首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
Positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic can buffer regulatory problems in infants facing the COVID-19 pandemic 新冠肺炎大流行前的积极共付可以缓解面临新冠肺炎大流行的婴儿的监管问题。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12561
Tiago Miguel Pinto, Bárbara Figueiredo

Coparenting can be a development-enhancing or risk-promoting environment for infant regulatory capacity, mainly in the presence of adversity. This study aimed to analyze the association between positive and negative coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity in the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic, an adverse condition. A sample of 71 first-born infants and their mothers and fathers from a longitudinal cohort in Portugal were assessed at 2 weeks postpartum before the COVID-19 pandemic and again at 6 months postpartum, before (n = 35) or during the COVID-19 pandemic (n = 36). Parents completed measures of positive and negative coparenting and infant regulatory capacity in both assessment waves. Results revealed that the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic moderates the association between mothers' and fathers' positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity at 6 months. The association between positive coparenting and regulatory capacity was stronger in infants facing the COVID-19 pandemic, than in infants who did not face the COVID-19 pandemic. Positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic may be a development-enhancing environment for infant regulatory capacity in the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Positive coparenting may buffer regulatory problems in infants facing adverse conditions.

共同养育可能是一种促进婴儿调节能力发展或风险促进的环境,主要是在逆境中。本研究旨在分析新冠肺炎大流行前的阳性和阴性共付与新冠肺炎大流行(一种不良情况)下婴儿调节能力之间的关系。在新冠肺炎大流行前产后2周,以及在产后6个月、新冠肺炎大流行前(n=35)或大流行期间(n=36),对来自葡萄牙纵向队列的71名首次出生婴儿及其父母的样本进行了评估。父母在两次评估浪潮中都完成了积极和消极分担以及婴儿监管能力的测量。结果显示,新冠肺炎大流行的存在调节了新冠肺炎大流行前母亲和父亲的阳性共付与6个月婴儿监管能力之间的关联。面对新冠肺炎大流行的婴儿比没有面对新冠肺炎大流行的婴儿,积极共付与监管能力之间的关联更强。新冠肺炎大流行前的积极共租可能是新冠肺炎大流行情况下婴儿监管能力的发展环境。积极的共租可能会缓解面临不利条件的婴儿的监管问题。
{"title":"Positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic can buffer regulatory problems in infants facing the COVID-19 pandemic","authors":"Tiago Miguel Pinto,&nbsp;Bárbara Figueiredo","doi":"10.1111/infa.12561","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12561","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Coparenting can be a development-enhancing or risk-promoting environment for infant regulatory capacity, mainly in the presence of adversity. This study aimed to analyze the association between positive and negative coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity in the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic, an adverse condition. A sample of 71 first-born infants and their mothers and fathers from a longitudinal cohort in Portugal were assessed at 2 weeks postpartum before the COVID-19 pandemic and again at 6 months postpartum, before (<i>n</i> = 35) or during the COVID-19 pandemic (<i>n</i> = 36). Parents completed measures of positive and negative coparenting and infant regulatory capacity in both assessment waves. Results revealed that the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic moderates the association between mothers' and fathers' positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic and infant regulatory capacity at 6 months. The association between positive coparenting and regulatory capacity was stronger in infants facing the COVID-19 pandemic, than in infants who did not face the COVID-19 pandemic. Positive coparenting previous to the COVID-19 pandemic may be a development-enhancing environment for infant regulatory capacity in the presence of the COVID-19 pandemic. Positive coparenting may buffer regulatory problems in infants facing adverse conditions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"1067-1085"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41133586","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correlates of infant pointing frequency in the first year 第一年婴儿指向频率的相关性。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-25 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12560
Ebru Ger, Aylin C. Küntay, Sura Ertaş, Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar, Ulf Liszkowski

This study examines the emergence of concurrent correlates of infant pointing frequency with the aim of contributing to its ontogenetic theories. We measured monthly from 8 to 12 months infants' (N = 56) index-finger pointing frequency along with several candidate correlates: (1) family socioeconomic status (SES), (2) mothers' pointing production, and (3) infants' point following to targets in front of and behind them. Results revealed that (1) infants increased their pointing frequency across age, but high-SES infants had a steeper increase, and a higher pointing frequency than low-SES infants from 10 months onward, (2) maternal pointing frequency was not associated with infant pointing frequency at any age, (3) infants' point following abilities to targets behind their visual fields was positively associated with their pointing frequency at 12 months, after pointing had already emerged around 10 months. Findings suggest that family SES impacts infants' pointing development more generally, not just through maternal pointing. The association between pointing and following points to targets behind, but not in front, suggests that a higher level of referential understanding emerges after, and perhaps through the production of pointing.

这项研究考察了婴儿指向频率的并发相关性的出现,目的是为其个体遗传学理论做出贡献。我们测量了8至12个月婴儿(N=56)的食指指向频率,以及几个候选相关性:(1)家庭社会经济地位(SES),(2)母亲的指向能力,以及(3)婴儿对前后目标的点跟踪。结果显示:(1)婴儿的指向频率随着年龄的增长而增加,但从10个月起,高SES婴儿的指向次数增加得更大,并且比低SES婴儿更高;(2)母亲的指向次数与任何年龄的婴儿指向次数无关,(3)婴儿对视野后面目标的点跟踪能力与他们在12个月时的指向频率呈正相关,而在10个月左右已经出现了指向。研究结果表明,家庭SES更普遍地影响婴儿的指向发育,而不仅仅是通过母亲的指向。指向和跟随指向后面而不是前面的目标之间的联系表明,在指向之后,也许通过指向的产生,会产生更高水平的参照理解。
{"title":"Correlates of infant pointing frequency in the first year","authors":"Ebru Ger,&nbsp;Aylin C. Küntay,&nbsp;Sura Ertaş,&nbsp;Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar,&nbsp;Ulf Liszkowski","doi":"10.1111/infa.12560","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12560","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This study examines the emergence of concurrent correlates of infant pointing frequency with the aim of contributing to its ontogenetic theories. We measured monthly from 8 to 12 months infants' (<i>N</i> = 56) index-finger pointing frequency along with several candidate correlates: (1) family socioeconomic status (SES), (2) mothers' pointing production, and (3) infants' point following to targets in front of and behind them. Results revealed that (1) infants increased their pointing frequency across age, but high-SES infants had a steeper increase, and a higher pointing frequency than low-SES infants from 10 months onward, (2) maternal pointing frequency was not associated with infant pointing frequency at any age, (3) infants' point following abilities to targets behind their visual fields was positively associated with their pointing frequency at 12 months, after pointing had already emerged around 10 months. Findings suggest that family SES impacts infants' pointing development more generally, not just through maternal pointing. The association between pointing and following points to targets behind, but not in front, suggests that a higher level of referential understanding emerges after, and perhaps through the production of pointing.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"986-1006"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12560","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154256","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Evidence of tactile arm stepping in newborns and its responsiveness to optic flows specifying self-translation 新生儿触觉手臂踩踏的证据及其对指定自我翻译的视觉流的反应。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-20 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12559
Marianne Barbu-Roth, David I. Anderson

Although the arms participate in many forms of human locomotion, we know very little about when arm movements emerge during locomotor development. Here we investigated whether newborns would make tactile arm stepping movements when we supported them almost horizontally so their hands touched a surface and blocked their leg movements. Building off prior work showing that newborns make more crawling and air stepping leg movements when exposed to optic flows specifying forward and backward self-translation, we also examined whether newborns would make more tactile arm steps when exposed to forward and backward optic flows compared to a random optic flow that did not specify translation. We found that newborns can perform arm stepping and produce a significantly higher number of tactile arm steps in the optic flow condition specifying backward translation than in the random optic flow condition. Both translating optic flow conditions had significantly higher numbers of alternating arm steps than the random optic flow condition. These findings show that tactile arm stepping exists at birth and that optic flows can facilitate their production, similar to leg stepping. We argue that these results further support the idea that a quadrupedal organization underlies early upright stepping.

尽管手臂参与了多种形式的人类运动,但我们对手臂运动在运动发育过程中何时出现知之甚少。在这里,我们调查了当我们几乎水平地支撑新生儿,使他们的手接触表面并阻挡他们的腿部运动时,新生儿是否会做出触觉手臂踩踏动作。先前的研究表明,新生儿在暴露于指定向前和向后自平移的光流时,会做出更多的爬行和气步腿运动,在此基础上,我们还研究了与不指定平移的随机光流相比,新生儿在接触向前和向后的光流后,是否会做出更多触觉手臂步进。我们发现,与随机光流条件相比,新生儿在指定向后平移的光流条件下可以进行手臂步进,并产生明显更高数量的触觉手臂步进。两种平移光流条件的交替臂步数都明显高于随机光流条件。这些发现表明,触觉手臂踩踏在出生时就存在,光流可以促进其产生,类似于腿部踩踏。我们认为,这些结果进一步支持了一种观点,即两足动物组织是早期直立行走的基础。
{"title":"Evidence of tactile arm stepping in newborns and its responsiveness to optic flows specifying self-translation","authors":"Marianne Barbu-Roth,&nbsp;David I. Anderson","doi":"10.1111/infa.12559","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12559","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although the arms participate in many forms of human locomotion, we know very little about when arm movements emerge during locomotor development. Here we investigated whether newborns would make tactile arm stepping movements when we supported them almost horizontally so their hands touched a surface and blocked their leg movements. Building off prior work showing that newborns make more crawling and air stepping leg movements when exposed to optic flows specifying forward and backward self-translation, we also examined whether newborns would make more tactile arm steps when exposed to forward and backward optic flows compared to a random optic flow that did not specify translation. We found that newborns can perform arm stepping and produce a significantly higher number of tactile arm steps in the optic flow condition specifying backward translation than in the random optic flow condition. Both translating optic flow conditions had significantly higher numbers of alternating arm steps than the random optic flow condition. These findings show that tactile arm stepping exists at birth and that optic flows can facilitate their production, similar to leg stepping. We argue that these results further support the idea that a quadrupedal organization underlies early upright stepping.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"1052-1066"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41169418","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Relation of infants' and mothers' pointing to infants' vocabulary measured directly and with parental reports 直接测量婴儿和母亲指向婴儿词汇的关系以及与父母报告的关系。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-01 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12558
Sura Ertaş, Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar, Ebru Ger, Ulf Liszkowski, Aylin C. Küntay

Infants' and parents' pointing gestures predict infants' concurrent and prospective language development. Most studies have measured vocabulary size using parental reports. However, parents tend to underestimate or overestimate infants' vocabulary necessitating the use of direct measures alongside parent reports. The present study examined whether mothers' index-finger pointing, and infants' whole-hand and index-finger pointing at 14 months associate with infants' receptive and expressive vocabulary based on parental reports and directly measured lexical processing efficiency (LPE) concurrently at 14 months and prospectively at 18 months. We used the decorated room paradigm to measure pointing frequency, the Turkish communicative development inventory I to measure infants' receptive vocabulary, Turkish communicative development inventory II to measure their expressive vocabulary, and the Looking-While-Listening (LWL) task to measure LPE. At 14 months, 34 mother-infant dyads, and at 18 months, 30 dyads were included in the analyses. We found that only infants' index-finger pointing frequency at 14 months predicted their LPE (both reaction time and accuracy) prospectively at 18 months but not concurrently at 14 months. Neither maternal pointing nor infants' pointing predicted their receptive and expressive vocabulary based on indirect measurement. The results extend the evidence on the relation between index-finger pointing and language development to a more direct measure of vocabulary.

婴儿和父母的指指手势可以预测婴儿同时和未来的语言发展。大多数研究都使用父母报告来测量词汇量。然而,父母往往低估或高估了婴儿的词汇量,因此有必要在父母报告的同时使用直接测量。本研究根据父母的报告,调查了母亲的食指指向、婴儿在14个月时的整只手和食指指向是否与婴儿的接受和表达词汇有关,并直接测量了14个月和18个月时词汇处理效率(LPE)。我们使用装饰室范式来测量指向频率,土耳其语交际发展量表I来测量婴儿的接受性词汇,土耳其语交流发展量表II来测量他们的表达性词汇,边听边看(LWL)任务来测量LPE。14个月时,34对母子和18个月时的30对被纳入分析。我们发现,只有婴儿在14个月时的食指指向频率可以预测他们在18个月时发生的LPE(反应时间和准确性),而不是同时预测14个月的LPE。无论是母亲的指向还是婴儿的指向,都不能基于间接测量来预测他们的接受和表达词汇。研究结果将食指指向与语言发展之间关系的证据扩展到更直接的词汇测量。
{"title":"Relation of infants' and mothers' pointing to infants' vocabulary measured directly and with parental reports","authors":"Sura Ertaş,&nbsp;Sümeyye Koşkulu-Sancar,&nbsp;Ebru Ger,&nbsp;Ulf Liszkowski,&nbsp;Aylin C. Küntay","doi":"10.1111/infa.12558","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12558","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Infants' and parents' pointing gestures predict infants' concurrent and prospective language development. Most studies have measured vocabulary size using parental reports. However, parents tend to underestimate or overestimate infants' vocabulary necessitating the use of direct measures alongside parent reports. The present study examined whether mothers' index-finger pointing, and infants' whole-hand and index-finger pointing at 14 months associate with infants' receptive and expressive vocabulary based on parental reports and directly measured lexical processing efficiency (LPE) concurrently at 14 months and prospectively at 18 months. We used the decorated room paradigm to measure pointing frequency, the Turkish communicative development inventory I to measure infants' receptive vocabulary, Turkish communicative development inventory II to measure their expressive vocabulary, and the Looking-While-Listening (LWL) task to measure LPE. At 14 months, 34 mother-infant dyads, and at 18 months, 30 dyads were included in the analyses. We found that only infants' index-finger pointing frequency at 14 months predicted their LPE (both reaction time and accuracy) prospectively at 18 months but not concurrently at 14 months. Neither maternal pointing nor infants' pointing predicted their receptive and expressive vocabulary based on indirect measurement. The results extend the evidence on the relation between index-finger pointing and language development to a more direct measure of vocabulary.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"1007-1029"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-09-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12558","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10127102","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early social referencing predicts object mastery motivation in infancy: Social antecedents of object mastery motivation 早期社会参照预测婴儿时期的对象掌握动机:对象掌握动机的社会前因。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-08-07 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12557
Amelia Yanchik, Judith M. Gardner, Bernard Z. Karmel, Peter Vietze

The researchers sought to understand the typical development of social referencing and object mastery motivation in infancy and to determine the relationship between social referencing and object mastery behaviors in infants from 7 to 22 months of age. The study included 36 infants who were followed as part of a longitudinal study of at-risk infants but were not determined to need care in the neonatal intesive care unit at birth. Both mastery behaviors of persistence and success showed a statistically significant effect of age, while social behaviors remained stable from 7 to 22 months. Social behaviors at 7 and 10 months were correlated with persistence at 22 months and success at 16 to 22 months demonstrating that early social referencing predicts object mastery behaviors in later infancy. Further research should determine if this trend extends to early childhood.

研究人员试图了解婴儿期社会参照和对象掌握动机的典型发展,并确定7至22个月大婴儿的社会参照与对象掌握行为之间的关系。这项研究包括36名婴儿,他们是高危婴儿纵向研究的一部分,但被确定为出生时不需要在新生儿重症监护室接受护理。坚持和成功的掌握行为都显示出年龄的统计学显著影响,而社交行为在7至22个月内保持稳定。7个月和10个月的社交行为与22个月的持续性和16-22个月的成功率相关,这表明早期社交参考可以预测婴儿后期的对象掌握行为。进一步的研究应该确定这种趋势是否延伸到幼儿期。
{"title":"Early social referencing predicts object mastery motivation in infancy: Social antecedents of object mastery motivation","authors":"Amelia Yanchik,&nbsp;Judith M. Gardner,&nbsp;Bernard Z. Karmel,&nbsp;Peter Vietze","doi":"10.1111/infa.12557","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12557","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The researchers sought to understand the typical development of social referencing and object mastery motivation in infancy and to determine the relationship between social referencing and object mastery behaviors in infants from 7 to 22 months of age. The study included 36 infants who were followed as part of a longitudinal study of at-risk infants but were not determined to need care in the neonatal intesive care unit at birth. Both mastery behaviors of persistence and success showed a statistically significant effect of age, while social behaviors remained stable from 7 to 22 months. Social behaviors at 7 and 10 months were correlated with persistence at 22 months and success at 16 to 22 months demonstrating that early social referencing predicts object mastery behaviors in later infancy. Further research should determine if this trend extends to early childhood.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 6","pages":"976-985"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9954118","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Automated measurement of infant and mother Duchenne facial expressions in the Face-to-Face/Still-Face 自动测量婴儿和母亲的杜氏面部表情在面对面/静止的脸
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-19 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12556
Yeojin Amy Ahn, Itir Önal Ertuğrul, Sy-Miin Chow, Jeffrey F. Cohn, Daniel S. Messinger

Although still-face effects are well-studied, little is known about the degree to which the Face-to-Face/Still-Face (FFSF) is associated with the production of intense affective displays. Duchenne smiling expresses more intense positive affect than non-Duchenne smiling, while Duchenne cry-faces express more intense negative affect than non-Duchenne cry-faces. Forty 4-month-old infants and their mothers completed the FFSF, and key affect-indexing facial Action Units (AUs) were coded by expert Facial Action Coding System coders for the first 30 s of each FFSF episode. Computer vision software, automated facial affect recognition (AFAR), identified AUs for the entire 2-min episodes. Expert coding and AFAR produced similar infant and mother Duchenne and non-Duchenne FFSF effects, highlighting the convergent validity of automated measurement. Substantive AFAR analyses indicated that both infant Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiling declined from the FF to the SF, but only Duchenne smiling increased from the SF to the RE. In similar fashion, the magnitude of mother Duchenne smiling changes over the FFSF were 2–4 times greater than non-Duchenne smiling changes. Duchenne expressions appear to be a sensitive index of intense infant and mother affective valence that are accessible to automated measurement and may be a target for future FFSF research.

尽管静止面孔效应研究得很好,但人们对面对面/静止面孔(FFSF)与产生强烈情感表现的关联程度知之甚少。杜氏微笑比非杜氏微笑表达出更强烈的积极情绪,而杜氏哭脸比非杜氏哭脸表达出更强烈的消极情绪。44个月大的婴儿及其母亲完成FFSF,由面部动作编码系统专家编码每个FFSF前30秒的关键情感索引面部动作单元(au)。计算机视觉软件,自动面部情感识别(AFAR),识别出整个2分钟剧集的au。专家编码和AFAR产生了相似的母婴杜氏和非杜氏FFSF效应,突出了自动化测量的收敛效度。实质性的AFAR分析表明,婴儿杜氏微笑和非杜氏微笑从FF到SF都有所下降,但只有杜氏微笑从SF到RE有所增加。同样,母亲杜氏微笑在FFSF中的变化幅度是非杜氏微笑变化的2-4倍。杜氏表达是婴儿和母亲强烈情感效价的敏感指标,可以自动测量,可能是未来FFSF研究的目标。
{"title":"Automated measurement of infant and mother Duchenne facial expressions in the Face-to-Face/Still-Face","authors":"Yeojin Amy Ahn,&nbsp;Itir Önal Ertuğrul,&nbsp;Sy-Miin Chow,&nbsp;Jeffrey F. Cohn,&nbsp;Daniel S. Messinger","doi":"10.1111/infa.12556","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12556","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Although still-face effects are well-studied, little is known about the degree to which the Face-to-Face/Still-Face (FFSF) is associated with the production of intense affective displays. Duchenne smiling expresses more intense positive affect than non-Duchenne smiling, while Duchenne cry-faces express more intense negative affect than non-Duchenne cry-faces. Forty 4-month-old infants and their mothers completed the FFSF, and key affect-indexing facial Action Units (AUs) were coded by expert Facial Action Coding System coders for the first 30 s of each FFSF episode. Computer vision software, automated facial affect recognition (AFAR), identified AUs for the entire 2-min episodes. Expert coding and AFAR produced similar infant and mother Duchenne and non-Duchenne FFSF effects, highlighting the convergent validity of automated measurement. Substantive AFAR analyses indicated that both infant Duchenne and non-Duchenne smiling declined from the FF to the SF, but only Duchenne smiling increased from the SF to the RE. In similar fashion, the magnitude of mother Duchenne smiling changes over the FFSF were 2–4 times greater than non-Duchenne smiling changes. Duchenne expressions appear to be a sensitive index of intense infant and mother affective valence that are accessible to automated measurement and may be a target for future FFSF research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 5","pages":"910-929"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-07-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12556","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"10013948","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
An object's categorizability impacts whether infants encode surface features into their object representations 对象的可分类性影响婴儿是否将表面特征编码到他们的对象表征中
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-07-03 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12555
Melissa M. Kibbe, Aimee E. Stahl

Infants encode the surface features of simple, unfamiliar objects (e.g., red triangle) and the categorical identities of familiar, categorizable objects (e.g., car) into their representations of these objects. We asked whether 16–18-month-olds ignore non-diagnostic surface features (e.g., color) in favor of encoding an object's categorical identity (e.g., car) when objects are from familiar categories. In Experiment 1 (n = 18), we hid a categorizable object inside an opaque box. In No Switch trials, infants retrieved the object that was hidden. In Switch trials, infants retrieved a different object: an object from a different category (Between-Category-Switch trials) or a different object from the same category (Within-Category-Switch trials). We measured infants' subsequent searching in the box. Infants' pattern of searching suggested that only infants who completed a Within-Category-Switch trial as their first Switch trial encoded objects' surface features, and an exploratory analysis suggested that infants who completed a Between-Category-Switch trial as their first Switch trial only encoded objects' categories. In Experiment 2 (n = 18), we confirmed that these results were due to objects' categorizability. These results suggest infants may tailor the way they encode categorizable objects depending on which object dimensions are perceived to be task relevant.

婴儿将简单的、不熟悉的物体(如红色三角形)的表面特征和熟悉的、可分类的物体(如汽车)的分类特征编码到他们对这些物体的表征中。我们询问16 - 18个月大的婴儿,当物体来自熟悉的类别时,他们是否会忽略非诊断性的表面特征(如颜色),而倾向于编码物体的类别身份(如汽车)。在实验1 (n = 18)中,我们将一个可分类的对象隐藏在一个不透明的盒子中。在没有开关的实验中,婴儿找回了被隐藏的物体。在切换试验中,婴儿检索到不同的对象:来自不同类别的对象(类别间切换试验)或来自同一类别的不同对象(类别内切换试验)。我们测量了婴儿随后在盒子里的搜索。婴儿的搜索模式表明,只有第一次完成类别内切换试验的婴儿才会对物体的表面特征进行编码,探索性分析表明,第一次完成类别间切换试验的婴儿只会对物体的类别进行编码。在实验2 (n = 18)中,我们证实了这些结果是由于对象的可分类性。这些结果表明,婴儿可能会根据他们认为与任务相关的物体维度来调整他们对可分类物体的编码方式。
{"title":"An object's categorizability impacts whether infants encode surface features into their object representations","authors":"Melissa M. Kibbe,&nbsp;Aimee E. Stahl","doi":"10.1111/infa.12555","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12555","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Infants encode the surface features of simple, unfamiliar objects (e.g., red triangle) and the categorical identities of familiar, categorizable objects (e.g., car) into their representations of these objects. We asked whether 16–18-month-olds ignore non-diagnostic surface features (e.g., color) in favor of encoding an object's categorical identity (e.g., car) when objects are from familiar categories. In Experiment 1 (<i>n</i> = 18), we hid a categorizable object inside an opaque box. In No Switch trials, infants retrieved the object that was hidden. In Switch trials, infants retrieved a different object: an object from a different category (Between-Category-Switch trials) or a different object from the same category (Within-Category-Switch trials). We measured infants' subsequent searching in the box. Infants' pattern of searching suggested that only infants who completed a Within-Category-Switch trial as their first Switch trial encoded objects' surface features, and an exploratory analysis suggested that infants who completed a Between-Category-Switch trial as their first Switch trial only encoded objects' categories. In Experiment 2 (<i>n</i> = 18), we confirmed that these results were due to objects' categorizability. These results suggest infants may tailor the way they encode categorizable objects depending on which object dimensions are perceived to be task relevant.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 5","pages":"958-972"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-07-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9901951","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Assessing the language of 2 year-olds: From theory to practice 评估两岁儿童的语言能力:从理论到实践
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-06-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12554
Emily Jackson, Dani Levine, Jill de Villiers, Aquiles Iglesias, Kathy Hirsh-Pasek, Roberta Michnick Golinkoff

Early screening for language problems is a priority given the importance of language for success in school and interpersonal relationships. The paucity of reliable behavioral instruments for this age group prompted the development of a new touchscreen language screener for 2-year-olds that relies on language comprehension. Developmental literature guided selection of age-appropriate markers of language disorder risk that are culturally and dialectally neutral and could be reliably assessed. Items extend beyond products of linguistic knowledge (vocabulary and syntax) and tap the process by which children learn language, also known as fast mapping. After piloting an extensive set of items (139), two phases of testing with over 500 children aged 2; 0–2; 11 were conducted to choose the final 40-item set. Rasch analysis was used to select the best fitting and least redundant items. Norms were created based on 270 children. Sufficient test-retest reliability, Cronbach's alpha, and convergent validity with the MB-CDI and PPVT are reported. This quick behavioral measure of language capabilities could support research studies and facilitate the early detection of language problems.

考虑到语言对学业成功和人际关系的重要性,早期筛查语言问题是一个优先事项。这一年龄段缺乏可靠的行为工具,这促使人们开发了一种新的触摸屏语言筛检器,用于两岁儿童,这种筛检器依赖于语言理解。发展文献指导选择适合年龄的语言障碍风险标记,这些标记在文化和方言上是中立的,并且可以可靠地评估。项目超越了语言知识(词汇和句法)的产物,挖掘了儿童学习语言的过程,也被称为快速映射。在试点了一系列广泛的项目(139)之后,对500多名2岁儿童进行了两个阶段的测试;0 - 2;11人进行了问卷调查,以选出最终的40项问卷。采用Rasch分析法选择最佳拟合项和最少冗余项。规范是根据270名儿童制定的。据报道,MB-CDI和PPVT具有足够的重测信度、Cronbach's alpha和收敛效度。这种对语言能力的快速行为测量可以支持研究并促进语言问题的早期发现。
{"title":"Assessing the language of 2 year-olds: From theory to practice","authors":"Emily Jackson,&nbsp;Dani Levine,&nbsp;Jill de Villiers,&nbsp;Aquiles Iglesias,&nbsp;Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,&nbsp;Roberta Michnick Golinkoff","doi":"10.1111/infa.12554","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12554","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Early screening for language problems is a priority given the importance of language for success in school and interpersonal relationships. The paucity of reliable behavioral instruments for this age group prompted the development of a new touchscreen language screener for 2-year-olds that relies on language comprehension. Developmental literature guided selection of age-appropriate markers of language disorder risk that are culturally and dialectally neutral and could be reliably assessed. Items extend beyond products of linguistic knowledge (vocabulary and syntax) and tap the <i>process</i> by which children learn language, also known as fast mapping. After piloting an extensive set of items (139), two phases of testing with over 500 children aged 2; 0–2; 11 were conducted to choose the final 40-item set. Rasch analysis was used to select the best fitting and least redundant items. Norms were created based on 270 children. Sufficient test-retest reliability, Cronbach's alpha, and convergent validity with the MB-CDI and PPVT are reported. This quick behavioral measure of language capabilities could support research studies and facilitate the early detection of language problems.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 5","pages":"930-957"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9902209","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
Persistent symptoms of maternal post-traumatic stress following childbirth across the first months postpartum: Associations with perturbations in maternal behavior and infant avoidance of social gaze toward mother 产后头几个月产妇创伤后应激的持续症状:与产妇行为紊乱和婴儿回避对母亲的社会注视的关系
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-06-17 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12553
Sofie Rousseau, Tamar Feldman, Inbal Shlomi Polachek, Tahl I. Frenkel

Recent literature identifies childbirth as a potentially traumatic event, following which mothers may develop symptoms of Post-Traumatic-Stress-Following-Childbirth (PTS-FC). The current study examines whether stable symptoms of PTS-FC during the early postpartum period may impose risk for perturbations in maternal behavior and infant social-engagement with mother, controlling for comorbid postpartum internalizing symptoms. Mother-infant dyads (N = 192) were recruited from the general population, during the third trimester of pregnancy. 49.5% of the mothers were primipara, and 48.4% of the infants were girls. Maternal PTS-FC was assessed at 3-day, 1-month and 4-month postpartum, via self-report and clinician-administered interview. Latent Profile Analysis generated two profiles of symptomology: “Stable-High-PTS-FC” (17.0%), and “Stable-Low-PTS-FC” (83%). Membership in the “Stable-High-PTS-FC” profile associated with perturbed maternal sensitivity, which was in turn significantly associated with infant avoidance of social gaze toward mother (Indirect effect β = −0.15). Results suggest the need for early screening and inform the planning of early preventive interventions.

最近的文献表明,分娩是一种潜在的创伤性事件,随后母亲可能出现分娩后创伤应激(PTS-FC)的症状。目前的研究考察了产后早期PTS-FC的稳定症状是否会对母亲行为和婴儿与母亲的社会参与造成扰动的风险,并控制了产后共病的内化症状。在妊娠晚期从普通人群中招募了192对母子。初产妇占49.5%,女婴占48.4%。在产后3天、1个月和4个月,通过自我报告和临床访谈对产妇的PTS-FC进行评估。潜在特征分析产生了两种症状特征:“稳定-高pts - fc”(17.0%)和“稳定-低pts - fc”(83%)。“稳定-高pts - fc”特征与不安的母亲敏感性相关,这反过来又与婴儿回避对母亲的社交凝视显著相关(间接效应β = - 0.15)。结果提示需要进行早期筛查,并为早期预防干预计划提供信息。
{"title":"Persistent symptoms of maternal post-traumatic stress following childbirth across the first months postpartum: Associations with perturbations in maternal behavior and infant avoidance of social gaze toward mother","authors":"Sofie Rousseau,&nbsp;Tamar Feldman,&nbsp;Inbal Shlomi Polachek,&nbsp;Tahl I. Frenkel","doi":"10.1111/infa.12553","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12553","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent literature identifies childbirth as a potentially traumatic event, following which mothers may develop symptoms of Post-Traumatic-Stress-Following-Childbirth (PTS-FC). The current study examines whether stable symptoms of PTS-FC during the early postpartum period may impose risk for perturbations in maternal behavior and infant social-engagement with mother, controlling for comorbid postpartum internalizing symptoms. Mother-infant dyads (<i>N</i> = 192) were recruited from the general population, during the third trimester of pregnancy. 49.5% of the mothers were primipara, and 48.4% of the infants were girls. Maternal PTS-FC was assessed at 3-day, 1-month and 4-month postpartum, via self-report and clinician-administered interview. Latent Profile Analysis generated two profiles of symptomology: “Stable-High-PTS-FC” (17.0%), and “Stable-Low-PTS-FC” (83%). Membership in the “Stable-High-PTS-FC” profile associated with perturbed maternal sensitivity, which was in turn significantly associated with infant avoidance of social gaze toward mother (Indirect effect <i>β</i> = −0.15). Results suggest the need for early screening and inform the planning of early preventive interventions.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 5","pages":"882-909"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-06-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12553","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9901452","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early psychosocial risk factors and postnatal parental reflective functioning 早期心理社会风险因素和产后父母的反思功能。
IF 2.6 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-05-27 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12552
Philippa Arkle, Fionnuala Larkin, Ying Wang, Yujin Lee, Amy Fernandez, Lydia Y. Li, Elizabeth Meins

Psychosocial factors have been found to relate to parental reflective functioning (PRF), a parent's ability to mentalize about themselves and their child. Relations between maternal psychosocial risk factors and PRF were investigated in a community sample. A sample of mothers (n = 146) was assessed for risk factors when infants were 6 months, infant temperament was assessed using an observational measure, and PRF was assessed with the Parent Development Interview-Revised (PDI). PRF was measured again with the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) when children were 4 years (n = 105) and 5 years (n = 92), with an additional sample of mothers (n = 48) tested at these two timepoints. Results showed that in infancy, total maternal psychosocial risk related to lower PDI-PRF; regression analyses highlighted low socioeconomic status, unplanned pregnancy, and low maternal anxiety as independent predictors of lower PDI-PRF. PDI-PRF scores at 6 months did not relate to PRFQ scores, but PRFQ subscales showed stability over time from age 4–5. Results are discussed with regard to the impact of maternal psychosocial risk and infant temperament on PRF and the stability and concordance of PRF measures.

心理社会因素已被发现与父母的反思功能(PRF)有关,即父母对自己和孩子的心理化能力。在一个社区样本中调查了母亲心理社会危险因素与PRF之间的关系。在婴儿6个月大时,对母亲样本(n=146)进行风险因素评估,使用观察性测量评估婴儿气质,并使用修订的父母发展访谈(PDI)评估PRF。在儿童4岁(n=105)和5岁(n=92)时,用父母反思功能问卷(PRFQ)再次测量PRF,并在这两个时间点对母亲(n=48)的额外样本进行测试。结果表明,在婴儿期,母亲的总心理社会风险与PDI-PRF较低有关;回归分析强调,低社会经济地位、计划外怀孕和低母亲焦虑是PDI-PRF较低的独立预测因素。6个月时的PDI-PRF评分与PRFQ评分无关,但从4-5岁开始,PRFQ分量表显示出随时间的稳定性。讨论了母亲心理社会风险和婴儿气质对PRF的影响以及PRF测量的稳定性和一致性。
{"title":"Early psychosocial risk factors and postnatal parental reflective functioning","authors":"Philippa Arkle,&nbsp;Fionnuala Larkin,&nbsp;Ying Wang,&nbsp;Yujin Lee,&nbsp;Amy Fernandez,&nbsp;Lydia Y. Li,&nbsp;Elizabeth Meins","doi":"10.1111/infa.12552","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12552","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Psychosocial factors have been found to relate to parental reflective functioning (PRF), a parent's ability to mentalize about themselves and their child. Relations between maternal psychosocial risk factors and PRF were investigated in a community sample. A sample of mothers (<i>n</i> = 146) was assessed for risk factors when infants were 6 months, infant temperament was assessed using an observational measure, and PRF was assessed with the Parent Development Interview-Revised (PDI). PRF was measured again with the Parental Reflective Functioning Questionnaire (PRFQ) when children were 4 years (<i>n</i> = 105) and 5 years (<i>n</i> = 92), with an additional sample of mothers (<i>n</i> = 48) tested at these two timepoints. Results showed that in infancy, total maternal psychosocial risk related to lower PDI-PRF; regression analyses highlighted low socioeconomic status, unplanned pregnancy, and low maternal anxiety as independent predictors of lower PDI-PRF. PDI-PRF scores at 6 months did not relate to PRFQ scores, but PRFQ subscales showed stability over time from age 4–5. Results are discussed with regard to the impact of maternal psychosocial risk and infant temperament on PRF and the stability and concordance of PRF measures.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"28 5","pages":"864-881"},"PeriodicalIF":2.6,"publicationDate":"2023-05-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/infa.12552","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9898167","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 1
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1