616
Edna Orr, Gabriela Kashy Rosenbaum

Caregivers may perceive pointing as an indication of infants' readiness to learn, thereby increasing their tendency to label objects regardless of the infant's gesture type and context. This was investigated in this study by tracking 35 infants at home at the ages of 11 and 13 months and observing their interactions with their mothers during object manipulation. We focused on four types of communicative gestures: typical giving gestures, gestures contingent on exploration, gestures contingent on play, and pointing. We analyzed maternal response tendencies, including affirmation, naming, discourse, and pretense. The results revealed that when infants reached the age of 13 months, they tripled their pointing production; in turn, the maternal response changed entirely, with naming becoming the preferred response to all types of gestures. Furthermore, when infants were 13 months old and offered an object contingent on play acts, mothers increased their pretense acts sevenfold. Based on the most informative responses to infants among those examined, we argue that an increase in the number of pointing gestures may gradually be associated with the establishment of the maternal perception that an infant is ready to learn and a subsequent increase in naming and pretense production by the mother.

照看者可能会认为婴儿用手指表示他们准备好学习,从而增加他们给物体贴标签的倾向,而不管婴儿的手势类型和背景如何。本研究通过跟踪 35 名 11 个月和 13 个月大的婴儿在家的情况,观察他们在操作物体时与母亲的互动,对此进行了研究。我们重点研究了四种类型的交流手势:典型的给予手势、探索手势、游戏手势和指向手势。我们分析了母亲的反应倾向,包括肯定、命名、话语和伪装。结果表明,当婴儿长到 13 个月大时,他们的指点动作增加了两倍;反过来,母性的反应也完全改变了,在所有类型的手势中,命名成为首选反应。此外,当婴儿长到 13 个月大并以游戏行为为条件提供物品时,母亲的假装行为增加了七倍。根据所研究的婴儿中信息量最大的反应,我们认为,指点手势数量的增加可能与母亲认为婴儿已准备好学习以及随后母亲命名和矫饰行为的增加有关。
{"title":"“My baby is ready to learn”—The role of infant pointing in redirecting maternal responses to be more informative","authors":"Edna Orr,&nbsp;Gabriela Kashy Rosenbaum","doi":"10.1111/infa.12616","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12616","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Caregivers may perceive pointing as an indication of infants' readiness to learn, thereby increasing their tendency to label objects regardless of the infant's gesture type and context. This was investigated in this study by tracking 35 infants at home at the ages of 11 and 13 months and observing their interactions with their mothers during object manipulation. We focused on four types of communicative gestures: typical giving gestures, gestures contingent on exploration, gestures contingent on play, and pointing. We analyzed maternal response tendencies, including affirmation, naming, discourse, and pretense. The results revealed that when infants reached the age of 13 months, they tripled their pointing production; in turn, the maternal response changed entirely, with naming becoming the preferred response to all types of gestures. Furthermore, when infants were 13 months old and offered an object contingent on play acts, mothers increased their pretense acts sevenfold. Based on the most informative responses to infants among those examined, we argue that an increase in the number of pointing gestures may gradually be associated with the establishment of the maternal perception that an infant is ready to learn and a subsequent increase in naming and pretense production by the mother.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"908-932"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141731512","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Not too shy to point! Exploring the relationship between shyness and pointing in the second year 不害羞地指点!探索二年级学生害羞与指点之间的关系。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-19 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12610
Cristina Colonnesi, Eliala A. Salvadori, Frans J. Oort, Daniel S. Messinger

Infants' use of pointing gestures to direct and share attention develops during the first 2 years of life. Shyness, defined as an approach-avoidance motivational conflict during social interactions, may influence infants’ use of pointing. Recent research distinguished between positive (gaze and/or head aversions while smiling) and non-positive (gaze and/or head aversions without smiling) shyness, which are related to different social and cognitive skills. We investigated whether positive and non-positive shyness in 12-month-old (n = 38; 15 girls) and 15-month-old (n = 45; 15 girls) infants were associated with their production of pointing gestures. Infants' expressions of shyness were observed during a social-exposure task in which the infant entered the laboratory room in their parent's arms and was welcomed by an unfamiliar person who provided attention and compliments. Infants' pointing was measured with a pointing task involving three stimuli: pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral. Positive shyness was positively associated with overall pointing at 15 months, especially in combination with high levels of non-positive shyness. In addition, infants who displayed more non-positive shyness pointed more frequently to direct the attention of the social partner to an unpleasant (vs. neutral) stimulus at both ages. Results indicate that shyness influences the early use of pointing to emotionally charged stimuli.

婴儿使用指向手势来引导和分享注意力是在出生后的头 2 年中逐渐形成的。害羞被定义为社会交往中的接近-回避动机冲突,它可能会影响婴儿使用手指。最近的研究区分了积极害羞(微笑时目光和/或头部回避)和非积极害羞(不微笑时目光和/或头部回避),它们与不同的社交和认知技能有关。我们研究了 12 个月大(38 个;15 个女孩)和 15 个月大(45 个;15 个女孩)婴儿的积极害羞和非积极害羞是否与他们做出指点手势有关。在一项社会接触任务中,婴儿在父母的怀抱中进入实验室房间,并受到一名陌生人员的欢迎,该人员会给予关注和赞美,在此过程中,婴儿的害羞表现会被观察到。婴儿的指点能力是通过指点任务测量的,该任务包括三种刺激:令人愉快的、令人不快的和中性的。15 个月大时,积极害羞与婴儿的整体指点能力呈正相关,尤其是在非积极害羞水平较高的情况下。此外,在两个月龄时,表现出更多非积极害羞情绪的婴儿会更频繁地指向不愉快(相对于中性)刺激,以引导社交伙伴的注意力。研究结果表明,害羞会影响婴儿早期对情绪性刺激物使用手指。
{"title":"Not too shy to point! Exploring the relationship between shyness and pointing in the second year","authors":"Cristina Colonnesi,&nbsp;Eliala A. Salvadori,&nbsp;Frans J. Oort,&nbsp;Daniel S. Messinger","doi":"10.1111/infa.12610","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12610","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Infants' use of pointing gestures to direct and share attention develops during the first 2 years of life. Shyness, defined as an approach-avoidance motivational conflict during social interactions, may influence infants’ use of pointing. Recent research distinguished between positive (gaze and/or head aversions while smiling) and non-positive (gaze and/or head aversions without smiling) shyness, which are related to different social and cognitive skills. We investigated whether positive and non-positive shyness in 12-month-old (<i>n</i> = 38; 15 girls) and 15-month-old (<i>n</i> = 45; 15 girls) infants were associated with their production of pointing gestures. Infants' expressions of shyness were observed during a social-exposure task in which the infant entered the laboratory room in their parent's arms and was welcomed by an unfamiliar person who provided attention and compliments. Infants' pointing was measured with a pointing task involving three stimuli: pleasant, unpleasant, and neutral. Positive shyness was positively associated with overall pointing at 15 months, especially in combination with high levels of non-positive shyness. In addition, infants who displayed more non-positive shyness pointed more frequently to direct the attention of the social partner to an unpleasant (vs. neutral) stimulus at both ages. Results indicate that shyness influences the early use of pointing to emotionally charged stimuli.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"693-712"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12610","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141731513","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddler's memory and media—Picture book reading and watching video content are associated with memory at 2 years of age 幼儿的记忆力与媒体--阅读图画书和观看视频内容与 2 岁幼儿的记忆力有关。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12609
Felix-Sebastian Koch, Annette Sundqvist, Ulrika Birberg Thornberg, Rachel Barr, Mikael Heimann

Memory develops across the course of the first years of life and is influenced by daily experiences, such as exposure to media like books and television. Memory as tapped by Deferred imitation (DI) requires that toddlers form a representation of the target actions that they can later use to reproduce the actions and in addition to measuring memory for real live events, it can also be used to measure memory for events viewed through media. Toddlers are frequently exposed to multiple forms of digital media in addition to more traditional forms of picture book reading. In a within-subjects design, memory was assessed with a DI task in 2-year-olds (n = 89) using the Frankfurt Imitation Test. Deferred imitation was assessed after live and video demonstrations. Parents completed a survey about children's media use. Picture book reading for less than 30 min a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated live. Watching video content for more than 1 h a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated on video. Results are interpreted in terms of individual differences in experiences of traditional and digital media and the development of symbolic understanding.

记忆力的发展贯穿于生命的最初几年,并受到日常经历的影响,如接触书籍和电视等媒体。延迟模仿(DI)的记忆要求学步儿童形成目标动作的表象,以便他们以后可以用来再现这些动作,除了测量对真实事件的记忆外,它还可以用来测量对通过媒体观看的事件的记忆。除了更传统的图画书阅读形式外,幼儿还经常接触多种形式的数字媒体。在受试者内设计中,我们使用法兰克福模仿测试对两岁幼儿(n = 89)的 DI 任务进行了记忆评估。在现场和视频演示后,对延迟模仿进行了评估。家长填写了一份有关儿童使用媒体情况的调查表。每天阅读图画书少于 30 分钟的儿童对现场演示动作的记忆得分较低。每天观看视频内容的时间超过 1 小时,则预示孩子对视频演示动作的记忆得分较低。研究从传统媒体和数字媒体体验的个体差异以及符号理解的发展角度对结果进行了解释。
{"title":"Toddler's memory and media—Picture book reading and watching video content are associated with memory at 2 years of age","authors":"Felix-Sebastian Koch,&nbsp;Annette Sundqvist,&nbsp;Ulrika Birberg Thornberg,&nbsp;Rachel Barr,&nbsp;Mikael Heimann","doi":"10.1111/infa.12609","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12609","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Memory develops across the course of the first years of life and is influenced by daily experiences, such as exposure to media like books and television. Memory as tapped by Deferred imitation (DI) requires that toddlers form a representation of the target actions that they can later use to reproduce the actions and in addition to measuring memory for real live events, it can also be used to measure memory for events viewed through media. Toddlers are frequently exposed to multiple forms of digital media in addition to more traditional forms of picture book reading. In a within-subjects design, memory was assessed with a DI task in 2-year-olds (<i>n</i> = 89) using the Frankfurt Imitation Test. Deferred imitation was assessed after live and video demonstrations. Parents completed a survey about children's media use. Picture book reading for less than 30 min a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated live. Watching video content for more than 1 h a day predicted lower memory scores for actions demonstrated on video. Results are interpreted in terms of individual differences in experiences of traditional and digital media and the development of symbolic understanding.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"729-749"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12609","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Co-regulation, relationship quality, and infant distress vocalizations observed during mother-infant interactions: Influences of maternal depression and different contexts 在母婴互动过程中观察到的共同调节、关系质量和婴儿苦恼发声:母亲抑郁和不同环境的影响。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12617
Elizabeth L. Leong, Dale M. Stack, Olivia K. Lazimbat, Samantha Bouchard, Tiffany M. Field

Mother-infant interactions are co-regulated and provide the foundation for mother-infant relationship quality. The implications of maternal depression and contextual demands (i.e., reinstating the interaction following maternal unavailability and vocalized infant distress) on observationally coded co-regulation in mother-infant dyads (n = 40) at 4-months was investigated. Associations among co-regulation patterns and mother-infant relationship quality was also examined. Dyads participated in Still-Face (SF) and Separation (SP) procedures, with periods of maternal emotional and physical unavailability. Co-regulation was captured using the Revised Relational Coding System. Relationship quality was examined using the Emotional Availability Scales. Dyads in the depressed group had significantly more unilateral exchanges than the non-depressed group following the SF and SP perturbations. The depressed group also had significantly more distress vocalizations during the SP perturbation than the non-depressed group. Co-regulation in the depressed group was less disrupted by the SF perturbation. Positive relationship quality dimensions (maternal sensitivity, structuring, and infant responsiveness) were associated with more symmetrical and less unilateral co-regulation regardless of the interaction period. There were also context-specific results pertaining to patterns of co-regulation and associated maternal hostility and infant responsiveness. Results highlight co-regulatory differences in depressed mothers and their infants and how these differences are exacerbated by contextual demands.

母婴互动是共同调节的,是母婴关系质量的基础。本研究调查了母亲抑郁和情境需求(即在母亲不在和婴儿发声求助后恢复互动)对母婴二人组(n = 40)4 个月时观察编码共同调节的影响。此外,还研究了共同调节模式与母婴关系质量之间的关联。母婴组合参加了静止面对(SF)和分离(SP)程序,在这两个程序中,母亲在情感和身体上都处于不在场状态。共同调节采用修订的关系编码系统(Revised Relational Coding System)。使用情感可得性量表对关系质量进行检测。与非抑郁组相比,抑郁组在受到 SF 和 SP 干扰后的单边交流明显增多。抑郁组在SP扰动时的苦恼发声也明显多于非抑郁组。抑郁组的共同调节能力受 SF 扰动的干扰较小。积极的关系质量维度(母性敏感性、结构化和婴儿反应性)与更对称和更少的单侧共同调节相关,而与交互作用期无关。在共同调节的模式以及相关的母亲敌意和婴儿反应性方面,也有针对具体情境的结果。研究结果突显了抑郁母亲及其婴儿的共同调节差异,以及这些差异如何因情境需求而加剧。
{"title":"Co-regulation, relationship quality, and infant distress vocalizations observed during mother-infant interactions: Influences of maternal depression and different contexts","authors":"Elizabeth L. Leong,&nbsp;Dale M. Stack,&nbsp;Olivia K. Lazimbat,&nbsp;Samantha Bouchard,&nbsp;Tiffany M. Field","doi":"10.1111/infa.12617","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12617","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Mother-infant interactions are co-regulated and provide the foundation for mother-infant relationship quality. The implications of maternal depression and contextual demands (i.e., reinstating the interaction following maternal unavailability and vocalized infant distress) on observationally coded co-regulation in mother-infant dyads (<i>n</i> = 40) at 4-months was investigated. Associations among co-regulation patterns and mother-infant relationship quality was also examined. Dyads participated in Still-Face (SF) and Separation (SP) procedures, with periods of maternal emotional and physical unavailability. Co-regulation was captured using the Revised Relational Coding System. Relationship quality was examined using the Emotional Availability Scales. Dyads in the depressed group had significantly more unilateral exchanges than the non-depressed group following the SF and SP perturbations. The depressed group also had significantly more distress vocalizations during the SP perturbation than the non-depressed group. Co-regulation in the depressed group was less disrupted by the SF perturbation. Positive relationship quality dimensions (maternal sensitivity, structuring, and infant responsiveness) were associated with more symmetrical and less unilateral co-regulation regardless of the interaction period. There were also context-specific results pertaining to patterns of co-regulation and associated maternal hostility and infant responsiveness. Results highlight co-regulatory differences in depressed mothers and their infants and how these differences are exacerbated by contextual demands.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"933-957"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12617","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Of the importance to reconsider individual variability in infant studies: Family traits do impact turn-taking perception in 6-month-olds 在婴儿研究中重新考虑个体差异的重要性:家庭特征确实会影响 6 个月大婴儿的轮流感知。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12612
Bastien Meunier, Stéphanie Barbu, Alice Rabiller, Nicolas Dollion, Alban Lemasson, Virginie Durier

Turn-taking is a universal pattern of human conversations characterized by a fast exchange of turns between speakers and an avoidance of overlaps. Language is embedded in this conversational skill acquired well before it during infancy, through everyday interactions with caregivers. The earliness of this skill and its link with language allows us to test whether social environment shapes early language development. We therefore study turn-taking perception of 6-month-old infants by measuring their gazes during video presentation of three different conversational situations where the turn is explicitly given, normally taken or taken with an overlap. We studied 51 infants to cover several family and infant characteristics: infants' sex, presence of siblings, and family socioeconomic status (SES). We found that infants looked more at the second speaker when she overlapped the first speaker than in the other situations, but not all infants were equally sensitive. Indeed, infants from high-SES families reacted differently to the three situations, while infants from the two lower SES categories did not. Also, only singletons reacted differently by looking more at the second speaker after the overlapping and turn-giving situations, and not after the turn-taking situation. Our results emphasize the importance of early social experiences on language development.

轮流说话是人类对话的一种普遍模式,其特点是说话者之间快速轮流交流,避免重叠。这种会话技能蕴含着语言,早在婴儿期就已通过与照看者的日常互动习得。这种技能的出现及其与语言的联系使我们能够检验社会环境是否会影响早期语言的发展。因此,我们对 6 个月大婴儿的轮流感知进行了研究,方法是测量他们在视频演示三种不同对话情境时的注视情况,即明确给出轮流、正常轮流或重叠轮流。我们对 51 名婴儿进行了研究,涵盖了若干家庭和婴儿特征:婴儿性别、是否有兄弟姐妹以及家庭社会经济地位(SES)。我们发现,与其他情况相比,当第二个说话者与第一个说话者重叠时,婴儿会更多地注视第二个说话者,但并非所有婴儿都同样敏感。事实上,来自高社会经济地位家庭的婴儿对三种情况的反应不同,而来自两种较低社会经济地位家庭的婴儿则没有。此外,只有单亲家庭的婴儿在重叠情境和轮流情境下会做出不同的反应,即更多地注视第二个说话者,而在轮流情境下则不会。我们的研究结果强调了早期社会经验对语言发展的重要性。
{"title":"Of the importance to reconsider individual variability in infant studies: Family traits do impact turn-taking perception in 6-month-olds","authors":"Bastien Meunier,&nbsp;Stéphanie Barbu,&nbsp;Alice Rabiller,&nbsp;Nicolas Dollion,&nbsp;Alban Lemasson,&nbsp;Virginie Durier","doi":"10.1111/infa.12612","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12612","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Turn-taking is a universal pattern of human conversations characterized by a fast exchange of turns between speakers and an avoidance of overlaps. Language is embedded in this conversational skill acquired well before it during infancy, through everyday interactions with caregivers. The earliness of this skill and its link with language allows us to test whether social environment shapes early language development. We therefore study turn-taking perception of 6-month-old infants by measuring their gazes during video presentation of three different conversational situations where the turn is explicitly given, normally taken or taken with an overlap. We studied 51 infants to cover several family and infant characteristics: infants' sex, presence of siblings, and family socioeconomic status (SES). We found that infants looked more at the second speaker when she overlapped the first speaker than in the other situations, but not all infants were equally sensitive. Indeed, infants from high-SES families reacted differently to the three situations, while infants from the two lower SES categories did not. Also, only singletons reacted differently by looking more at the second speaker after the overlapping and turn-giving situations, and not after the turn-taking situation. Our results emphasize the importance of early social experiences on language development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 6","pages":"894-907"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12612","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724772","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants' facial electromyographic responses to the sight of emotional interpersonal touch 婴儿面部肌电图对情绪性人际触摸的反应。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-06-14 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12600
Margaret Addabbo, Victoria Licht, Chiara Turati

Adult studies have shown that observed interpersonal touch provides crucial information about others' emotional states. Yet, despite the unique communicative function of touch during development, very little is known about infants' sensitivity to the emotional valence of observed touches. To investigate this issue, we measured facial electromyographic (EMG) activity in response to positive (caress) and negative (scratches) observed touches in a sample of 11-month-old infants. Facial EMG activity was measured over the zygomaticus major (ZM) and corrugator supercilii muscles, respectively involved in positive (i.e., smiling) and negative (i.e., frowning) facial expressions. Results have shown distinct activations of the ZM during the observation of scratches and caresses. In particular, significantly greater activation of the ZM (smiling muscle) emerged specifically in response to the observation of caresses compared to scratches. Our finding suggests that, in infancy, observed affective touches can evoke emotional facial reactions.

成人研究表明,观察到的人际触摸提供了有关他人情绪状态的重要信息。然而,尽管触摸在发育过程中具有独特的交流功能,但人们对婴儿对观察到的触摸的情绪价值的敏感性却知之甚少。为了研究这个问题,我们对 11 个月大的婴儿样本进行了面部肌电图(EMG)活动的测量,以观察他们对正面(爱抚)和负面(抓挠)触摸的反应。面部肌电图活动是通过颧大肌(ZM)和皱纹上提肌测量的,这两块肌肉分别参与正面(即微笑)和负面(即皱眉)的面部表情。研究结果表明,在观察抓挠和爱抚时,ZM 的激活明显不同。尤其是在观察爱抚时,ZM(微笑肌)的激活程度明显高于观察抓挠时。我们的研究结果表明,在婴儿期,观察到的情感抚摸可以唤起面部的情绪反应。
{"title":"Infants' facial electromyographic responses to the sight of emotional interpersonal touch","authors":"Margaret Addabbo,&nbsp;Victoria Licht,&nbsp;Chiara Turati","doi":"10.1111/infa.12600","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12600","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Adult studies have shown that observed interpersonal touch provides crucial information about others' emotional states. Yet, despite the unique communicative function of touch during development, very little is known about infants' sensitivity to the emotional valence of observed touches. To investigate this issue, we measured facial electromyographic (EMG) activity in response to positive (caress) and negative (scratches) observed touches in a sample of 11-month-old infants. Facial EMG activity was measured over the zygomaticus major (ZM) and corrugator supercilii muscles, respectively involved in positive (i.e., smiling) and negative (i.e., frowning) facial expressions. Results have shown distinct activations of the ZM during the observation of scratches and caresses. In particular, significantly greater activation of the ZM (smiling muscle) emerged specifically in response to the observation of caresses compared to scratches. Our finding suggests that, in infancy, observed affective touches can evoke emotional facial reactions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"660-671"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-06-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141318648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddlers' prior social experience with speakers influences their word learning 幼儿先前与说话者的社交经验会影响他们的单词学习。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-29 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12608
Elise Breitfeld, Anna M. Compton, Jenny R. Saffran

Toddlers prefer to learn from familiar adults, particularly their caregivers, and perform better on word learning tasks when taught by caregivers than by strangers. However, it remains unclear why toddlers learn better from caregivers than from strangers. One possibility is that toddlers are more receptive to learning from individuals whom they have found to be engaging in previous interactions. The current study tested whether toddlers learn more from an unfamiliar adult who was previously engaging than from an unfamiliar adult who was previously unengaging. Toddlers (27–29 months, N = 40) were taught labels for novel objects by two different experimenters. Prior to word learning, toddlers watched pre-recorded videos of one experimenter utilizing engaging behaviors (i.e., using infant-directed speech, gestures, eye contact, and positive affect) and one experimenter utilizing unengaging behaviors (i.e., using adult-directed speech, no gestures, no eye contact, and neutral affect). Both experimenters were equally engaging during labeling. Word learning was then tested using a looking-while-listening paradigm. The results of linear mixed-effects model, cluster-based permutation, and growth curve analyses suggest heightened performance for words that were taught by the experimenter who was previously engaging. These results begin to reveal the kinds of social experiences that promote success in early word learning.

学步儿童更喜欢向熟悉的成人学习,尤其是向他们的看护人学习,而且在由看护人教他们学习单词时,他们的表现要好于陌生人。然而,为什么幼儿从看护人那里学到的东西比从陌生人那里学到的东西更多,目前还不清楚。一种可能是,幼儿更容易接受向他们在以前的互动中发现的人学习。本研究测试了学步儿童是否更容易从以前有互动的陌生成人那里学习,而不是从以前没有互动的陌生成人那里学习。幼儿(27-29 个月,N=40)由两名不同的实验者教授新奇物体的标签。在学习单词之前,幼儿观看了预先录制的视频,视频中一名实验者使用了吸引幼儿的行为(即使用婴儿引导的语言、手势、眼神交流和积极的情感),另一名实验者使用了不吸引幼儿的行为(即使用成人引导的语言、无手势、无眼神交流和中性情感)。在贴标签时,两位实验者都同样投入。然后采用边听边看的范式对单词学习进行测试。线性混合效应模型、基于聚类的排列分析和成长曲线分析的结果表明,由先前投入的实验者教授的单词学习成绩会有所提高。这些结果开始揭示了促进早期单词学习成功的社会经验类型。
{"title":"Toddlers' prior social experience with speakers influences their word learning","authors":"Elise Breitfeld,&nbsp;Anna M. Compton,&nbsp;Jenny R. Saffran","doi":"10.1111/infa.12608","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12608","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Toddlers prefer to learn from familiar adults, particularly their caregivers, and perform better on word learning tasks when taught by caregivers than by strangers. However, it remains unclear why toddlers learn better from caregivers than from strangers. One possibility is that toddlers are more receptive to learning from individuals whom they have found to be engaging in previous interactions. The current study tested whether toddlers learn more from an unfamiliar adult who was previously engaging than from an unfamiliar adult who was previously unengaging. Toddlers (27–29 months, <i>N</i> = 40) were taught labels for novel objects by two different experimenters. Prior to word learning, toddlers watched pre-recorded videos of one experimenter utilizing engaging behaviors (i.e., using infant-directed speech, gestures, eye contact, and positive affect) and one experimenter utilizing unengaging behaviors (i.e., using adult-directed speech, no gestures, no eye contact, and neutral affect). Both experimenters were equally engaging during labeling. Word learning was then tested using a looking-while-listening paradigm. The results of linear mixed-effects model, cluster-based permutation, and growth curve analyses suggest heightened performance for words that were taught by the experimenter who was previously engaging. These results begin to reveal the kinds of social experiences that promote success in early word learning.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"771-788"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12608","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141161909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive control in infancy: Attentional predictors using a tablet-based measure 婴儿期的认知控制:使用平板电脑测量法预测注意力。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12599
Emma Macrae, Bosiljka Milosavljevic, Laura Katus, Luke Mason, Marta Perapoch Amadó, Maria Rozhko, Michelle de Haan, Clare E. Elwell, Sophie E. Moore, Sarah Lloyd-Fox, The BRIGHT Project Team

Cognitive control is a predictor of later-life outcomes and may underpin higher order executive processes. The present study examines the development of early cognitive control during the first 24-month. We evaluated a tablet-based assessment of cognitive control among infants aged 18- and 24-month. We also examined concurrent and longitudinal associations between attentional disengagement, general cognitive skills and cognitive control. Participants (N = 60, 30 female) completed the tablet-task at 18- and 24-month of age. Attentional disengagement and general cognitive development were assessed at 5-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-month using an eye-tracking measure and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), respectively. The cognitive control task demonstrated good internal consistency, sensitivity to age-related change in performance and stable individual differences. No associations were found between infant cognitive control and MSEL scores longitudinally or concurrently. The eye-tracking task revealed that slower attentional disengagement at 8-month, but faster disengagement at 18-month, predicted higher cognitive control scores at 24-month. This task may represent a useful tool for measuring emergent cognitive control. The multifaceted relationship between attention and infant cognitive control suggests that the rapid development of the attentional system in infancy results in distinct attentional skills, at different ages, being relevant for cognitive control development.

认知控制能力是日后生活结果的预测因素,可能是高阶执行过程的基础。本研究探讨了 24 个月内婴儿早期认知控制能力的发展情况。我们对 18 个月和 24 个月婴儿的认知控制能力进行了基于平板电脑的评估。我们还研究了注意力脱离、一般认知技能和认知控制之间的并行和纵向关联。参与者(N = 60,30 名女性)分别在 18 个月和 24 个月大时完成了平板电脑任务。在5、8、12、18和24个月大时,分别使用眼动追踪测量法和穆伦早期学习量表(MSEL)对注意力分离和一般认知发展进行了评估。认知控制任务表现出良好的内部一致性、对与年龄相关的表现变化的敏感性和稳定的个体差异。在婴儿认知控制和 MSEL 分数之间没有发现纵向或并发的关联。眼动跟踪任务显示,8 个月大时注意力分离较慢,而 18 个月大时注意力分离较快,这预示着 24 个月大时认知控制得分较高。这项任务可能是测量新生认知控制能力的有用工具。注意力与婴儿认知控制能力之间的多方面关系表明,婴儿期注意力系统的快速发展导致不同年龄段的注意力技能与认知控制能力的发展息息相关。
{"title":"Cognitive control in infancy: Attentional predictors using a tablet-based measure","authors":"Emma Macrae,&nbsp;Bosiljka Milosavljevic,&nbsp;Laura Katus,&nbsp;Luke Mason,&nbsp;Marta Perapoch Amadó,&nbsp;Maria Rozhko,&nbsp;Michelle de Haan,&nbsp;Clare E. Elwell,&nbsp;Sophie E. Moore,&nbsp;Sarah Lloyd-Fox,&nbsp;The BRIGHT Project Team","doi":"10.1111/infa.12599","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12599","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Cognitive control is a predictor of later-life outcomes and may underpin higher order executive processes. The present study examines the development of early cognitive control during the first 24-month. We evaluated a tablet-based assessment of cognitive control among infants aged 18- and 24-month. We also examined concurrent and longitudinal associations between attentional disengagement, general cognitive skills and cognitive control. Participants (<i>N</i> = 60, 30 female) completed the tablet-task at 18- and 24-month of age. Attentional disengagement and general cognitive development were assessed at 5-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-month using an eye-tracking measure and the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), respectively. The cognitive control task demonstrated good internal consistency, sensitivity to age-related change in performance and stable individual differences. No associations were found between infant cognitive control and MSEL scores longitudinally or concurrently. The eye-tracking task revealed that <i>slower</i> attentional disengagement at 8-month, but <i>faster</i> disengagement at 18-month, predicted higher cognitive control scores at 24-month. This task may represent a useful tool for measuring emergent cognitive control. The multifaceted relationship between attention and infant cognitive control suggests that the rapid development of the attentional system in infancy results in distinct attentional skills, at different ages, being relevant for cognitive control development.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 4","pages":"631-655"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12599","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141071657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of goal saliency and verbal information on selective imitation in 16- to 18-month-olds 目标显著性和语言信息对 16 至 18 个月幼儿选择性模仿的影响。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12601
Léonie Trouillet, Ricarda Bothe, Nivedita Mani, Birgit Elsner

This study aimed to assess which action component (movement or goal) infants prioritize in their imitation behavior when they get information about its relevance from two important sources: perceptual goal saliency and experimenter's verbal information. 16- to 18-month-olds (N = 72) observed how the experimenter moved a toy mouse with a hopping or sliding movement onto one of two empty spaces (low goal saliency) or 2D circles (medium saliency), or inside one of two 3D houses (high saliency). Before the demonstration, the experimenter verbally announced the movement style or the goal. Results showed that verbal action descriptions did not influence infants' imitation. However, matching previous findings, infants imitated the goal more often than the movement in the high-saliency condition, and the movement more often than the goal in the low-saliency condition. Moreover, in the novel medium-saliency condition, infants imitated both components equally often. Thus, selective imitation varied as a function of perceptual goal saliency, but not of verbal cues. This suggests that perceptual features can enhance infants' bottom-up processing and imitation of action components, while the impact of top-down processes based on verbal cues may vary depending on task characteristics and infants' verbal abilities, inducing a need for further research.

本研究旨在评估当婴儿从两个重要来源(感知目标显著性和实验者的口头信息)获得相关信息时,他们在模仿行为中会优先考虑哪个动作部分(动作或目标)。16 到 18 个月大的婴儿(72 人)观察实验者如何用跳跃或滑动的动作将玩具老鼠移动到两个空位(低目标突出)或二维圆圈(中等突出)中的一个,或两个三维房子(高突出)中的一个。在演示之前,实验者会口头宣布动作方式或目标。结果显示,口头动作描述并不影响婴儿的模仿。然而,与之前的研究结果一致的是,在高显著性条件下,婴儿模仿目标的次数多于模仿动作的次数,而在低显著性条件下,婴儿模仿动作的次数多于模仿目标的次数。此外,在新颖的中等清晰度条件下,婴儿模仿两个部分的频率相同。因此,选择性模仿随知觉目标显著性的变化而变化,但与言语线索无关。这表明,感知特征可以增强婴儿对动作成分的自下而上的处理和模仿,而基于语言线索的自上而下的过程的影响则可能因任务特征和婴儿的语言能力而异,因此需要进一步的研究。
{"title":"The impact of goal saliency and verbal information on selective imitation in 16- to 18-month-olds","authors":"Léonie Trouillet,&nbsp;Ricarda Bothe,&nbsp;Nivedita Mani,&nbsp;Birgit Elsner","doi":"10.1111/infa.12601","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12601","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study aimed to assess which action component (movement or goal) infants prioritize in their imitation behavior when they get information about its relevance from two important sources: perceptual goal saliency and experimenter's verbal information. 16- to 18-month-olds (<i>N</i> = 72) observed how the experimenter moved a toy mouse with a hopping or sliding movement onto one of two empty spaces (low goal saliency) or 2D circles (medium saliency), or inside one of two 3D houses (high saliency). Before the demonstration, the experimenter verbally announced the movement style or the goal. Results showed that verbal action descriptions did not influence infants' imitation. However, matching previous findings, infants imitated the goal more often than the movement in the high-saliency condition, and the movement more often than the goal in the low-saliency condition. Moreover, in the novel medium-saliency condition, infants imitated both components equally often. Thus, selective imitation varied as a function of perceptual goal saliency, but not of verbal cues. This suggests that perceptual features can enhance infants' bottom-up processing and imitation of action components, while the impact of top-down processes based on verbal cues may vary depending on task characteristics and infants' verbal abilities, inducing a need for further research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"713-728"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12601","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141066452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Prosody outweighs statistics in 6-month-old German-learning infants' speech segmentation 在 6 个月大的德语学习婴儿的语音分段中,拟声词的作用大于统计量
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-05-04 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12593
Mireia Marimon, Alan Langus, Barbara Höhle

It is well established that infants use various cues to find words within fluent speech from about 7 to 8 months of age. Research suggests that two main mechanisms support infants' speech segmentation: prosodic cues like the word stress patterns, and distributional cues like transitional probabilities (TPs). We tested 6-month-old German-learning infants' use of prosodic and statistical cues for speech segmentation in three experiments. In Experiment 1, infants were familiarized with an artificial language string where TPs signaled either word boundaries or iambic words—a stress pattern that is disfavored in German. Experiment 2 was a control and only the test phase was presented. In Experiment 3, prosodic cues were absent in the string and only TPs signaled word boundaries. All experiments included the same conditions at test: disyllabic words with high TPs in the string, words with low TPs and words with non-co-occurring syllables. Results showed that infants relied more strongly on prosodic cues than on TPs for word segmentation. Notably, no segmentation evidence emerged when prosodic cues were absent in the string. This finding underlines early impacts of language-specific structural properties on segmentation mechanisms.

众所周知,婴儿从 7 到 8 个月大开始就会使用各种线索在流利的语音中寻找单词。研究表明,支持婴儿语音分段的机制主要有两种:单词重音模式等前音线索和过渡概率(TPs)等分布线索。我们在三个实验中测试了 6 个月大的德语学习婴儿使用前音和统计线索进行语音分段的情况。在实验 1 中,婴儿熟悉了一个人工语串,其中的 TPs 表示单词边界或单音词(德语中不喜欢的重音模式)。实验 2 是对照组,只呈现测试阶段。在实验 3 中,语串中没有前音线索,只有 TPs 表示单词边界。所有实验都包括相同的测试条件:字符串中 TP 值高的双音节词、TP 值低的词和音节不共存的词。结果表明,在进行单词分段时,婴儿更依赖于前音线索而非 TPs。值得注意的是,如果词串中没有前音线索,则不会出现分词证据。这一发现强调了语言特有的结构特性对分词机制的早期影响。
{"title":"Prosody outweighs statistics in 6-month-old German-learning infants' speech segmentation","authors":"Mireia Marimon,&nbsp;Alan Langus,&nbsp;Barbara Höhle","doi":"10.1111/infa.12593","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12593","url":null,"abstract":"<p>It is well established that infants use various cues to find words within fluent speech from about 7 to 8 months of age. Research suggests that two main mechanisms support infants' speech segmentation: prosodic cues like the word stress patterns, and distributional cues like transitional probabilities (TPs). We tested 6-month-old German-learning infants' use of prosodic and statistical cues for speech segmentation in three experiments. In Experiment 1, infants were familiarized with an artificial language string where TPs signaled either word boundaries or iambic words—a stress pattern that is disfavored in German. Experiment 2 was a control and only the test phase was presented. In Experiment 3, prosodic cues were absent in the string and only TPs signaled word boundaries. All experiments included the same conditions at test: disyllabic words with high TPs in the string, words with low TPs and words with non-co-occurring syllables. Results showed that infants relied more strongly on prosodic cues than on TPs for word segmentation. Notably, no segmentation evidence emerged when prosodic cues were absent in the string. This finding underlines early impacts of language-specific structural properties on segmentation mechanisms.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"29 5","pages":"750-770"},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-05-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12593","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140839918","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1