首页 > 最新文献

Social Development最新文献

英文 中文
The role of between-group competition in children's within-group merit-based resource allocation 组间竞争在儿童组内择优资源分配中的作用
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-12-29 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12728
Xue Xiao, Qian Wang, Yanfang Li
The present study tested how 5- to 6-year-old and 7- to 8-year-old children allocate with in-group collaborators according to merit in the context of external between-group competition. Children (N = 310) first were asked to collaborate with a high- or low-merit partner to complete an intergroup game in the form of competition (further divided into win and lose conditions) or noncompetition. Afterward, they were asked to allocate, reason about, and express their expected allocations toward the in-group collaborator. We found that 5–6-year-olds allocated meritoriously with collaborators in the first-party context. In contrast, 7–8-year-olds were affected by external between-group competition. Specifically, compared with the noncompetitive condition, 7–8-year-olds conducted equal or roughly equal allocations with the in-group collaborator and referenced Equality and Affiliation more frequently in the win and lose conditions. Furthermore, both 5–6-year-olds and 7–8-year-olds expected teachers to allocate meritoriously across the win, lose and noncompetitive conditions, indicating that they realized that social norms require them to make allocations based on merit rather than social relationships. The findings suggest that with age, children weighed the moral concerns of merit and the social concerns of in-group harmony when determining the allocation of resources.
本研究测试了 5 至 6 岁和 7 至 8 岁的儿童在外部群体间竞争的背景下如何根据优劣分配与群体内合作者的关系。首先,研究人员要求儿童(310 人)与高分或低分的伙伴合作,以竞争(又分为赢和输两种情况)或非竞争的形式完成一个群体间游戏。之后,要求他们分配、推理并表达他们对组内合作者的预期分配。我们发现,在第一方情境中,5-6 岁儿童对合作者的分配是有利的。与此相反,7-8 岁儿童受到外部群体间竞争的影响。具体地说,与非竞争条件相比,7-8 岁幼儿与组内合作者进行了平等或大致平等的分配,并且在赢和输的条件下更频繁地引用平等和亲缘关系。此外,5-6 岁和 7-8 岁的幼儿都希望教师在输赢和非竞争条件下都能按功劳分配,这表明他们意识到社会规范要求他们根据功劳而不是社会关系来分配。研究结果表明,随着年龄的增长,幼儿在决定资源分配时,会权衡功绩的道德考量和组内和谐的社会考量。
{"title":"The role of between-group competition in children's within-group merit-based resource allocation","authors":"Xue Xiao, Qian Wang, Yanfang Li","doi":"10.1111/sode.12728","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12728","url":null,"abstract":"The present study tested how 5- to 6-year-old and 7- to 8-year-old children allocate with in-group collaborators according to merit in the context of external between-group competition. Children (<i>N</i> = 310) first were asked to collaborate with a high- or low-merit partner to complete an intergroup game in the form of competition (further divided into win and lose conditions) or noncompetition. Afterward, they were asked to allocate, reason about, and express their expected allocations toward the in-group collaborator. We found that 5–6-year-olds allocated meritoriously with collaborators in the first-party context. In contrast, 7–8-year-olds were affected by external between-group competition. Specifically, compared with the noncompetitive condition, 7–8-year-olds conducted equal or roughly equal allocations with the in-group collaborator and referenced <i>Equality</i> and <i>Affiliation</i> more frequently in the win and lose conditions. Furthermore, both 5–6-year-olds and 7–8-year-olds expected teachers to allocate meritoriously across the win, lose and noncompetitive conditions, indicating that they realized that social norms require them to make allocations based on merit rather than social relationships. The findings suggest that with age, children weighed the moral concerns of merit and the social concerns of in-group harmony when determining the allocation of resources.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"17 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-12-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139065839","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social integration in the activity peer group in sport and non-sport organized activities: Links with depressive symptoms in adolescence 体育和非体育组织活动中同伴群体的社会整合:与青少年抑郁症状的联系
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-27 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12727
Charles-Étienne White-Gosselin, François Poulin, Anne-Sophie Denault
Organized activities can provide a conducive context for various social processes that may prevent internalizing problems. Some types of organized activities, such as team sports, seem particularly favorable to these positive experiences. The aim of this 4-year longitudinal study is to describe the changes in the feeling of social integration into the organized activity peer group and to examine whether this social process predicts depressive symptoms in adolescence. Team sports also are proposed to promote a high sense of social integration. A total of 292 adolescents (62% female) were followed annually from ages 14 to 17. The type of main organized activity practiced and the feeling of social integration into the activity peer group was measured each year. Depressive symptoms were self-reported at the beginning and end of this period. Latent growth analyses showed that social integration into the organized activity peer group was high and decreasing during adolescence. Social integration was higher in team sports compared to individual sports and non-sport activities as a whole. Finally, a high and sustained level of social integration during adolescence was associated with a low level of depressive symptoms at the end of adolescence, controlling for important covariates. These results suggest that organized activities, particularly team sports, provide a favorable context for developing a feeling of social integration, and that this may protect against depressive symptoms.
有组织的活动可以为各种社会进程提供有利的环境,防止问题内化。某些有组织的活动,如团队运动,似乎特别有利于这些积极的经历。这项为期4年的纵向研究的目的是描述社会融入有组织活动同伴群体的感觉的变化,并检查这种社会过程是否预测青少年的抑郁症状。团队运动也可以促进高度的社会融合感。从14岁到17岁,每年对292名青少年(62%为女性)进行随访。每年测量主要有组织活动的类型和融入活动同伴群体的感觉。抑郁症状在这段时间的开始和结束时都是自我报告的。潜在增长分析显示,青少年时期有组织活动同伴群体的社会整合程度较高,但有所下降。整体而言,团体运动的社会融合程度高于个人运动和非体育活动。最后,在控制重要协变量的情况下,青春期高水平和持续的社会融合与青春期结束时低水平的抑郁症状相关。这些结果表明,有组织的活动,特别是团队运动,为发展社会融合感提供了有利的环境,这可能会防止抑郁症状。
{"title":"Social integration in the activity peer group in sport and non-sport organized activities: Links with depressive symptoms in adolescence","authors":"Charles-Étienne White-Gosselin, François Poulin, Anne-Sophie Denault","doi":"10.1111/sode.12727","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12727","url":null,"abstract":"Organized activities can provide a conducive context for various social processes that may prevent internalizing problems. Some types of organized activities, such as team sports, seem particularly favorable to these positive experiences. The aim of this 4-year longitudinal study is to describe the changes in the feeling of social integration into the organized activity peer group and to examine whether this social process predicts depressive symptoms in adolescence. Team sports also are proposed to promote a high sense of social integration. A total of 292 adolescents (62% female) were followed annually from ages 14 to 17. The type of main organized activity practiced and the feeling of social integration into the activity peer group was measured each year. Depressive symptoms were self-reported at the beginning and end of this period. Latent growth analyses showed that social integration into the organized activity peer group was high and decreasing during adolescence. Social integration was higher in team sports compared to individual sports and non-sport activities as a whole. Finally, a high and sustained level of social integration during adolescence was associated with a low level of depressive symptoms at the end of adolescence, controlling for important covariates. These results suggest that organized activities, particularly team sports, provide a favorable context for developing a feeling of social integration, and that this may protect against depressive symptoms.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138541422","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Connecting adolescent friendships to physical health outcomes: A narrative review 将青少年友谊与身体健康结果联系起来:一项叙述性回顾
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-20 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12726
Alexandra D. Ehrhardt, Hannah L. Schacter
Friendships are developmentally significant peer relationships that meaningfully contribute to adolescent adjustment. Despite extensive evidence that friendships contribute to adolescents’ psychological well-being and mental health, less is known about the connections between adolescents’ friendships and physical health outcomes. Therefore, the current review synthesizes a growing body of research examining associations between adolescent friendships and physical health. The findings reviewed provide evidence for links between the quantity, quality, and stability of adolescents’ friendships and their corresponding subjective and physiological health. Consistency of findings varied as a function of friendship dimensions and health outcomes studied. In turn, we end the review with a discussion of conceptual and methodological consistencies and inconsistencies across the studies reviewed, a proposed agenda for future research, and a presentation of a novel process-oriented model explaining how friendships may contribute to adolescent physical health.
友谊是发展中重要的同伴关系,对青少年的适应有意义的贡献。尽管有大量证据表明友谊有助于青少年的心理健康和精神健康,但对青少年友谊和身体健康结果之间的联系知之甚少。因此,目前的综述综合了越来越多的关于青少年友谊和身体健康之间关系的研究。这些研究结果为青少年友谊的数量、质量和稳定性与他们相应的主观和生理健康之间的联系提供了证据。研究结果的一致性随着友谊维度和健康结果的变化而变化。最后,我们讨论了所审查研究在概念和方法上的一致性和不一致性,提出了未来研究的议程,并提出了一个新的以过程为导向的模型,解释友谊如何有助于青少年的身体健康。
{"title":"Connecting adolescent friendships to physical health outcomes: A narrative review","authors":"Alexandra D. Ehrhardt, Hannah L. Schacter","doi":"10.1111/sode.12726","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12726","url":null,"abstract":"Friendships are developmentally significant peer relationships that meaningfully contribute to adolescent adjustment. Despite extensive evidence that friendships contribute to adolescents’ psychological well-being and mental health, less is known about the connections between adolescents’ friendships and physical health outcomes. Therefore, the current review synthesizes a growing body of research examining associations between adolescent friendships and physical health. The findings reviewed provide evidence for links between the quantity, quality, and stability of adolescents’ friendships and their corresponding subjective and physiological health. Consistency of findings varied as a function of friendship dimensions and health outcomes studied. In turn, we end the review with a discussion of conceptual and methodological consistencies and inconsistencies across the studies reviewed, a proposed agenda for future research, and a presentation of a novel process-oriented model explaining how friendships may contribute to adolescent physical health.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"195 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138541443","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A multidimensional examination of children's endorsement of gender stereotypes 对儿童对性别陈规定型观念的认可进行多方面的审查
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-08 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12725
Cindy Faith Miller, Lorey A. Wheeler, Bobbi Woods
Abstract The present research applied a multidimensional framework to the study of gender stereotypes by investigating whether elementary school children display different levels of endorsement when considering distinct gender stereotype constructs (ability, category, and interest) and feminine versus masculine stereotypes. Study 1 ( N = 403) compared children's ability and category beliefs using a set of gender‐neutral skill items. Study 2 ( N = 539) extended this research by examining whether children showed different patterns of ability and category decisions for feminine versus masculine occupational items. Study 3 ( N = 974) furthered our understanding of the construct dimension by comparing children's interest and ability decisions within the STEM domain. Findings revealed that older elementary school children endorsed ability stereotypes more strongly than category stereotypes and, across all age groups, children endorsed interest stereotypes more strongly than ability stereotypes. Findings also revealed age differences in how children think about masculine versus feminine stereotypes. For masculine stereotypes, younger children showed stronger endorsement than older children; however, for feminine stereotypes, the reverse pattern was found such that older children showed more stereotyped thinking than younger children. The present study illustrates the benefits of employing a multidimensional framework to gain a more nuanced understanding of how children apply their increasing knowledge of gender stereotypes.
摘要本研究采用多维框架研究性别刻板印象,考察小学生在考虑不同的性别刻板印象构念(能力、类别和兴趣)和女性刻板印象与男性刻板印象时是否表现出不同程度的认同。研究1 (N = 403)使用一组性别中立的技能项目比较儿童的能力和类别信念。研究2 (N = 539)扩展了这一研究,考察了儿童在女性和男性职业项目上是否表现出不同的能力模式和类别决策。研究3 (N = 974)通过比较儿童在STEM领域的兴趣和能力决策,进一步加深了我们对建构维度的理解。调查结果显示,年龄较大的小学生更倾向于能力刻板印象,而不是类别刻板印象;在所有年龄组中,儿童更倾向于兴趣刻板印象,而不是能力刻板印象。研究结果还揭示了儿童对男性和女性刻板印象的看法存在年龄差异。对于男性刻板印象,年龄较小的儿童比年龄较大的儿童表现出更强的认同;然而,对于女性的刻板印象,发现了相反的模式,即年龄较大的孩子比年龄较小的孩子表现出更多的刻板印象。目前的研究说明了采用多维框架的好处,以便更细致地了解儿童如何应用他们对性别刻板印象的日益增加的知识。
{"title":"A multidimensional examination of children's endorsement of gender stereotypes","authors":"Cindy Faith Miller, Lorey A. Wheeler, Bobbi Woods","doi":"10.1111/sode.12725","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12725","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The present research applied a multidimensional framework to the study of gender stereotypes by investigating whether elementary school children display different levels of endorsement when considering distinct gender stereotype constructs (ability, category, and interest) and feminine versus masculine stereotypes. Study 1 ( N = 403) compared children's ability and category beliefs using a set of gender‐neutral skill items. Study 2 ( N = 539) extended this research by examining whether children showed different patterns of ability and category decisions for feminine versus masculine occupational items. Study 3 ( N = 974) furthered our understanding of the construct dimension by comparing children's interest and ability decisions within the STEM domain. Findings revealed that older elementary school children endorsed ability stereotypes more strongly than category stereotypes and, across all age groups, children endorsed interest stereotypes more strongly than ability stereotypes. Findings also revealed age differences in how children think about masculine versus feminine stereotypes. For masculine stereotypes, younger children showed stronger endorsement than older children; however, for feminine stereotypes, the reverse pattern was found such that older children showed more stereotyped thinking than younger children. The present study illustrates the benefits of employing a multidimensional framework to gain a more nuanced understanding of how children apply their increasing knowledge of gender stereotypes.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":" 23","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135340974","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Chinese children show racial and skin‐tone salience but little color evasion 中国儿童表现出种族和肤色的突出,但很少回避肤色
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12724
Dan Gao, Mitch van Geel, Junsheng Liu, Judi Mesman
Abstract Children may notice racial differences (i.e., show racial and/or skin‐tone salience), but deliberately avoid mentioning such differences (i.e., color evasion) with increasing age. This contradiction may be due to prevailing egalitarian social norms about race. Color evasion is understudied among children in China. In a sample of 155 Chinese children (71 girls and 84 boys) aged 7–11 years from urban regions of China, we collected measures of children's racial and/or skin‐tone salience and color evasion as well as their attitudes toward light‐skinned East Asian, tan‐skinned East Asian, and White groups. Analyses revealed that racial differences were salient to Chinese children, and that they showed little color evasion. Skin tone was most salient in children's categorization performance. We found a preference hierarchy where light‐skinned East Asian people were favored most, and White people least. Color evasion was negatively related to Chinese children's positive attitudes toward White people. The findings emphasize the importance of social contexts in shaping children's racial attitudes.
儿童可能会注意到种族差异(即表现出种族和/或肤色的显著性),但随着年龄的增长,他们会故意避免提及这些差异(即回避肤色)。这种矛盾可能是由于普遍存在的关于种族的平等主义社会规范。在中国,儿童回避颜色的研究不足。在155名来自中国城市地区的7-11岁的中国儿童(71名女孩和84名男孩)的样本中,我们收集了儿童的种族和/或肤色显著性和逃避颜色的测量,以及他们对浅肤色东亚人、棕褐色肤色东亚人和白人群体的态度。分析显示,种族差异对中国儿童来说是显著的,他们很少回避肤色。肤色对儿童分类表现的影响最为显著。我们发现了一个偏好等级,浅色皮肤的东亚人最受欢迎,而白人最不受欢迎。回避颜色与中国儿童对白人的积极态度呈负相关。研究结果强调了社会环境在塑造儿童种族态度方面的重要性。
{"title":"Chinese children show racial and skin‐tone salience but little color evasion","authors":"Dan Gao, Mitch van Geel, Junsheng Liu, Judi Mesman","doi":"10.1111/sode.12724","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12724","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Children may notice racial differences (i.e., show racial and/or skin‐tone salience), but deliberately avoid mentioning such differences (i.e., color evasion) with increasing age. This contradiction may be due to prevailing egalitarian social norms about race. Color evasion is understudied among children in China. In a sample of 155 Chinese children (71 girls and 84 boys) aged 7–11 years from urban regions of China, we collected measures of children's racial and/or skin‐tone salience and color evasion as well as their attitudes toward light‐skinned East Asian, tan‐skinned East Asian, and White groups. Analyses revealed that racial differences were salient to Chinese children, and that they showed little color evasion. Skin tone was most salient in children's categorization performance. We found a preference hierarchy where light‐skinned East Asian people were favored most, and White people least. Color evasion was negatively related to Chinese children's positive attitudes toward White people. The findings emphasize the importance of social contexts in shaping children's racial attitudes.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"37 3","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135476717","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's interpretations of parental responses to children's negative emotions in Chinese urban and rural communities 中国城乡儿童对父母消极情绪反应的解读
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12723
Ruyi Ding, Wei He, Tuo Liu, Junhao Pan, Qian Wang
Abstract Children's interpretations of parenting behaviors offer valuable insights into cultural meanings of parenting. This study examined how Chinese urban and rural children interpreted six different types of parental responses to children's negative emotions (PRCNE), which have traditionally been identified as supportive (e.g., emotion‐focused responses, problem‐focused responses, and expressive encouragement ) versus nonsupportive ones (e.g., minimization, parental distress, punitive responses ) in Western cultures. Based on surveyed samples of 976 children, demographically matched samples of 102 urban ( M age = 14.23 years) and 100 rural ( M age = 14.38 years) children were generated for analysis, using propensity score matching (PSM). Results revealed that compared with rural children, urban children rated problem‐focused responses as more normative, whereas parental distress and punitive responses as less normative. Additionally, urban children evaluated emotion‐focused responses , problem‐focused responses , expressive encouragement , and minimization as less negative, and parental distress as less positive than rural children. In urban communities, emotion‐focused responses , problem‐focused responses , and encouragement were evaluated most positively and least negatively, followed by minimization , and lastly parental distress and punitive responses . In rural communities, emotion‐focused responses and problem‐focused responses were evaluated most positively and least negatively, followed by encouragement and minimization , and lastly parental distress and punitive responses . The findings highlight the diverse interpretations children have towards PRCNE across different cultural contexts.
儿童对父母行为的解读为父母教养的文化意义提供了宝贵的见解。本研究考察了中国城市和农村儿童如何理解父母对儿童消极情绪的六种不同类型的反应(PRCNE),这些反应在西方文化中传统上被认为是支持性的(如情绪聚焦反应、问题聚焦反应和表达性鼓励),而非支持性的(如最小化、父母痛苦、惩罚性反应)。在976名儿童调查样本的基础上,采用倾向得分匹配(PSM)方法,生成102名城市儿童(M年龄= 14.23岁)和100名农村儿童(M年龄= 14.38岁)的人口统计学匹配样本进行分析。结果显示,与农村儿童相比,城市儿童认为以问题为中心的反应更规范,而父母的痛苦和惩罚反应则不那么规范。此外,与农村儿童相比,城市儿童对情感聚焦反应、问题聚焦反应、表达性鼓励和最小化反应的负面评价较低,对父母痛苦的正面评价较低。在城市社区中,以情绪为中心的反应、以问题为中心的反应和鼓励被评价为最积极和最不消极,其次是最小化反应,最后是父母痛苦和惩罚反应。在农村社区,以情绪为中心的反应和以问题为中心的反应被评价为最积极和最不消极,其次是鼓励和最小化,最后是父母的痛苦和惩罚反应。研究结果强调了不同文化背景下儿童对PRCNE的不同理解。
{"title":"Children's interpretations of parental responses to children's negative emotions in Chinese urban and rural communities","authors":"Ruyi Ding, Wei He, Tuo Liu, Junhao Pan, Qian Wang","doi":"10.1111/sode.12723","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12723","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Children's interpretations of parenting behaviors offer valuable insights into cultural meanings of parenting. This study examined how Chinese urban and rural children interpreted six different types of parental responses to children's negative emotions (PRCNE), which have traditionally been identified as supportive (e.g., emotion‐focused responses, problem‐focused responses, and expressive encouragement ) versus nonsupportive ones (e.g., minimization, parental distress, punitive responses ) in Western cultures. Based on surveyed samples of 976 children, demographically matched samples of 102 urban ( M age = 14.23 years) and 100 rural ( M age = 14.38 years) children were generated for analysis, using propensity score matching (PSM). Results revealed that compared with rural children, urban children rated problem‐focused responses as more normative, whereas parental distress and punitive responses as less normative. Additionally, urban children evaluated emotion‐focused responses , problem‐focused responses , expressive encouragement , and minimization as less negative, and parental distress as less positive than rural children. In urban communities, emotion‐focused responses , problem‐focused responses , and encouragement were evaluated most positively and least negatively, followed by minimization , and lastly parental distress and punitive responses . In rural communities, emotion‐focused responses and problem‐focused responses were evaluated most positively and least negatively, followed by encouragement and minimization , and lastly parental distress and punitive responses . The findings highlight the diverse interpretations children have towards PRCNE across different cultural contexts.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"13 2","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135480251","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Mother‐child bidirectional influences in the development of concern for others: Disentangling positive parenting in two predominantly white, North American Samples 母子对他人关怀发展的双向影响:在两个以白人为主的北美样本中解开积极养育的纠缠
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12721
Lindsey C. Partington, Paul D. Hastings
Abstract Mother's positive parenting predicts children's development of concern for others; however, it is unclear which distinct positive parenting behaviors contribute to children's concern for others. We examined the bidirectional associations between mothers’ warmth and reasoning and children's concern toward an adult in distress at 4 and 6 years. We tested these associations in two independent samples with parallel methods, a U.S. community sample (Study 1, N = 83, 44% female, 73.6% White, median income range = $75,000–$90,000 USD) and a Canadian sample at risk for externalizing problems (Study 2, N = 98, 50% female, 82.7% White, median income range = $70,000–$80,000 CND). Child gender and externalizing problems were examined as moderators of these bidirectional socialization processes. In Study 1, a cross‐lagged model (CLM) found that maternal warmth positively predicted children's concern for others over 2 years, whereas children's concern for others inversely predicted future maternal reasoning. Multigroup comparisons found these lagged effects were unique to boys only. Study 2 partially replicated Study 1, revealing fully bidirectional socialization effects unique to boys. Maternal reasoning positively predicted the development of boys’ concern for others over 2 years, and boys’ greater concern for others at age 4 elicited greater maternal reasoning over 2 years. Maternal warmth positively predicted concern for others only for children with elevated externalizing problems. These findings support a differentiated approach to positive parenting research, revealing that distinct parenting behaviors may meet individual child needs uniquely.
母亲积极的养育方式对儿童关心他人的发展有预测作用;然而,目前尚不清楚哪些独特的积极的父母行为有助于孩子关心他人。我们研究了母亲的温暖和推理与儿童在4岁和6岁时对处于困境的成年人的关注之间的双向关联。我们用平行方法在两个独立样本中检验了这些关联,一个是美国社区样本(研究1,N = 83, 44%女性,73.6%白人,收入中位数范围= 75,000 - 90,000美元),另一个是有外部化问题风险的加拿大样本(研究2,N = 98, 50%女性,82.7%白人,收入中位数范围= 70,000 - 80,000加元)。儿童性别和外化问题被视为这些双向社会化过程的调节因素。在研究1中,交叉滞后模型(cross - lag model, CLM)发现母亲的温暖正向预测儿童对他人的关心,而儿童对他人的关心反向预测未来母亲的推理。多组比较发现,这些滞后效应只存在于男孩身上。研究2部分重复了研究1,揭示了男孩特有的完全双向社会化效应。母亲推理正向预测男孩2岁以上对他人关注的发展,男孩4岁时对他人更大的关注在2岁期间引发了更大的母亲推理。只有外化问题严重的儿童,母亲的温暖才能正向地预测对他人的关心。这些发现支持了积极育儿研究的差异化方法,揭示了不同的育儿行为可能满足孩子个体的独特需求。
{"title":"Mother‐child bidirectional influences in the development of concern for others: Disentangling positive parenting in two predominantly white, North American Samples","authors":"Lindsey C. Partington, Paul D. Hastings","doi":"10.1111/sode.12721","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12721","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Mother's positive parenting predicts children's development of concern for others; however, it is unclear which distinct positive parenting behaviors contribute to children's concern for others. We examined the bidirectional associations between mothers’ warmth and reasoning and children's concern toward an adult in distress at 4 and 6 years. We tested these associations in two independent samples with parallel methods, a U.S. community sample (Study 1, N = 83, 44% female, 73.6% White, median income range = $75,000–$90,000 USD) and a Canadian sample at risk for externalizing problems (Study 2, N = 98, 50% female, 82.7% White, median income range = $70,000–$80,000 CND). Child gender and externalizing problems were examined as moderators of these bidirectional socialization processes. In Study 1, a cross‐lagged model (CLM) found that maternal warmth positively predicted children's concern for others over 2 years, whereas children's concern for others inversely predicted future maternal reasoning. Multigroup comparisons found these lagged effects were unique to boys only. Study 2 partially replicated Study 1, revealing fully bidirectional socialization effects unique to boys. Maternal reasoning positively predicted the development of boys’ concern for others over 2 years, and boys’ greater concern for others at age 4 elicited greater maternal reasoning over 2 years. Maternal warmth positively predicted concern for others only for children with elevated externalizing problems. These findings support a differentiated approach to positive parenting research, revealing that distinct parenting behaviors may meet individual child needs uniquely.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"31 5","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135476051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parent‐child relationship buffers the impact of maternal psychological control on aggression in temperamentally surgent children 亲子关系缓冲了母亲心理控制对性情突发性儿童攻击行为的影响
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-11-07 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12722
Yao Sun, Charissa S. L. Cheah, Craig H. Hart
Children's temperamental surgency is associated with later child behavioral problems. However, the underlying mechanisms linking child surgency and child aggression, such as negative parental control, are relatively understudied. Moreover, the potential protective effect of a close parent‐child relationship on these associations remains untested, particularly among non‐White families. Participants included 259 Chinese American preschoolers (Mage = 4.5 years, SD = .9 years, 50% girls) and their mothers (Mage = 37.9 years, SD = 4.7 years), the present study examined the moderating effects of parent‐child relationship quality on the association between mother‐rated child surgency and teacher‐rated child aggression as mediated by maternal psychological control. Overall, results showed that child surgency was linked positively to maternal psychological control, which, in turn, led to higher levels of child aggression six months later, but only when the parent‐child relationship quality was less optimal. These findings indicate that psychological control is one underlying mechanism linking child temperament and child maladjustment, and that parent‐child relationship quality is an important protective factor. Findings also expand an existing theoretical framework by explicating how these variables are applicable to an Asian American population, suggesting the critical buffering role that parent‐child relationship quality plays. The study findings inform efforts to mitigate the potential negative effect of psychological control in reducing childhood aggression.
儿童的情绪异常与儿童日后的行为问题有关。然而,联系儿童外科手术和儿童攻击的潜在机制,如消极的父母控制,研究相对不足。此外,亲密的亲子关系对这些关联的潜在保护作用尚未得到检验,特别是在非白人家庭中。本研究以259名美籍华裔学龄前儿童(M年龄= 4.5岁,SD = 0.9岁,50%为女孩)及其母亲(M年龄= 37.9岁,SD = 4.7岁)为研究对象,探讨了亲子关系质量在母亲心理控制介导下,对母亲评价的儿童外科手术和教师评价的儿童攻击行为的调节作用。总体而言,研究结果表明,儿童外科手术与母亲的心理控制呈正相关,这反过来又导致六个月后儿童的攻击水平更高,但只有在亲子关系质量不太理想的情况下才会如此。这些研究结果表明,心理控制是儿童气质与儿童适应不良之间的一个潜在机制,而亲子关系质量是一个重要的保护因素。研究结果还扩展了现有的理论框架,解释了这些变量如何适用于亚裔美国人,表明亲子关系质量起着关键的缓冲作用。研究结果为减轻心理控制在减少儿童攻击性方面的潜在负面影响提供了信息。
{"title":"Parent‐child relationship buffers the impact of maternal psychological control on aggression in temperamentally surgent children","authors":"Yao Sun, Charissa S. L. Cheah, Craig H. Hart","doi":"10.1111/sode.12722","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12722","url":null,"abstract":"Children's temperamental surgency is associated with later child behavioral problems. However, the underlying mechanisms linking child surgency and child aggression, such as negative parental control, are relatively understudied. Moreover, the potential protective effect of a close parent‐child relationship on these associations remains untested, particularly among non‐White families. Participants included 259 Chinese American preschoolers (Mage = 4.5 years, SD = .9 years, 50% girls) and their mothers (Mage = 37.9 years, SD = 4.7 years), the present study examined the moderating effects of parent‐child relationship quality on the association between mother‐rated child surgency and teacher‐rated child aggression as mediated by maternal psychological control. Overall, results showed that child surgency was linked positively to maternal psychological control, which, in turn, led to higher levels of child aggression six months later, but only when the parent‐child relationship quality was less optimal. These findings indicate that psychological control is one underlying mechanism linking child temperament and child maladjustment, and that parent‐child relationship quality is an important protective factor. Findings also expand an existing theoretical framework by explicating how these variables are applicable to an Asian American population, suggesting the critical buffering role that parent‐child relationship quality plays. The study findings inform efforts to mitigate the potential negative effect of psychological control in reducing childhood aggression.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"18 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-11-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135476464","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental warmth and guilt induction: Associations with prosocial behaviors and the mediating role of values in Chinese adolescents 父母温暖与内疚感诱导:中国青少年亲社会行为的关联及价值观的中介作用
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12714
Tracy K. Y. Wong, Chiaki Konishi
Abstract Parental guilt induction is thought to promote empathy and social attunement in Chinese cultures, whereas parental warmth is thought to facilitate prosocial development across diverse cultures. However, research on the relative roles of guilt induction and warmth and how they are associated with prosocial behaviors in Chinese communities has been limited. Additionally, the roles of paternal parenting and potential motivational mediators in non‐Western contexts remain largely unexplored. The present study considered both culturally prevalent (guilt induction) and universal (warmth) parenting practices and their associations with other‐ versus self‐oriented prosocial behaviors, as well as the mediating role of adolescents’ values (i.e., self‐transcendence vs. self‐transcendence). Gender and age differences were also examined. Participants were 439 Grades 7–11 adolescents (242 girls) from Hong Kong. Path analyses indicated that self‐transcendence values mediated the link between parental (maternal and paternal) warmth and both forms of prosocial behaviors. Self‐enhancement values mediated the link between maternal guilt induction and self‐oriented prosocial behaviors. Multigroup analyses revealed no significant gender and age differences. Current findings suggest that parental warmth is likely to facilitate prosocial behaviors through the cultivation of self‐transcendence values whereas guilt induction may facilitate values and prosocial behaviors that are more self‐oriented in Chinese contexts.
在中国文化中,父母内疚诱导被认为促进共情和社会协调,而父母温暖被认为促进不同文化中的亲社会发展。然而,在中国社区中,关于内疚诱导和温暖的相对作用及其与亲社会行为之间关系的研究却很少。此外,在非西方背景下,父亲教养和潜在动机中介的作用在很大程度上仍未被探索。本研究考虑了文化上普遍的(内疚诱导)和普遍的(温暖)养育方式及其与他人取向和自我取向亲社会行为的关系,以及青少年价值观的中介作用(即自我超越与自我超越)。性别和年龄的差异也被检查。研究对象为439名香港7至11年级青少年(242名女生)。通径分析表明,自我超越价值观在父母(母亲和父亲)温暖与两种形式的亲社会行为之间起中介作用。自我提升价值观在母亲内疚感诱发与自我导向亲社会行为之间起中介作用。多组分析显示,性别和年龄没有显著差异。目前的研究结果表明,父母的温暖可能通过自我超越价值观的培养来促进亲社会行为,而内疚诱导可能通过自我导向的价值观和亲社会行为来促进中国情境下的亲社会行为。
{"title":"Parental warmth and guilt induction: Associations with prosocial behaviors and the mediating role of values in Chinese adolescents","authors":"Tracy K. Y. Wong, Chiaki Konishi","doi":"10.1111/sode.12714","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12714","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Parental guilt induction is thought to promote empathy and social attunement in Chinese cultures, whereas parental warmth is thought to facilitate prosocial development across diverse cultures. However, research on the relative roles of guilt induction and warmth and how they are associated with prosocial behaviors in Chinese communities has been limited. Additionally, the roles of paternal parenting and potential motivational mediators in non‐Western contexts remain largely unexplored. The present study considered both culturally prevalent (guilt induction) and universal (warmth) parenting practices and their associations with other‐ versus self‐oriented prosocial behaviors, as well as the mediating role of adolescents’ values (i.e., self‐transcendence vs. self‐transcendence). Gender and age differences were also examined. Participants were 439 Grades 7–11 adolescents (242 girls) from Hong Kong. Path analyses indicated that self‐transcendence values mediated the link between parental (maternal and paternal) warmth and both forms of prosocial behaviors. Self‐enhancement values mediated the link between maternal guilt induction and self‐oriented prosocial behaviors. Multigroup analyses revealed no significant gender and age differences. Current findings suggest that parental warmth is likely to facilitate prosocial behaviors through the cultivation of self‐transcendence values whereas guilt induction may facilitate values and prosocial behaviors that are more self‐oriented in Chinese contexts.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"34 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136103352","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Investigating the skills of a preschool leader: A latent profile analysis 调查幼儿领导的技能:一个潜在的侧面分析
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-30 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12716
Lindsey L. Held, Ansley T. Gilpin, Mengya Xia
Abstract Individuals emerge as leaders across the lifespan; however, research investigating early childhood leaders is scant. This study assessed leadership in early childhood ( N = 375) by using latent profile analysis of secondary data to examine how skill profiles are related to preschool leader scores. Skill profiles included scores for executive functioning, emotion regulation, imagination, theory of mind, vocabulary, and reception of prosocial behaviors (likeability). Six distinct profiles emerged with variation of high and low skill groupings. Four profiles with higher skill groupings also scored the highest on the leader construct but were not statistically different from each other. These findings support previous research that suggests that leaders may be identifiable in early childhood, and that there may be more than one pathway to early childhood leadership (i.e., differing skill combinations). With further research, these findings may have implications for curriculum development and the selection of peer models in clinical practice.
个人在一生中都会成为领导者;然而,关于儿童早期领导者的研究很少。本研究通过对二手数据的潜在特征分析来考察技能特征与学前领导得分之间的关系,对儿童早期领导能力(N = 375)进行了评估。技能档案包括执行功能、情绪调节、想象力、心理理论、词汇和接受亲社会行为(亲和力)的分数。六种不同的特征随着高技能和低技能分组的变化而出现。具有较高技能分组的四种概况在领导结构上得分最高,但彼此之间没有统计学差异。这些发现支持了先前的研究,即领导者可能在儿童早期就被识别出来,并且可能有不止一种途径可以达到儿童早期的领导力(即不同的技能组合)。通过进一步的研究,这些发现可能会对课程开发和临床实践中同伴模式的选择产生影响。
{"title":"Investigating the skills of a preschool leader: A latent profile analysis","authors":"Lindsey L. Held, Ansley T. Gilpin, Mengya Xia","doi":"10.1111/sode.12716","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12716","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Individuals emerge as leaders across the lifespan; however, research investigating early childhood leaders is scant. This study assessed leadership in early childhood ( N = 375) by using latent profile analysis of secondary data to examine how skill profiles are related to preschool leader scores. Skill profiles included scores for executive functioning, emotion regulation, imagination, theory of mind, vocabulary, and reception of prosocial behaviors (likeability). Six distinct profiles emerged with variation of high and low skill groupings. Four profiles with higher skill groupings also scored the highest on the leader construct but were not statistically different from each other. These findings support previous research that suggests that leaders may be identifiable in early childhood, and that there may be more than one pathway to early childhood leadership (i.e., differing skill combinations). With further research, these findings may have implications for curriculum development and the selection of peer models in clinical practice.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"294 ","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136023453","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1