首页 > 最新文献

Infancy最新文献

英文 中文
Exploring Developmental Connections: Sleep Patterns, Self-Locomotion, and Vocabulary Growth in Early Childhood
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-24 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12650
Margherita Belia, Marilyn Vihman, Tamar Keren-Portnoy

Current research indicates likely developmental connections between the evolution of sleep patterns, motor skills progression, and the expansion of vocabulary. These connections are grounded in the well-established role of sleep in memory and learning, as well as in the cascading effects on language development of the acquisition of new motor skills. However, no study has so far undertaken a comprehensive and systematic examination of these connections or explored their developmental trajectory over time. Yet understanding vocabulary development depends on considering development in the sleep regulation and motor domains, to provide a biologically grounded explanation of how early lexicons are built and strengthened. This study investigates the links between vocabulary growth and two significant changes occurring over the first 2 years of life: self-initiated locomotion and the consolidation of overnight sleep. Our results reveal mutual associations between these domains, which tend to emerge during periods of marked developmental change in language, motor skills, and sleep patterns regulation. Moreover, these associations were observed to change over time, suggesting dynamic interconnections between these developmental domains. Our findings point toward the importance of investigating vocabulary development from a dynamic systems perspective, as the product of continuous interactions between cognition, the body and the environment.

{"title":"Exploring Developmental Connections: Sleep Patterns, Self-Locomotion, and Vocabulary Growth in Early Childhood","authors":"Margherita Belia,&nbsp;Marilyn Vihman,&nbsp;Tamar Keren-Portnoy","doi":"10.1111/infa.12650","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12650","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Current research indicates likely developmental connections between the evolution of sleep patterns, motor skills progression, and the expansion of vocabulary. These connections are grounded in the well-established role of sleep in memory and learning, as well as in the cascading effects on language development of the acquisition of new motor skills. However, no study has so far undertaken a comprehensive and systematic examination of these connections or explored their developmental trajectory over time. Yet understanding vocabulary development depends on considering development in the sleep regulation and motor domains, to provide a biologically grounded explanation of how early lexicons are built and strengthened. This study investigates the links between vocabulary growth and two significant changes occurring over the first 2 years of life: self-initiated locomotion and the consolidation of overnight sleep. Our results reveal mutual associations between these domains, which tend to emerge during periods of marked developmental change in language, motor skills, and sleep patterns regulation. Moreover, these associations were observed to change over time, suggesting dynamic interconnections between these developmental domains. Our findings point toward the importance of investigating vocabulary development from a dynamic systems perspective, as the product of continuous interactions between cognition, the body and the environment.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11760629/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143034352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Maternal Emotional Availability Supports Child Communicative Development Regardless of Child Temperament—Findings From the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-24 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12649
Denise Ollas-Skogster, Riikka Korja, Akie Yada, Elina Mainela-Arnold, Hasse Karlsson, David J. Bridgett, Pirkko Rautakoski, Linnea Karlsson, Saara Nolvi

The interplay of emotional availability (EA) and child temperament in association with early language development is understudied. We explored associations between maternal EA and infant communicative development and possible moderations by child temperament. Participants were 151 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study. Path models of associations between 8-month maternal EA and 14-month communicative development and moderation by infant temperament traits were created using SEM. Results show that EA positively predicted a latent variable of communicative development at 14 months. No direct longitudinal effect of EA on 30-month vocabulary was found. Child surgency/extraversion at 6 and 12 months significantly predicted 14-month communicative skills. Temperament did not moderate the association between EA and communicative development. Findings underscore the additive role of maternal caregiving and early surgency/extraversion in predicting early communicative development. The emotional aspects of parenting should be acknowledged as contributors to early communicative development in future studies.

{"title":"Maternal Emotional Availability Supports Child Communicative Development Regardless of Child Temperament—Findings From the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study","authors":"Denise Ollas-Skogster,&nbsp;Riikka Korja,&nbsp;Akie Yada,&nbsp;Elina Mainela-Arnold,&nbsp;Hasse Karlsson,&nbsp;David J. Bridgett,&nbsp;Pirkko Rautakoski,&nbsp;Linnea Karlsson,&nbsp;Saara Nolvi","doi":"10.1111/infa.12649","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12649","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The interplay of emotional availability (EA) and child temperament in association with early language development is understudied. We explored associations between maternal EA and infant communicative development and possible moderations by child temperament. Participants were 151 mother-child dyads from the FinnBrain Birth Cohort Study. Path models of associations between 8-month maternal EA and 14-month communicative development and moderation by infant temperament traits were created using SEM. Results show that EA positively predicted a latent variable of communicative development at 14 months. No direct longitudinal effect of EA on 30-month vocabulary was found. Child surgency/extraversion at 6 and 12 months significantly predicted 14-month communicative skills. Temperament did not moderate the association between EA and communicative development. Findings underscore the additive role of maternal caregiving and early surgency/extraversion in predicting early communicative development. The emotional aspects of parenting should be acknowledged as contributors to early communicative development in future studies.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11758768/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143034353","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Infant and Toddler Curiosity Questionnaire: A Validated Caregiver-Report Measure of Curiosity in Children From 5 to 24 Months
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.70001
Elena C. Altmann, Marina Bazhydai, Didar Karadağ, Gert Westermann

Humans are curious. Especially children are known for their drive to explore and learn, which is crucial for developing in and navigating through our complex world. Naturally, some children may be more curious than others, leading to differences in how they structure their own learning experiences, subsequently impacting their developmental trajectories. However, there is a gap in the research field for a reliable measure of such differences early in development. Across three studies, we present the development and assessment of the Infant and Toddler Curiosity Questionnaire (ITCQ), the first caregiver report measure to fill this gap. Items cover observable exploration behaviors in 5- to 24-month-olds to capture general tendencies of their desire to actively explore their immediate surroundings and are evaluated on a 7-point Likert-scale. Exploratory factor analyses and structural equation modeling on a sample of N = 370 UK caregivers led to the final selection of 23 items and provided evidence that the scale allows the reliable computation of an overall curiosity score, with three emergent subscales (Sensory, Investigative, and Interactive) explaining additional variance in the data. Furthermore, the scale had good test-retest reliability after 7–14 days (N = 67) and related to the child's temperament (N = 75; positively with surgency and effortful control, negatively with negative affect) offering evidence of its validity as a trait measure. Together, these results support the scale's reliability and validity, showcasing the ITCQ as a powerful tool for developmental research.

{"title":"The Infant and Toddler Curiosity Questionnaire: A Validated Caregiver-Report Measure of Curiosity in Children From 5 to 24 Months","authors":"Elena C. Altmann,&nbsp;Marina Bazhydai,&nbsp;Didar Karadağ,&nbsp;Gert Westermann","doi":"10.1111/infa.70001","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.70001","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Humans are curious. Especially children are known for their drive to explore and learn, which is crucial for developing in and navigating through our complex world. Naturally, some children may be more curious than others, leading to differences in how they structure their own learning experiences, subsequently impacting their developmental trajectories. However, there is a gap in the research field for a reliable measure of such differences early in development. Across three studies, we present the development and assessment of the Infant and Toddler Curiosity Questionnaire (ITCQ), the first caregiver report measure to fill this gap. Items cover observable exploration behaviors in 5- to 24-month-olds to capture general tendencies of their desire to actively explore their immediate surroundings and are evaluated on a 7-point Likert-scale. Exploratory factor analyses and structural equation modeling on a sample of <i>N</i> = 370 UK caregivers led to the final selection of 23 items and provided evidence that the scale allows the reliable computation of an overall curiosity score, with three emergent subscales (<i>Sensory, Investigative</i>, and <i>Interactive</i>) explaining additional variance in the data. Furthermore, the scale had good test-retest reliability after 7–14 days (<i>N</i> = 67) and related to the child's temperament (<i>N</i> = 75; positively with surgency and effortful control, negatively with negative affect) offering evidence of its validity as a trait measure. Together, these results support the scale's reliability and validity, showcasing the ITCQ as a powerful tool for developmental research.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11758190/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143034354","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Emotional Movement Kinematics Guide Twelve-Month-Olds’ Visual, but Not Manual, Exploration 情感运动运动学指导12个月大婴儿的视觉探索,而不是手动探索。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-22 DOI: 10.1111/infa.70000
Joanna M. Rutkowska, Julia Mermier, Marlene Meyer, Hermann Bulf, Chiara Turati, Sabine Hunnius

The ability to recognize and act on others' emotions is crucial for navigating social interactions successfully and learning about the world. One way in which others' emotions are observable is through their movement kinematics. Movement information is available even at a distance or when an individual's face is not visible. Infants have been shown to be sensitive to emotions in movement kinematics of transporting actions, like moving an object from one to another place. However, it is still unknown whether they associate the manipulated object with the emotions contained in moving it, and whether they use this information to guide their own exploration of this object. In this study, 12-month-old infants watched actors transporting two toys with positive or negative emotional valence. Then, infants were given the possibility to interact with the same toys. We expected the infants to look at and touch the toy handled in a positive manner more, compared to the toy handled in a negative manner. Our results showed that infants looked at the positive toys more than at the negative toys, but that infants touched both toys for the same amount of time. Also, there was no difference in which toy they manually explored first.

识别他人情绪并对其采取行动的能力对于成功驾驭社交互动和了解世界至关重要。观察他人情绪的一种方式是通过他们的运动运动学。即使在远处或看不到个人的脸时,也可以获得运动信息。婴儿在搬运动作的运动运动学中对情绪很敏感,比如把一个物体从一个地方移到另一个地方。然而,尚不清楚他们是否将被操纵的物体与移动物体时所包含的情绪联系起来,以及他们是否使用这些信息来指导自己对该物体的探索。在这项研究中,12个月大的婴儿观看演员搬运两个具有积极或消极情绪效价的玩具。然后,婴儿有可能与同样的玩具互动。我们期望婴儿更多地观看和触摸以积极方式处理的玩具,而不是以消极方式处理的玩具。我们的研究结果表明,婴儿看积极玩具的时间比看消极玩具的时间要长,但婴儿触摸这两种玩具的时间是一样的。此外,他们首先手动探索哪个玩具也没有区别。
{"title":"Emotional Movement Kinematics Guide Twelve-Month-Olds’ Visual, but Not Manual, Exploration","authors":"Joanna M. Rutkowska,&nbsp;Julia Mermier,&nbsp;Marlene Meyer,&nbsp;Hermann Bulf,&nbsp;Chiara Turati,&nbsp;Sabine Hunnius","doi":"10.1111/infa.70000","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.70000","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The ability to recognize and act on others' emotions is crucial for navigating social interactions successfully and learning about the world. One way in which others' emotions are observable is through their movement kinematics. Movement information is available even at a distance or when an individual's face is not visible. Infants have been shown to be sensitive to emotions in movement kinematics of transporting actions, like moving an object from one to another place. However, it is still unknown whether they associate the manipulated object with the emotions contained in moving it, and whether they use this information to guide their own exploration of this object. In this study, 12-month-old infants watched actors transporting two toys with positive or negative emotional valence. Then, infants were given the possibility to interact with the same toys. We expected the infants to look at and touch the toy handled in a positive manner more, compared to the toy handled in a negative manner. Our results showed that infants looked at the positive toys more than at the negative toys, but that infants touched both toys for the same amount of time. Also, there was no difference in which toy they manually explored first.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11753196/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014097","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cultural Differences in Visual Attention Emerge in Infancy 幼儿时期视觉注意的文化差异。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-18 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12651
Megan J. Heise, Marek Meristo, Mika Ueno, Shoji Itakura, Stephanie M. Carlson

East Asians are more likely than North Americans to attend to visual scenes holistically, focusing on the relations between objects and their background rather than isolating components. This cultural difference in context sensitivity—greater attentional allocation to the background of an image or scene—has been attributed to socialization, yet it is unknown how early in development it appears, and whether it is moderated by social information. We employed eye-tracking to investigate context-sensitivity in 15-month-olds in Japan (n = 45) and the United States (n = 52). Viewing faces, Japanese infants were more attentive and studied the background longer than U.S. infants. Viewing cartoon videos, Japanese infants looked at the background twice as long as U.S. infants, particularly for objects with eyes. In parent-child book reading, Japanese parents referred to the background significantly more than U.S. parents, although this was uncorrelated with infant behavior on the preceding tasks. These findings illustrated that cultural differences in attention are detectable in infancy, and that sustained attention may be an important foundation upon which culturally-specific attentional styles are built. Overall, results were consistent with the view that a context-sensitive orientation first emerges for social information and later generalizes to non-social situations.

东亚人比北美人更有可能从整体上关注视觉场景,关注物体与背景之间的关系,而不是孤立的组成部分。这种背景敏感性的文化差异——更多的注意力分配到图像或场景的背景上——被归因于社会化,但尚不清楚它在发展的早期出现,以及它是否受到社会信息的调节。我们采用眼动追踪研究了日本(n = 45)和美国(n = 52) 15个月大的婴儿的语境敏感性。在观察人脸时,日本婴儿比美国婴儿更专注,研究背景的时间也更长。在观看卡通视频时,日本婴儿看背景的时间是美国婴儿的两倍,尤其是有眼睛的物体。在亲子阅读中,日本父母提到背景的次数明显多于美国父母,尽管这与婴儿在上述任务中的行为无关。这些发现表明,注意的文化差异在婴儿期就可以检测到,持续的注意可能是建立文化特定注意风格的重要基础。总的来说,结果与情境敏感取向首先出现在社会信息中,然后推广到非社会情境的观点一致。
{"title":"Cultural Differences in Visual Attention Emerge in Infancy","authors":"Megan J. Heise,&nbsp;Marek Meristo,&nbsp;Mika Ueno,&nbsp;Shoji Itakura,&nbsp;Stephanie M. Carlson","doi":"10.1111/infa.12651","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12651","url":null,"abstract":"<p>East Asians are more likely than North Americans to attend to visual scenes holistically, focusing on the relations between objects and their background rather than isolating components. This cultural difference in context sensitivity—greater attentional allocation to the background of an image or scene—has been attributed to socialization, yet it is unknown how early in development it appears, and whether it is moderated by social information. We employed eye-tracking to investigate context-sensitivity in 15-month-olds in Japan (<i>n =</i> 45) and the United States (<i>n =</i> 52). Viewing faces, Japanese infants were more attentive and studied the background longer than U.S. infants. Viewing cartoon videos, Japanese infants looked at the background twice as long as U.S. infants, particularly for objects with eyes. In parent-child book reading, Japanese parents referred to the background significantly more than U.S. parents, although this was uncorrelated with infant behavior on the preceding tasks. These findings illustrated that cultural differences in attention are detectable in infancy, and that sustained attention may be an important foundation upon which culturally-specific attentional styles are built. Overall, results were consistent with the view that a context-sensitive orientation first emerges for social information and later generalizes to non-social situations.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11742511/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"143014091","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Active Infant's Developing Role in Musical Interactions: Insights From an Online Parent Questionnaire 活跃婴儿在音乐互动中的发展角色:来自在线父母问卷的见解。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-08 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12648
Angela Dou, Laura K. Cirelli

Musical interactions between caregivers and their infants typically rely on a limited repertoire of live vocal songs and recorded music. Research suggests that these well-known songs are especially effective at eliciting engaged behaviors from infants in controlled settings, but how infants respond to familiar music with their caregivers in their everyday environment remains unclear. The current study used an online questionnaire to quantify how often and why caregivers present certain songs and musical recordings to their infants. Using a cross-sectional approach, we explored infants' changing behavioral profiles to music from birth to 24 months. Caregivers additionally reported on their feelings of affective attachment toward their infants. Results reveal that caregivers sing and play recorded music for younger and older infants at comparably high rates. In turn, infants actively respond to their favorite songs and recordings by demonstrating positive emotions, movements, and attentive listening. Caregivers mainly consider their infants' musical preferences when building their shared musical repertoire at home. Both caregivers' engagement in musical activities with their children and infants' enthusiastic responsiveness to singing predicted stronger dyadic attachment bonding. Caregivers and infants jointly contribute to building musical relationships, and these musical relationships may be intertwined with their emerging social-emotional bonds.

看护人和婴儿之间的音乐互动通常依赖于有限的现场演唱曲目和录制的音乐。研究表明,在受控环境下,这些著名的歌曲在激发婴儿参与行为方面特别有效,但婴儿在日常环境中如何与照顾者一起对熟悉的音乐做出反应尚不清楚。目前的研究使用了一份在线问卷来量化护理人员向婴儿播放某些歌曲和音乐唱片的频率和原因。采用横断面方法,我们探讨了婴儿从出生到24个月对音乐的行为特征的变化。照顾者还报告了他们对婴儿的情感依恋。结果显示,照顾者为年幼和年长的婴儿唱歌和播放录制好的音乐的比例相当高。反过来,婴儿通过表现出积极的情绪、动作和专注的倾听来积极地回应他们最喜欢的歌曲和录音。照顾者在家里建立共同的音乐曲目时主要考虑婴儿的音乐偏好。照顾者与孩子一起参与音乐活动和婴儿对唱歌的热情反应都预示着更强的二元依恋关系。照顾者和婴儿共同有助于建立音乐关系,这些音乐关系可能与他们新兴的社会情感纽带交织在一起。
{"title":"The Active Infant's Developing Role in Musical Interactions: Insights From an Online Parent Questionnaire","authors":"Angela Dou,&nbsp;Laura K. Cirelli","doi":"10.1111/infa.12648","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12648","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Musical interactions between caregivers and their infants typically rely on a limited repertoire of live vocal songs and recorded music. Research suggests that these well-known songs are especially effective at eliciting engaged behaviors from infants in controlled settings, but how infants respond to familiar music with their caregivers in their everyday environment remains unclear. The current study used an online questionnaire to quantify how often and why caregivers present certain songs and musical recordings to their infants. Using a cross-sectional approach, we explored infants' changing behavioral profiles to music from birth to 24 months. Caregivers additionally reported on their feelings of affective attachment toward their infants. Results reveal that caregivers sing and play recorded music for younger and older infants at comparably high rates. In turn, infants actively respond to their favorite songs and recordings by demonstrating positive emotions, movements, and attentive listening. Caregivers mainly consider their infants' musical preferences when building their shared musical repertoire at home. Both caregivers' engagement in musical activities with their children and infants' enthusiastic responsiveness to singing predicted stronger dyadic attachment bonding. Caregivers and infants jointly contribute to building musical relationships, and these musical relationships may be intertwined with their emerging social-emotional bonds.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2025-01-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11711306/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142956877","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Association of Spousal Relationship Quality and Social Support With Maternal-Infant Bonding: Moderating Roles of Maternal Age and Paternal Occupation 配偶关系质量、社会支持对母婴关系的影响:母亲年龄和父亲职业的调节作用。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-30 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12647
Kirsten F. Siebach, Soim Park, Maryam Mansoor, Najia Atif, Ahmed Zaidi, Atif Rahman, Abid Malik, Pamela J. Surkan

This study examined associations between spousal relationship quality and social support with mother-infant bonding among women in Rawalpindi, Pakistan (Intervention Arm: n = 352, Mage = 25.1, SD = 4.7; Control Arm: n = 358, Mage = 25.3, SD = 4.5). We used cross-sectional data from the Happy Mother-Healthy Baby intervention study, a randomized controlled trial conducted between 2019 and 2022. Women were enrolled at their first prenatal visit (at or before 22 weeks of gestation) and followed until 6-week postpartum. Spousal relationship quality, social support, and bonding were assessed at 6-week postpartum. Linear regression analyses found relationship quality (b = 3.85) and social support (b = 1.99) were positively associated with bonding. Analyses were adjusted for mother's education, treatment group, exposure to perinatal intimate partner violence, postpartum depressive symptoms, husband's education, husband's occupation, infant birthweight, and preterm birth. Husband's occupation (unskilled/unemployed vs. professional/skilled) significantly moderated the relationship between social support (b = 3.27 vs. b = 1.37) and relationship quality (b = 5.36 vs. b = 2.95) with bonding. Maternal age (≤ 25 years old vs. > 25 years old) significantly moderated the association between relationship quality and bonding (b = 4.96 vs. b = 2.49). Results suggest that interventions focused on improving social support and relationship quality among anxious women and their spouses could improve maternal-infant bonding. Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03880032; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03880032

本研究考察了巴基斯坦拉瓦尔品第妇女的配偶关系质量和社会支持与母婴关系之间的关系(干预组:n = 352, Mage = 25.1, SD = 4.7;控制臂:n = 358, Mage = 25.3, SD = 4.5)。我们使用了快乐母亲-健康婴儿干预研究的横断面数据,这是一项在2019年至2022年期间进行的随机对照试验。妇女在她们的第一次产前检查(怀孕22周或之前)登记,并一直随访到产后6周。在产后6周对配偶关系质量、社会支持和结合进行评估。线性回归分析发现,关系质量(b = 3.85)和社会支持(b = 1.99)与亲密关系呈正相关。分析调整了母亲受教育程度、治疗组、围产期亲密伴侣暴力暴露、产后抑郁症状、丈夫受教育程度、丈夫职业、婴儿出生体重和早产情况。丈夫的职业(非技术/失业vs专业/技术)显著调节了社会支持(b = 3.27 vs. b = 1.37)和关系质量(b = 5.36 vs. b = 2.95)与关系的关系。母亲年龄(≤25岁对≤25岁)显著调节关系质量与亲密关系的相关性(b = 4.96对b = 2.49)。结果表明,干预措施的重点是提高社会支持和焦虑妇女及其配偶之间的关系质量,可以改善母婴关系。试验注册:ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03880032;https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03880032。
{"title":"The Association of Spousal Relationship Quality and Social Support With Maternal-Infant Bonding: Moderating Roles of Maternal Age and Paternal Occupation","authors":"Kirsten F. Siebach,&nbsp;Soim Park,&nbsp;Maryam Mansoor,&nbsp;Najia Atif,&nbsp;Ahmed Zaidi,&nbsp;Atif Rahman,&nbsp;Abid Malik,&nbsp;Pamela J. Surkan","doi":"10.1111/infa.12647","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12647","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>This study examined associations between spousal relationship quality and social support with mother-infant bonding among women in Rawalpindi, Pakistan (Intervention Arm: <i>n</i> = 352, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 25.1, SD = 4.7; Control Arm: <i>n</i> = 358, <i>M</i><sub>age</sub> <i>=</i> 25.3, SD = 4.5). We used cross-sectional data from the <i>Happy Mother-Healthy Baby</i> intervention study, a randomized controlled trial conducted between 2019 and 2022. Women were enrolled at their first prenatal visit (at or before 22 weeks of gestation) and followed until 6-week postpartum. Spousal relationship quality, social support, and bonding were assessed at 6-week postpartum. Linear regression analyses found relationship quality (<i>b</i> = 3.85) and social support (<i>b</i> = 1.99) were positively associated with bonding. Analyses were adjusted for mother's education, treatment group, exposure to perinatal intimate partner violence, postpartum depressive symptoms, husband's education, husband's occupation, infant birthweight, and preterm birth. Husband's occupation (unskilled/unemployed vs. professional/skilled) significantly moderated the relationship between social support (<i>b</i> = 3.27 vs. <i>b</i> = 1.37) and relationship quality (<i>b</i> = 5.36 vs. <i>b</i> = 2.95) with bonding. Maternal age (≤ 25 years old vs. &gt; 25 years old) significantly moderated the association between relationship quality and bonding (<i>b</i> = 4.96 vs. <i>b</i> = 2.49). Results suggest that interventions focused on improving social support and relationship quality among anxious women and their spouses could improve maternal-infant bonding. <b>Trial Registration:</b> ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03880032; https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT03880032</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142910839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Dynamics of Looking and Smiling Differ for Young Infants at Elevated Likelihood for ASD 自闭症可能性高的婴儿看和笑的动态不同。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-23 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12646
Julia Yurkovic-Harding, Jessica Bradshaw

Social smiling is the earliest gained social communication skill, emerging around 2 months of age. From 2 to 6-months, infants primarily smile in response to caregivers. After 6 months, infants coordinate social smiles with other social cues to initiate interactions with the caregiver. Social smiling is reduced in older infants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) but has rarely been studied before 6 months of life. The current study therefore aimed to understand the component parts of infant social smiles, namely look to caregiver and smile, during face-to-face interactions in 3 and 4-month-old infants at elevated (EL) and low likelihood (LL) for ASD. We found that EL and LL infants looked to their caregiver and smiled for similar amounts of time and at similar rates, suggesting that social smiling manifests similarly in both groups. A nuanced difference between groups emerged when considering temporal dynamics of looking and smiling. Specifically, 3-month-old EL infants demonstrated extended looking to the caregiver after smile offset. These findings suggest that social smiling is largely typical in EL infants in early infancy, with subtle differences in temporal coupling. Future research is needed to understand the full magnitude of these differences and their implications for social development.

社交微笑是最早获得的社交沟通技能,大约在2个月大时出现。从2个月到6个月,婴儿主要用微笑回应照顾者。6个月后,婴儿将社交微笑与其他社交线索协调起来,开始与照顾者互动。患有自闭症谱系障碍(ASD)的大婴儿的社交微笑会减少,但在6个月前很少有研究。因此,目前的研究旨在了解婴儿社交微笑的组成部分,即看着照顾者和微笑,在3和4个月大的婴儿在面对面的互动中,高(EL)和低可能性(LL)的ASD。我们发现,EL和LL婴儿看着他们的照顾者,微笑的时间和频率相似,这表明社交微笑在两组中表现得相似。在考虑眼神和微笑的时间动态时,两组之间出现了细微的差异。具体来说,3个月大的EL婴儿在微笑抵消后表现出对照顾者的延伸注视。这些发现表明,社交性微笑在婴儿期早期的EL婴儿中非常典型,在时间耦合方面存在细微差异。今后需要进行研究,以了解这些差异的全部规模及其对社会发展的影响。
{"title":"The Dynamics of Looking and Smiling Differ for Young Infants at Elevated Likelihood for ASD","authors":"Julia Yurkovic-Harding,&nbsp;Jessica Bradshaw","doi":"10.1111/infa.12646","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12646","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Social smiling is the earliest gained social communication skill, emerging around 2 months of age. From 2 to 6-months, infants primarily smile in response to caregivers. After 6 months, infants coordinate social smiles with other social cues to initiate interactions with the caregiver. Social smiling is reduced in older infants with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) but has rarely been studied before 6 months of life. The current study therefore aimed to understand the component parts of infant social smiles, namely look to caregiver and smile, during face-to-face interactions in 3 and 4-month-old infants at elevated (EL) and low likelihood (LL) for ASD. We found that EL and LL infants looked to their caregiver and smiled for similar amounts of time and at similar rates, suggesting that social smiling manifests similarly in both groups. A nuanced difference between groups emerged when considering temporal dynamics of looking and smiling. Specifically, 3-month-old EL infants demonstrated extended looking to the caregiver after smile offset. These findings suggest that social smiling is largely typical in EL infants in early infancy, with subtle differences in temporal coupling. Future research is needed to understand the full magnitude of these differences and their implications for social development.</p>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142883189","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Forms and Functions of Gestures in Preverbal 12- to 15-Months Old Infants 语言前12 ~ 15个月婴儿手势的形式和功能
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-10 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12645
Shreejata Gupta, Eulalie Pequay, Clément François, Isabelle Dautriche

Speech and co-speech gestures always go hand in hand. Whether we find the precursors of these co-speech gestures in infants before they master their native language still remains an open question. Except for deictic gestures, there is little agreement on the existence of iconic, non-referential and conventional gestures before children start producing their first words. Here, we bridge this knowledge gap by leveraging an ethological method already established for describing speech independent gestures in nonhuman primates, to analyze the spontaneous gestures produced by infants when interacting with their caregivers. We manually annotated video recordings of infant-caregiver interactions (26 h) from the CHILDES platform, to describe the gesture forms, types and functions in six infants from 12 to 15 months of age. We describe 62 gesture forms in the preverbal repertoire. These were categorized into deictic, iconic, non-referential and conventional gesture types, similar to co-speech gesture types. We also find that the type-function relation of preverbal gestures map similarly to type-meaning relation of co-speech gestures. Taken together, our results illustrate linguistic properties of infant gestures in the absence of speech, suggesting them to be precursors of co-speech gestures.

语音和辅助语音手势总是相辅相成的。我们是否在婴儿掌握母语之前就发现了这些共同语言手势的先兆,仍然是一个悬而未决的问题。除了指示手势外,在孩子开始说第一个单词之前,人们对是否存在符号手势、非指涉手势和传统手势的看法并不一致。在这里,我们利用已经建立的行为学方法来描述非人类灵长类动物的独立语言手势,来分析婴儿在与照顾者互动时产生的自发手势,从而弥合这一知识差距。我们对来自CHILDES平台的婴儿-看护者互动视频(26小时)进行了手工注释,以描述6名12至15个月大的婴儿的手势形式、类型和功能。我们描述了62手势形式在言语前的剧目。这些手势被分为指示手势、标志手势、非指涉手势和传统手势类型,类似于同语手势类型。我们还发现言语前手势的类型-功能关系与同语手势的类型-意义关系相似。综上所述,我们的研究结果说明了在没有语言的情况下婴儿手势的语言特性,表明它们是共同语言手势的先驱。
{"title":"Forms and Functions of Gestures in Preverbal 12- to 15-Months Old Infants","authors":"Shreejata Gupta,&nbsp;Eulalie Pequay,&nbsp;Clément François,&nbsp;Isabelle Dautriche","doi":"10.1111/infa.12645","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12645","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Speech and co-speech gestures always go hand in hand. Whether we find the precursors of these co-speech gestures in infants before they master their native language still remains an open question. Except for deictic gestures, there is little agreement on the existence of iconic, non-referential and conventional gestures before children start producing their first words. Here, we bridge this knowledge gap by leveraging an ethological method already established for describing speech independent gestures in nonhuman primates, to analyze the spontaneous gestures produced by infants when interacting with their caregivers. We manually annotated video recordings of infant-caregiver interactions (26 h) from the <i>CHILDES platform</i>, to describe the gesture forms, types and functions in six infants from 12 to 15 months of age. We describe 62 gesture forms in the preverbal repertoire. These were categorized into deictic, iconic, non-referential and conventional gesture types, similar to co-speech gesture types. We also find that the type-function relation of preverbal gestures map similarly to type-meaning relation of co-speech gestures. Taken together, our results illustrate linguistic properties of infant gestures in the absence of speech, suggesting them to be precursors of co-speech gestures.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11631824/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142807996","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infants' Knowledge of Individual Words: Investigating Links Between Parent Report and Looking Time 婴儿单字知识:父母报告与注视时间的关系研究。
IF 2 2区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-05 DOI: 10.1111/infa.12641
Melanie López Pérez, Charlotte Moore, Andrea Sander-Montant, Krista Byers-Heinlein

Assessing early vocabulary development commonly involves parent report methods and behavioral tasks like looking-while-listening. While both yield reliable aggregate scores, findings are mixed regarding their reliability in measuring infants' knowledge of individual words. Using archival data from 126 monolingual and bilingual 14–31-month-olds, we further examined links across these methods at the word level, while controlling for potentially confounding child-level factors. When data were averaged at the child level, performance on the looking-while-listening task correlated well with parent-reported word production of the same words, as expected. However, mixed-effects model comparisons suggested that at the word level, looking-while-listening performance was significantly predicted by age and total productive vocabulary, but not by parent-reported knowledge of a word once these factors were controlled for. These findings invite careful consideration regarding the adequacy of these two popular methods for capturing children's idiosyncratic knowledge of individual words.

评估早期词汇发展通常包括家长报告方法和像边听边看这样的行为任务。虽然这两种方法都能得出可靠的综合分数,但在测量婴儿对单个单词知识的可靠性方面,结果却不尽相同。利用126名14-31个月大的单语和双语婴儿的档案数据,我们进一步研究了这些方法在单词水平上的联系,同时控制了潜在的混淆儿童水平的因素。当数据在儿童水平上平均时,正如预期的那样,在“边听边看”任务中的表现与父母报告的相同单词的单词生成量密切相关。然而,混合效应模型的比较表明,在单词水平上,“边听边看”的表现与年龄和总有效词汇量显著相关,但一旦这些因素得到控制,则与父母报告的单词知识无关。这些发现需要仔细考虑这两种流行的方法是否足够,以捕捉儿童对单个单词的特殊知识。
{"title":"Infants' Knowledge of Individual Words: Investigating Links Between Parent Report and Looking Time","authors":"Melanie López Pérez,&nbsp;Charlotte Moore,&nbsp;Andrea Sander-Montant,&nbsp;Krista Byers-Heinlein","doi":"10.1111/infa.12641","DOIUrl":"10.1111/infa.12641","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Assessing early vocabulary development commonly involves parent report methods and behavioral tasks like looking-while-listening. While both yield reliable aggregate scores, findings are mixed regarding their reliability in measuring infants' knowledge of individual words. Using archival data from 126 monolingual and bilingual 14–31-month-olds, we further examined links across these methods at the word level, while controlling for potentially confounding child-level factors. When data were averaged at the child level, performance on the looking-while-listening task correlated well with parent-reported word production of the same words, as expected. However, mixed-effects model comparisons suggested that at the word level, looking-while-listening performance was significantly predicted by age and total productive vocabulary, but not by parent-reported knowledge of a word once these factors were controlled for. These findings invite careful consideration regarding the adequacy of these two popular methods for capturing children's idiosyncratic knowledge of individual words.</p>","PeriodicalId":47895,"journal":{"name":"Infancy","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11621253/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787062","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Infancy
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1