首页 > 最新文献

Social Development最新文献

英文 中文
Solitary groups: A latent profile analysis of motivations for social withdrawal and experiences of solitude in late childhood and early adolescence 孤独群体:对童年晚期和青少年早期社会退缩动机和孤独体验的潜在特征分析
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12742
Matilde Brunetti, Stefania Sette, Tiffany Cheng, Fiorenzo Laghi, Emiddia Longobardi, Concetta Pastorelli, Antonio Zuffianò, Robert J. Coplan
The present study aims to differentiate groups of children and early adolescents characterized by their motivations for social withdrawal and personal experiences with solitude. Participants were = 561 (307 girls) children and early adolescents, aged 8–14 years (M = 11.32, SD = 1.63), who completed self-report assessments of motivations for social withdrawal (i.e., shyness, unsociability), social/asocial dissatisfaction (i.e., loneliness, aloneliness), time alone, affect during solitude, personality traits (i.e., Big Five), and indices of internalizing difficulties (i.e., social anxiety, depression). Results from a Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) provided evidence of three distinct groups characterized by different motivations for social withdrawal and experiences with solitude: (1) the shy group, characterized by higher levels of loneliness, social anxiety, depression, and emotional instability; (2) the unsociable group, who reported higher levels of aloneliness and average scores of extraversion and internalizing difficulties; and (3) the sociable group, characterized by lower levels of both loneliness and aloneliness, and higher levels of extraversion. Overall, findings confirmed the heterogeneity in how children and early adolescents experience solitude, their motivations, and individual dispositions.
本研究旨在根据儿童和早期青少年的社交退缩动机和个人独处经历来区分他们的群体。参与研究的儿童和青少年共有 561 人(307 名女孩),年龄在 8-14 岁之间(中位数 = 11.32,标准差 = 1.63),他们完成了有关社交退缩动机(即害羞、不合群)、社交/非社交不满(即孤独、寂寞)、独处时间、独处时的情绪、人格特质(即五大人格)和内化困难指数(即社交焦虑、抑郁)的自我报告评估。潜特征分析(LPA)的结果表明,有三个不同的群体,其特点是退出社会的动机和独处的经历各不相同:(1) 害羞群体,其特点是孤独感、社交焦虑、抑郁和情绪不稳定程度较高;(2) 不善交际群体,他们报告的孤独感程度较高,外向性和内化困难的平均得分较高;(3) 善于交际群体,其特点是孤独感和孤独感程度较低,外向性程度较高。总之,研究结果证实了儿童和青少年体验孤独的方式、动机和个人倾向的异质性。
{"title":"Solitary groups: A latent profile analysis of motivations for social withdrawal and experiences of solitude in late childhood and early adolescence","authors":"Matilde Brunetti, Stefania Sette, Tiffany Cheng, Fiorenzo Laghi, Emiddia Longobardi, Concetta Pastorelli, Antonio Zuffianò, Robert J. Coplan","doi":"10.1111/sode.12742","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12742","url":null,"abstract":"The present study aims to differentiate groups of children and early adolescents characterized by their motivations for social withdrawal and personal experiences with solitude. Participants were <i>N </i>= 561 (307 girls) children and early adolescents, aged 8–14 years (<i>M</i> = 11.32, SD = 1.63), who completed self-report assessments of motivations for social withdrawal (i.e., shyness, unsociability), social/asocial dissatisfaction (i.e., loneliness, aloneliness), time alone, affect during solitude, personality traits (i.e., Big Five), and indices of internalizing difficulties (i.e., social anxiety, depression). Results from a Latent Profile Analysis (LPA) provided evidence of three distinct groups characterized by different motivations for social withdrawal and experiences with solitude: (1) the <i>shy</i> group, characterized by higher levels of loneliness, social anxiety, depression, and emotional instability; (2) the <i>unsociable</i> group, who reported higher levels of aloneliness and average scores of extraversion and internalizing difficulties; and (3) the <i>sociable</i> group, characterized by lower levels of both loneliness and aloneliness, and higher levels of extraversion. Overall, findings confirmed the heterogeneity in how children and early adolescents experience solitude, their motivations, and individual dispositions.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140601391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early intergroup coalition: Toddlers attribute fair distributions to Black rather than White distributors 早期群际联盟:幼儿将公平分配归功于黑人而非白人分配者
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-31 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12740
Alessandra Geraci, Elena Commodari, Paola Perucchini
Racial concepts emerge in preschool age, and affect children's evaluations of others’ actions. This research investigated whether 2.5-year-old and 7-year-old children's (= 160; 100% White) evaluations may be influenced by an initial racial bias when both out-group and in-group protagonists were evaluated directly by attributing the responsibility of negative or positive outcomes (i.e., fair or unfair distributions of resources). After seeing each of two familiarization events, displaying fair and unfair distributions performed by a hidden distributor, toddlers were asked to assign positive and negative outcomes to two distributors that were portrayed on drawings (Experiment 1) or photographs (Experiments 2–3) of White or Black faces. Seven-year-old children were assessed with the same forced-choice character task by using photographs (Experiment 4). Toddlers showed a significant tendency to attribute a fair distribution of resources to Black faces only when these were shown by photographs. Conversely, 7-year-olds manifested a pro-White/anti-Black racial bias. These findings support prior literature on the age-related development of race prejudice starting not before the three years of life, and shed light on an initial state that is aimed at intergroup coalitions.
种族观念在学龄前就已出现,并影响儿童对他人行为的评价。本研究调查了 2.5 岁和 7 岁儿童(人数=160;100% 白人)的评价是否会受到最初种族偏见的影响,即直接评价外群体和内群体主角时,他们会将负面或正面结果(即资源的公平或不公平分配)的责任归咎于外群体和内群体主角。在看过由隐藏的分配者分别进行的公平和不公平分配的两个熟悉事件后,幼儿被要求将积极和消极的结果归因于两个分配者,这两个分配者分别以白人或黑人面孔的图画(实验 1)或照片(实验 2-3)为描绘对象。七岁儿童也接受了同样的强迫选择人物任务,即使用照片进行评估(实验 4)。只有当照片显示的是黑人面孔时,幼儿才表现出明显的将资源公平分配给黑人面孔的倾向。相反,7 岁幼儿则表现出支持白人/反对黑人的种族偏见。这些发现支持了之前关于种族偏见的发展与年龄相关的文献,即种族偏见的发展始于出生后三岁之前,并揭示了以群体间联盟为目标的初始状态。
{"title":"Early intergroup coalition: Toddlers attribute fair distributions to Black rather than White distributors","authors":"Alessandra Geraci, Elena Commodari, Paola Perucchini","doi":"10.1111/sode.12740","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12740","url":null,"abstract":"Racial concepts emerge in preschool age, and affect children's evaluations of others’ actions. This research investigated whether 2.5-year-old and 7-year-old children's (<i>N </i>= 160; 100% White) evaluations may be influenced by an initial racial bias when both out-group and in-group protagonists were evaluated directly by attributing the responsibility of negative or positive outcomes (i.e., fair or unfair distributions of resources). After seeing each of two familiarization events, displaying fair and unfair distributions performed by a hidden distributor, toddlers were asked to assign positive and negative outcomes to two distributors that were portrayed on drawings (Experiment 1) or photographs (Experiments 2–3) of White or Black faces. Seven-year-old children were assessed with the same forced-choice character task by using photographs (Experiment 4). Toddlers showed a significant tendency to attribute a fair distribution of resources to Black faces only when these were shown by photographs. Conversely, 7-year-olds manifested a pro-White/anti-Black racial bias. These findings support prior literature on the age-related development of race prejudice starting not before the three years of life, and shed light on an initial state that is aimed at intergroup coalitions.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"9 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-31","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140601484","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
If looks could talk: Threat and familiarity influence with whom and how infants interact in ambiguous situations 如果外表会说话威胁和熟悉程度影响婴儿在模棱两可的情况下与谁互动以及如何互动
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12741
Samantha Ehli, Silvia Schneider, Albert Newen, Babett Voigt
We investigated how apparent threat of an ambiguous stimuli modulates infants’ looking to interaction partners of varying familiarity (mother, familiar experimenter, unfamiliar experimenter). We hypothesized a preference for familiar informants under higher apparent threat, but a preference for unfamiliar informants under lower apparent threat. The informant encouraged infants (N = 104, 8–13 months) to cross a visual cliff in one of two apparent threat conditions (lower vs. higher drop‐off). Under lower threat, infants looked equally long to all informants, but switched gazes more often between their mother and the cliff. Infants explored the cliff more and crossed more often in the mother condition compared to the other two conditions. They also expressed less negative affect in the presence of the mother compared to the unfamiliar experimenter, but not compared to the familiar experimenter. Under high threat, a similar pattern emerged, except that looking duration to the unfamiliar informant was shorter compared to the low threat condition. Heart rate acceleration appeared when infants were placed on the cliff compared to a baseline phase. Higher levels of negative affectivity (but not higher arousal) were observed under higher compared to lower threat. Overall, we found little evidence of the influence of threat within the visual cliff task. We argue that infants may have perceived the cliff as quite challenging even in the lower threat condition and call for more research on the situational embeddedness of early social learning processes.
我们研究了模糊刺激的表观威胁如何调节婴儿对不同熟悉程度的互动伙伴(母亲、熟悉的实验者、不熟悉的实验者)的观察。我们假设,在表观威胁较高的情况下,婴儿会偏好熟悉的信息提供者;而在表观威胁较低的情况下,婴儿会偏好不熟悉的信息提供者。信息提供者鼓励婴儿(104 名,8-13 个月)在两种明显威胁条件(较低落差与较高落差)之一下穿过视觉悬崖。在较低的威胁条件下,婴儿看所有信息提供者的时间都一样长,但在母亲和悬崖之间切换目光的频率更高。与其他两种情况相比,在母亲的情况下,婴儿探索悬崖的次数更多,越过悬崖的次数也更频繁。与不熟悉的实验者相比,婴儿在母亲面前表现出的负面情绪更少,但与熟悉的实验者相比则没有。在高威胁条件下,也出现了类似的模式,只是与低威胁条件相比,婴儿看陌生实验者的时间更短。与基线阶段相比,当婴儿被置于悬崖上时,心跳加速。与低威胁条件相比,高威胁条件下婴儿的消极情绪水平更高(但唤醒水平并不更高)。总体而言,我们在视觉悬崖任务中几乎没有发现威胁影响的证据。我们认为,即使在较低的威胁条件下,婴儿也可能认为悬崖具有相当的挑战性,并呼吁对早期社会学习过程的情景嵌入性进行更多的研究。
{"title":"If looks could talk: Threat and familiarity influence with whom and how infants interact in ambiguous situations","authors":"Samantha Ehli, Silvia Schneider, Albert Newen, Babett Voigt","doi":"10.1111/sode.12741","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12741","url":null,"abstract":"We investigated how apparent threat of an ambiguous stimuli modulates infants’ looking to interaction partners of varying familiarity (mother, familiar experimenter, unfamiliar experimenter). We hypothesized a preference for familiar informants under higher apparent threat, but a preference for unfamiliar informants under lower apparent threat. The informant encouraged infants (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 104, 8–13 months) to cross a visual cliff in one of two apparent threat conditions (lower vs. higher drop‐off). Under lower threat, infants looked equally long to all informants, but switched gazes more often between their mother and the cliff. Infants explored the cliff more and crossed more often in the mother condition compared to the other two conditions. They also expressed less negative affect in the presence of the mother compared to the unfamiliar experimenter, but not compared to the familiar experimenter. Under high threat, a similar pattern emerged, except that looking duration to the unfamiliar informant was shorter compared to the low threat condition. Heart rate acceleration appeared when infants were placed on the cliff compared to a baseline phase. Higher levels of negative affectivity (but not higher arousal) were observed under higher compared to lower threat. Overall, we found little evidence of the influence of threat within the visual cliff task. We argue that infants may have perceived the cliff as quite challenging even in the lower threat condition and call for more research on the situational embeddedness of early social learning processes.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"95 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140199179","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Convergent and divergent parental emotion socialization processes shape children's emotional responding 父母情绪社会化过程的趋同性和差异性塑造了儿童的情绪反应
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12739
Laura DeLoretta, Elizabeth L. Davis
Parents have an essential role in shaping children's emotional responses, a process called emotional socialization. Typically, researchers measure parental socialization behaviors via self‐report and in‐laboratory observations. However, the extent to which there is convergence between parents’ reported and enacted socialization practices is an open question. The aims of the current study were (1) to quantify the convergence/divergence in parents’ reported and enacted emotional socialization practices and (2) to analyze how convergence/divergence in specific parent socialization practices relates to children's emotional responding across different contexts (alone and with a parent). Participants were a diverse sample of 181 parent‐child dyads (children ages 3–11). We analyzed emotion socialization strategies (problem‐focused reactions, emotion‐focused reactions, expressive encouragement, punishing reactions, minimizing reactions, and distress reactions) as they related to children's behavioral and physiological responding to an emotional challenge. We found an overall pattern of divergence for most socialization strategies, but convergence for problem‐focused reactions. For children's observed distress, a convergent pattern of reported and enacted minimizing specifically was related to more distress while alone. For children's physiological reactivity, enacted problem‐solving was related to greater parasympathetic decreases across social context (from being alone to being with parent). Taken together, our findings suggest that convergence and divergence in parental emotional socialization practices may capture unique variability in how parents contextually respond to children's feelings, and children's emotional responding.
父母在塑造儿童的情绪反应方面起着至关重要的作用,这一过程被称为情绪社会化。通常情况下,研究人员通过自我报告和实验室观察来衡量父母的社会化行为。然而,父母所报告的社会化行为与所实施的社会化行为在多大程度上是一致的,这还是一个悬而未决的问题。本研究的目的是:(1)量化父母报告和实施的情感社会化行为的趋同/差异;(2)分析特定父母社会化行为的趋同/差异与儿童在不同情境下(单独和与父母在一起)的情感反应之间的关系。参与者是 181 个亲子二人组(儿童年龄为 3-11 岁)的不同样本。我们分析了情绪社会化策略(以问题为中心的反应、以情绪为中心的反应、表达性鼓励、惩罚性反应、最小化反应和痛苦反应)与儿童对情绪挑战的行为和生理反应之间的关系。我们发现,大多数社会化策略的总体模式是分歧的,但以问题为中心的反应则是趋同的。在儿童观察到的痛苦方面,报告和实施的具体最小化模式趋同,这与儿童独处时更多的痛苦有关。就儿童的生理反应而言,在不同的社会情境下(从独处到与父母在一起),制定的问题解决方法与副交感神经的减少有关。综上所述,我们的研究结果表明,父母情感社会化实践的趋同性和差异性可能会捕捉到父母如何在情境中对儿童的情感以及儿童的情感反应做出反应的独特变异性。
{"title":"Convergent and divergent parental emotion socialization processes shape children's emotional responding","authors":"Laura DeLoretta, Elizabeth L. Davis","doi":"10.1111/sode.12739","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12739","url":null,"abstract":"Parents have an essential role in shaping children's emotional responses, a process called emotional socialization. Typically, researchers measure parental socialization behaviors via self‐report and in‐laboratory observations. However, the extent to which there is convergence between parents’ reported and enacted socialization practices is an open question. The aims of the current study were (1) to quantify the convergence/divergence in parents’ reported and enacted emotional socialization practices and (2) to analyze how convergence/divergence in specific parent socialization practices relates to children's emotional responding across different contexts (alone and with a parent). Participants were a diverse sample of 181 parent‐child dyads (children ages 3–11). We analyzed emotion socialization strategies (problem‐focused reactions, emotion‐focused reactions, expressive encouragement, punishing reactions, minimizing reactions, and distress reactions) as they related to children's behavioral and physiological responding to an emotional challenge. We found an overall pattern of divergence for most socialization strategies, but convergence for problem‐focused reactions. For children's observed distress, a convergent pattern of reported and enacted minimizing specifically was related to more distress while alone. For children's physiological reactivity, enacted problem‐solving was related to greater parasympathetic decreases across social context (from being alone to being with parent). Taken together, our findings suggest that convergence and divergence in parental emotional socialization practices may capture unique variability in how parents contextually respond to children's feelings, and children's emotional responding.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140117004","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Long-term implications of childhood and adolescent popularity for social behavior and status in emerging adulthood 童年和青少年时期的受欢迎程度对成年后社会行为和地位的长期影响
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12735
Nina S. Chmielowice-Szymanski, Tessa A. M. Lansu, William J. Burk, Yvonne H. M. van den Berg, Antonius H. N. Cillessen
Peer status is associated with social functioning throughout childhood and adolescence, but little is known about its’ long-term implications. This study examined longitudinal associations of childhood and adolescent popularity with social behavior and status in emerging adulthood. In line with concurrent associations of popularity with social behavior, we hypothesized that childhood popularity would be associated with a positive, prosocial profile, and adolescent popularity would be associated with a powerful and forceful profile. As research has shown curvilinear associations of popularity with aggression in childhood and adolescence, we also examined whether longitudinal associations were curvilinear. Peer-nominated popularity was measured at 9 and 16 years of age for 118 longitudinal participants. At age 24, they participated in a survey and an online video call session together with three unfamiliar peers, after which everyone in the session rated each other's social behavior. Results showed significant curvilinear associations of childhood and adolescent popularity with emerging adult social behavior and status. Childhood popularity was associated with positive, prosocial behavior and status indicators, showing that in addition to high childhood popularity, also low childhood popularity was associated with higher peer-reported positive, prosocial behavior. High adolescent popularity was associated with both positive, prosocial, and powerful and forceful behavior and status indicators, with especially low adolescent popularity associated with lower levels of these behaviors and status in emerging adulthood. Childhood and adolescent popularity were thus uniquely and differentially associated with social behavior and status in emerging adulthood.
同伴地位与整个童年和青少年时期的社会功能有关,但对其长期影响却知之甚少。本研究考察了童年和青少年时期的受欢迎程度与成年后的社会行为和地位之间的纵向联系。根据受欢迎程度与社会行为的并行关系,我们假设童年时期的受欢迎程度与积极、亲社会的形象有关,而青少年时期的受欢迎程度与强大、有力的形象有关。研究表明,在童年和青少年时期,受欢迎程度与攻击行为之间存在曲线关系,因此我们还研究了纵向关系是否也是曲线关系。我们对 118 名纵向参与者在 9 岁和 16 岁时的同伴提名人气进行了测量。在 24 岁时,他们参加了一项调查,并与三名陌生同伴一起参加了在线视频通话会话,之后会话中的每个人都对彼此的社交行为进行了评分。结果显示,童年和青少年时期的受欢迎程度与成年后的社会行为和地位有明显的曲线关联。童年受欢迎程度与积极的亲社会行为和地位指标相关,这表明除了童年受欢迎程度高之外,童年受欢迎程度低也与同伴报告的积极的亲社会行为相关。青少年时期的高受欢迎程度与积极的、亲社会的、强势有力的行为和地位指标都有关,尤其是青少年时期的低受欢迎程度与这些行为和地位在成年后的较低水平有关。因此,童年和青少年时期的受欢迎程度与成年后的社会行为和地位有着独特和不同的关联。
{"title":"Long-term implications of childhood and adolescent popularity for social behavior and status in emerging adulthood","authors":"Nina S. Chmielowice-Szymanski, Tessa A. M. Lansu, William J. Burk, Yvonne H. M. van den Berg, Antonius H. N. Cillessen","doi":"10.1111/sode.12735","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12735","url":null,"abstract":"Peer status is associated with social functioning throughout childhood and adolescence, but little is known about its’ long-term implications. This study examined longitudinal associations of childhood and adolescent popularity with social behavior and status in emerging adulthood. In line with concurrent associations of popularity with social behavior, we hypothesized that childhood popularity would be associated with a positive, prosocial profile, and adolescent popularity would be associated with a powerful and forceful profile. As research has shown curvilinear associations of popularity with aggression in childhood and adolescence, we also examined whether longitudinal associations were curvilinear. Peer-nominated popularity was measured at 9 and 16 years of age for 118 longitudinal participants. At age 24, they participated in a survey and an online video call session together with three unfamiliar peers, after which everyone in the session rated each other's social behavior. Results showed significant curvilinear associations of childhood and adolescent popularity with emerging adult social behavior and status. Childhood popularity was associated with positive, prosocial behavior and status indicators, showing that in addition to high childhood popularity, also low childhood popularity was associated with higher peer-reported positive, prosocial behavior. High adolescent popularity was associated with both positive, prosocial, and powerful and forceful behavior and status indicators, with especially low adolescent popularity associated with lower levels of these behaviors and status in emerging adulthood. Childhood and adolescent popularity were thus uniquely and differentially associated with social behavior and status in emerging adulthood.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"27 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140071720","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measuring social and emotional functioning as a facet of positive youth development among children and adolescents in special education and mental health treatment 在接受特殊教育和心理健康治疗的儿童和青少年中测量社会和情感功能,作为青少年积极发展的一个方面
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-03-06 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12738
Brian Bello, Erin Flynn, G. John Geldhof, Dian Yu, Megan K. Mueller, Kristin Licardi, Kevin N. Morris
This study assessed the validity of a set of social and emotional functioning constructs derived from the PYD short form (PYD-SF) measure within a sample of children and adolescents with one or more mental health diagnoses related to social, emotional, or behavioral challenges. Using repeated measures design, responses to the PYD-SF and the Social-emotional assets and resilience scale were collected over two years from 369 students enrolled in special education. Multilevel factor analyses parsed within-person variability from between-person differences to understand the internal consistency of the target measures when used with clinical and special education populations. Associations between the social-emotional constructs from the PYD-SF and an assessment of social-emotional assets and resilience were examined. A two-level confirmatory factor analysis using seven PYD factors and the Social-Emotional Assets and Resilience Scale indicated a very good model fit; all factor loadings were statistically significant at both levels. Results supported the validity and internal reliability of the social-emotional constructs from the PYD-SF measure for assessing the social and emotional competencies of youth with social, emotional, and/or behavioral challenges, but with the caveat that researchers should consider a higher degree of specificity when using it with youth from this population. The broad factors utilized in previous work should be reframed to emphasize separate types of connection. When keeping this revised framing in mind, the social-emotional constructs of the PYD-SF measure provide a tool for teachers, parents, and clinicians to use in treatment planning and identifying strategies to support positive development for diverse youth.
本研究评估了从PYD简表(PYD-SF)量表中衍生出的一系列社会和情感功能建构的有效性,该量表的样本是被诊断出患有一种或多种与社会、情感或行为挑战相关的心理健康问题的儿童和青少年。采用重复测量设计,在两年内收集了 369 名接受特殊教育的学生对PYD-SF 和社会情感资产与复原力量表的回答。多层次因素分析从人与人之间的差异中解析出人与人之间的差异,以了解目标量表在临床和特殊教育人群中使用时的内部一致性。研究还考察了PYD-SF中的社会情感建构与社会情感资产和复原力评估之间的关联。使用七个PYD因子和社会情感资产与复原力量表进行的两级确认性因子分析表明,模型的拟合度非常高;所有因子载荷在两级均具有统计学意义。研究结果表明,PYD-SF 量表中的社会情感建构对于评估具有社会、情感和/或行为挑战的青少年的社会情感能力具有有效性和内部可靠性,但需要注意的是,研究人员在对这一人群中的青少年使用该量表时应考虑更高的特异性。以往工作中使用的宽泛因素应重新设计,以强调不同类型的联系。PYD-SF测量的社会情感建构为教师、家长和临床医生提供了一种工具,可用于制定治疗计划和确定支持不同青少年积极发展的策略。
{"title":"Measuring social and emotional functioning as a facet of positive youth development among children and adolescents in special education and mental health treatment","authors":"Brian Bello, Erin Flynn, G. John Geldhof, Dian Yu, Megan K. Mueller, Kristin Licardi, Kevin N. Morris","doi":"10.1111/sode.12738","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12738","url":null,"abstract":"This study assessed the validity of a set of social and emotional functioning constructs derived from the PYD short form (PYD-SF) measure within a sample of children and adolescents with one or more mental health diagnoses related to social, emotional, or behavioral challenges. Using repeated measures design, responses to the PYD-SF and the Social-emotional assets and resilience scale were collected over two years from 369 students enrolled in special education. Multilevel factor analyses parsed within-person variability from between-person differences to understand the internal consistency of the target measures when used with clinical and special education populations. Associations between the social-emotional constructs from the PYD-SF and an assessment of social-emotional assets and resilience were examined. A two-level confirmatory factor analysis using seven PYD factors and the Social-Emotional Assets and Resilience Scale indicated a very good model fit; all factor loadings were statistically significant at both levels. Results supported the validity and internal reliability of the social-emotional constructs from the PYD-SF measure for assessing the social and emotional competencies of youth with social, emotional, and/or behavioral challenges, but with the caveat that researchers should consider a higher degree of specificity when using it with youth from this population. The broad factors utilized in previous work should be reframed to emphasize separate types of connection. When keeping this revised framing in mind, the social-emotional constructs of the PYD-SF measure provide a tool for teachers, parents, and clinicians to use in treatment planning and identifying strategies to support positive development for diverse youth.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"12 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-03-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140071828","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Comparing methods of social preference assessment in childhood 比较儿童社会偏好评估方法
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-27 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12736
Benjamin deMayo, Kristina R. Olson
A central question in social cognitive development concerns the degree to which children prefer social ingroup members relative to social outgroup members. Forced‐choice measures and continuous rating scales are often used to assess these preferences, but little work has examined the extent to which these two methods yield similar or divergent estimates. In Study 1, we used a within‐subjects design to assess gender‐, race‐, and accent‐based preferences in 5–6‐year‐old predominantly white children (N = 100) with both a forced‐choice and a rating measure (on a 1–6 scale); replicating prior work, children expressed ingroup preference along all three dimensions regardless of how they were assessed. In Study 2, we replicated the discrepancy between forced‐choice and rating in children's ingroup gender preferences in a more racially diverse sample (N = 55). In both studies, while responses on forced‐choice and rating measures were correlated, estimates of ingroup preference were stronger in each domain when assessed with a forced‐choice measure. We discuss the implications for researchers who wish to assess social group preferences.
社会认知发展的一个核心问题是,相对于社会外群体成员,儿童对社会内群体成员的偏好程度。强迫选择测量法和连续评定量表经常被用来评估这些偏好,但很少有研究对这两种方法在多大程度上产生相似或不同的估计结果进行研究。在研究 1 中,我们采用了主体内设计,通过强迫选择和评分量表(1-6 分)对以白人为主的 5-6 岁儿童(100 人)的性别、种族和口音偏好进行了评估;与之前的研究结果相同,无论评估方式如何,儿童都在所有三个维度上表达了内群体偏好。在研究 2 中,我们在一个种族更加多样化的样本(N = 55)中重复了强迫选择和评分在儿童内群体性别偏好方面的差异。在这两项研究中,虽然强迫选择和评分测量的反应是相关的,但如果使用强迫选择测量进行评估,则每个领域的内群体偏好估计值都更强。我们讨论了这对希望评估社会群体偏好的研究人员的影响。
{"title":"Comparing methods of social preference assessment in childhood","authors":"Benjamin deMayo, Kristina R. Olson","doi":"10.1111/sode.12736","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12736","url":null,"abstract":"A central question in social cognitive development concerns the degree to which children prefer social ingroup members relative to social outgroup members. Forced‐choice measures and continuous rating scales are often used to assess these preferences, but little work has examined the extent to which these two methods yield similar or divergent estimates. In Study 1, we used a within‐subjects design to assess gender‐, race‐, and accent‐based preferences in 5–6‐year‐old predominantly white children (<jats:italic>N </jats:italic>= 100) with both a forced‐choice and a rating measure (on a 1–6 scale); replicating prior work, children expressed ingroup preference along all three dimensions regardless of how they were assessed. In Study 2, we replicated the discrepancy between forced‐choice and rating in children's ingroup gender preferences in a more racially diverse sample (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 55). In both studies, while responses on forced‐choice and rating measures were correlated, estimates of ingroup preference were stronger in each domain when assessed with a forced‐choice measure. We discuss the implications for researchers who wish to assess social group preferences.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"9 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140025413","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Shyness, social engagement, and conversational response times in children's dyadic interactions with an unfamiliar peer 儿童与陌生同伴的双人互动中的害羞、社交参与和对话反应时间
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12734
McLennon Wilson, Adrienne Richter Powell, Linda Sosa Hernandez, Emma Green, Claudia Labahn, Heather Henderson
To be a desirable social partner and develop healthy relationships with peers, a child must be able to engage with peers across a variety of contexts. Understanding the factors supporting high levels of social engagement with peers is thereby essential, requiring the development of nuanced and ecologically valid indices of social engagement. Building on recent adult work, the current study explores conversational response time as a novel index of children's social engagement with peers in a dyadic context. This study further explores relationship between conversational response time and children's shyness. Fifty-six 9- to 11-year-old children interacted with an unfamiliar peer in an unstructured setting and completed a self-report measure of shyness. Children's behaviour was coded for their conversational RTs and overall social engagement. Faster conversational RTs were significantly related to children's own social engagement and marginally related to their partners' engagement. Moreover, higher shyness in children's partners predicted faster conversational RTs in children themselves. New directions for using conversational RT as an index of children's social engagement and implications for accounts children's social development are discussed.
要成为理想的社会伙伴并与同伴建立健康的关系,儿童必须能够在各种环境中与同伴交往。因此,了解支持儿童与同伴进行高水平社交接触的因素至关重要,这就需要开发细致入微、生态学上有效的社交接触指数。在近期成人研究的基础上,本研究探索了会话反应时间,将其作为儿童与同伴在双人情境中进行社会交往的新指标。本研究进一步探讨了对话反应时间与儿童害羞之间的关系。56 名 9 至 11 岁的儿童在非结构化环境中与一名不熟悉的同伴进行了互动,并完成了一项关于害羞的自我报告测量。儿童的行为根据他们的对话反应时间和整体社交参与度进行编码。较快的会话RT与儿童自身的社会参与度有明显关系,而与他们伙伴的参与度关系不大。此外,儿童伙伴的害羞程度越高,儿童自己的会话RT越快。本研究讨论了将会话RT作为儿童社会参与指数的新方向,以及对儿童社会发展账户的影响。
{"title":"Shyness, social engagement, and conversational response times in children's dyadic interactions with an unfamiliar peer","authors":"McLennon Wilson, Adrienne Richter Powell, Linda Sosa Hernandez, Emma Green, Claudia Labahn, Heather Henderson","doi":"10.1111/sode.12734","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12734","url":null,"abstract":"To be a desirable social partner and develop healthy relationships with peers, a child must be able to engage with peers across a variety of contexts. Understanding the factors supporting high levels of social engagement with peers is thereby essential, requiring the development of nuanced and ecologically valid indices of social engagement. Building on recent adult work, the current study explores conversational response time as a novel index of children's social engagement with peers in a dyadic context. This study further explores relationship between conversational response time and children's shyness. Fifty-six 9- to 11-year-old children interacted with an unfamiliar peer in an unstructured setting and completed a self-report measure of shyness. Children's behaviour was coded for their conversational RTs and overall social engagement. Faster conversational RTs were significantly related to children's own social engagement and marginally related to their partners' engagement. Moreover, higher shyness in children's partners predicted faster conversational RTs in children themselves. New directions for using conversational RT as an index of children's social engagement and implications for accounts children's social development are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"22 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139754417","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How are mental state references represented in English and Japanese picture books? An analysis of the frequency of emotional and cognitive words and their relation to the self or others 英语和日语图画书中是如何表现心理状态的?分析情感和认知词汇的使用频率及其与自我或他人的关系
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-30 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12731
Yuko Okumura, Shunya Taguchi, Yasuhiro Kanakogi
The text of picture books is a fertile source through which young children learn about mental states. By focusing on English and Japanese books (N = 100; for children aged 3–5 years) as respective representatives of independent and interdependent cultures, the present study examined the cultural differences in the use of two types of mental state language: emotion and cognition. While our findings revealed no cultural differences in emotional word tokens or types, cognitive word tokens and types were higher in English picture books than in Japanese ones. Importantly, English picture books exhibited more self-oriented mental state references, while Japanese picture books had more other-oriented mental state references. Our study suggests that mental state references in picture books reflect culture-specific characteristics.
图画书文本是幼儿学习心理状态的重要来源。本研究以分别代表独立文化和相互依存文化的英文绘本和日文绘本(样本数=100;对象为 3-5 岁儿童)为对象,探讨了情感和认知这两种心理状态语言的文化差异。我们的研究结果表明,在情感词汇和类型方面没有文化差异,但认知词汇和类型在英语图画书中却高于日语图画书。重要的是,英语图画书表现出更多以自我为导向的心理状态参照,而日语图画书则表现出更多以他人为导向的心理状态参照。我们的研究表明,图画书中的心理状态参照反映了特定文化的特点。
{"title":"How are mental state references represented in English and Japanese picture books? An analysis of the frequency of emotional and cognitive words and their relation to the self or others","authors":"Yuko Okumura, Shunya Taguchi, Yasuhiro Kanakogi","doi":"10.1111/sode.12731","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12731","url":null,"abstract":"The text of picture books is a fertile source through which young children learn about mental states. By focusing on English and Japanese books (<i>N</i> = 100; for children aged 3–5 years) as respective representatives of independent and interdependent cultures, the present study examined the cultural differences in the use of two types of mental state language: emotion and cognition. While our findings revealed no cultural differences in emotional word tokens or types, cognitive word tokens and types were higher in English picture books than in Japanese ones. Importantly, English picture books exhibited more self-oriented mental state references, while Japanese picture books had more other-oriented mental state references. Our study suggests that mental state references in picture books reflect culture-specific characteristics.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"64 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139903129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
From everyday participation to ways of life: Development of Yurakare children in Bolivia's Amazonian area 从日常参与到生活方式:玻利维亚亚马逊地区尤拉卡雷儿童的发展
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-17 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12732
Natalia Siekiera, Arkadiusz Białek
Children's participation in the social structure from the first stages of life shapes not only their development but also how they learn to become well-adjusted members of their cultural environment. In the presented study, using focal-follow and participatory observation, we depict the reality in early and middle childhood (N = 23; ages 2–7) of Yurakare children living in Bolivia's Amazonian area. We attempt to determine whether the facets of the LOPI model (Learning by Observing and Pitching in) proposed by Rogoff are represented in the everyday way of life of Yurakare children. This is the first systematic, quantitative study of children's social environment and practice in this Indigenous community. The results show that the practices of the Yurakare people are based on two things: (1) inclusion of all ages in community life, which cultivates children to engage in useful activities even while having fun; (2) the primacy of mature activities, which is in line with the LOPI model.
儿童从生命的最初阶段开始就参与社会结构,这不仅影响了他们的成长,也影响了他们如何学习成为适应其文化环境的成员。在本研究中,我们通过焦点跟踪和参与式观察,描绘了生活在玻利维亚亚马逊地区的尤拉卡雷(Yurakare)儿童(23 人,2-7 岁)在童年早期和中期的现实生活。我们试图确定罗格夫提出的 LOPI 模式(通过观察和参与学习)的各个方面是否体现在尤拉卡雷儿童的日常生活方式中。这是首次对这一土著社区儿童的社会环境和实践进行系统的定量研究。研究结果表明,尤拉卡雷人的做法基于两点:(1) 将所有年龄段的人都纳入社区生活,培养儿童在玩乐的同时参与有益的活动;(2) 将成熟的活动放在首位,这与 LOPI 模式是一致的。
{"title":"From everyday participation to ways of life: Development of Yurakare children in Bolivia's Amazonian area","authors":"Natalia Siekiera, Arkadiusz Białek","doi":"10.1111/sode.12732","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12732","url":null,"abstract":"Children's participation in the social structure from the first stages of life shapes not only their development but also how they learn to become well-adjusted members of their cultural environment. In the presented study, using focal-follow and participatory observation, we depict the reality in early and middle childhood (<i>N</i> = 23; ages 2–7) of Yurakare children living in Bolivia's Amazonian area. We attempt to determine whether the facets of the LOPI model (Learning by Observing and Pitching in) proposed by Rogoff are represented in the everyday way of life of Yurakare children. This is the first systematic, quantitative study of children's social environment and practice in this Indigenous community. The results show that the practices of the Yurakare people are based on two things: (1) inclusion of all ages in community life, which cultivates children to engage in useful activities even while having fun; (2) the primacy of mature activities, which is in line with the LOPI model.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"37 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-01-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139515214","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1