首页 > 最新文献

Social Development最新文献

英文 中文
Linguistic but not minimal group membership modulates spontaneous level‐2 perspective interference in 8‐year‐old children 语言而非最小群体成员调节8岁儿童自发的2级视角干扰
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-21 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12719
Lívia Priyanka Elek, Ildikó Király, Réka Pető, Renáta Szücs, Fruzsina Elekes, Katalin Oláh
Abstract This paper presents evidence that social categorization affects spontaneous level‐2 visual perspective taking (L2PT) differently depending on the type of social category in 8‐year‐old. In Experiment 1 ( N = 46), children were paired with same‐age peers, who belonged to the same or a different minimal group. In Experiment 2 ( N = 42) children participated with an adult confederate, who either shared their cultural group membership or was a member of an out‐group (inferred from a linguistic cue, accent). In Experiment 3 ( N = 80), children were acting together with an adult confederate who was at the same time a member of the same or a different minimal group and shared their linguistic membership. This allowed us to investigate how these social categories influence each other. Spontaneous L2PT was not affected by the minimal group manipulation. However, accent weakened L2PT when it implied that the task partner belonged to an out‐group. When both category cues were present, accent—that could be an indicator of shared knowledge attribution—played a more pronounced role in attenuating L2PT. It is argued that social categories that are indicative of the partner's knowledge states but not ad hoc groups influence spontaneous mentalizing.
摘要社会分类对8岁儿童自发性2级视觉视角采取(L2PT)的影响随社会类别的不同而不同。在实验1 (N = 46)中,儿童与同龄同伴配对,他们属于相同或不同的最小组。在实验2 (N = 42)中,儿童与一名成年同谋者一起参与实验,该成年同谋者要么与他们的文化群体成员相同,要么是外群体成员(根据语言线索,口音推断)。在实验3 (N = 80)中,儿童与一个成年同谋者一起行动,该成年同谋者同时是同一或不同最小群体的成员,并分享他们的语言成员资格。这让我们得以研究这些社会类别是如何相互影响的。自发L2PT不受最小组操作的影响。然而,当口音暗示任务伙伴属于外群体时,会削弱L2PT。当两种类别线索都存在时,口音——可能是共享知识归因的一个指标——在减弱L2PT中发挥了更明显的作用。有人认为,表明伴侣的知识状态的社会类别而不是特定群体影响自发的心理化。
{"title":"Linguistic but not minimal group membership modulates spontaneous level‐2 perspective interference in 8‐year‐old children","authors":"Lívia Priyanka Elek, Ildikó Király, Réka Pető, Renáta Szücs, Fruzsina Elekes, Katalin Oláh","doi":"10.1111/sode.12719","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12719","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This paper presents evidence that social categorization affects spontaneous level‐2 visual perspective taking (L2PT) differently depending on the type of social category in 8‐year‐old. In Experiment 1 ( N = 46), children were paired with same‐age peers, who belonged to the same or a different minimal group. In Experiment 2 ( N = 42) children participated with an adult confederate, who either shared their cultural group membership or was a member of an out‐group (inferred from a linguistic cue, accent). In Experiment 3 ( N = 80), children were acting together with an adult confederate who was at the same time a member of the same or a different minimal group and shared their linguistic membership. This allowed us to investigate how these social categories influence each other. Spontaneous L2PT was not affected by the minimal group manipulation. However, accent weakened L2PT when it implied that the task partner belonged to an out‐group. When both category cues were present, accent—that could be an indicator of shared knowledge attribution—played a more pronounced role in attenuating L2PT. It is argued that social categories that are indicative of the partner's knowledge states but not ad hoc groups influence spontaneous mentalizing.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"72 0","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135511830","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Trusting others who vary in consistency between their personal standards and behavior: Differences by age, gender, and honesty trust beliefs 信任那些在个人标准和行为上不一致的人:年龄、性别和诚实信任信仰的差异
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-21 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12717
Ken J. Rotenberg, Becky MacDonald‐Taylor, Rebecca Holland
Abstract Three studies examined age, gender, and trust belief differences in using the consistency principle to judge the trustworthiness of persons who varied in the consistency between their personal standards and behavior. The participants were 78 adults ( Mage = 22 years) in Study 1, 160 children from four age groups (6‐7, 8–9, 10–11, and 12–13, year‐olds) in Study 2, and 46 10–11‐year‐olds in Study 3 ( N = 284). Approximately equal numbers of predominately White males and females participated. They judged the trustworthiness of persons who demonstrated consistency or inconsistency between their personal standards and behavior. It was found that adults, 12–13‐year‐olds, 10–11‐year‐olds, and 10–11‐year‐olds with high honesty trust beliefs in others judged the inconsistent persons as less trustworthy than consistent persons. Those participant samples judged inconsistent persons as untrustworthy and consistent persons as trustworthy. Male adults and 12–13‐year‐olds judged consistent persons as more trustworthy than their female counterparts. There are age trends, gender differences, and differences in honesty trust beliefs in the use of consistency principle to judge the trustworthiness of persons varying in the consistency between their personal standards and behavior.
摘要三项研究考察了年龄、性别和信任信念在判断个人标准和行为一致性方面的差异。研究1的参与者为78名成年人(年龄为22岁),研究2的参与者为160名来自4个年龄组(6 - 7岁、8-9岁、10-11岁和12-13岁)的儿童,研究3的参与者为46名10-11岁的儿童(N = 284)。以白人男性和女性为主的参与人数大致相等。他们判断那些在个人标准和行为上表现一致或不一致的人的可信度。研究发现,对他人具有高诚实信任信念的成年人、12-13岁、10-11岁和10-11岁的儿童认为不一致的人比一致的人更不值得信任。这些参与者认为前后矛盾的人是不值得信任的,而前后一致的人是值得信任的。男性成年人和12-13岁的孩子认为言行一致的人比他们的女性同伴更值得信赖。使用一致性原则判断人的可信度存在年龄趋势、性别差异和诚信信任信念差异,个人标准与行为的一致性存在差异。
{"title":"Trusting others who vary in consistency between their personal standards and behavior: Differences by age, gender, and honesty trust beliefs","authors":"Ken J. Rotenberg, Becky MacDonald‐Taylor, Rebecca Holland","doi":"10.1111/sode.12717","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12717","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Three studies examined age, gender, and trust belief differences in using the consistency principle to judge the trustworthiness of persons who varied in the consistency between their personal standards and behavior. The participants were 78 adults ( Mage = 22 years) in Study 1, 160 children from four age groups (6‐7, 8–9, 10–11, and 12–13, year‐olds) in Study 2, and 46 10–11‐year‐olds in Study 3 ( N = 284). Approximately equal numbers of predominately White males and females participated. They judged the trustworthiness of persons who demonstrated consistency or inconsistency between their personal standards and behavior. It was found that adults, 12–13‐year‐olds, 10–11‐year‐olds, and 10–11‐year‐olds with high honesty trust beliefs in others judged the inconsistent persons as less trustworthy than consistent persons. Those participant samples judged inconsistent persons as untrustworthy and consistent persons as trustworthy. Male adults and 12–13‐year‐olds judged consistent persons as more trustworthy than their female counterparts. There are age trends, gender differences, and differences in honesty trust beliefs in the use of consistency principle to judge the trustworthiness of persons varying in the consistency between their personal standards and behavior.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"102 17","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135513020","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Gender essentialism predicts prejudice against gender nonconformity in two cultural contexts 性别本质主义预测了两种文化背景下对性别不一致的偏见
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12720
Rachel D. Fine, Kristina R. Olson, Selin Gülgöz, Rachel Horton, Susan A. Gelman
Abstract Gender‐nonconforming children face a substantial amount of prejudice, making it important to investigate potential contributing factors. In a correlational study of 253 U.S. Midwestern and Pacific Northwestern 6‐ to 10‐year‐old gender‐conforming children (Age M = 7.95, SD = 1.43; 54% girl, 46% boy; 77% White), we examined how gender essentialism (beliefs that gender is biological, discrete, informative, and immutable) and gender identity essentialism (beliefs that gender identity is immutable) relate to prejudice against gender‐nonconforming children. We also examined whether these associations varied by the child's cultural context (rural, non‐diverse, conservative vs. urban, more diverse, liberal). We found a positive correlation between gender essentialism and prejudice, in both cultural contexts. Additionally, children from the more rural context endorsed more essentialism and expressed more prejudice than did their counterparts from the more urban context. However, we found no differences in children's gender identity essentialism by cultural context and no association with prejudice.
性别不一致的儿童面临着大量的偏见,因此调查潜在的影响因素非常重要。在一项对253名美国中西部和太平洋西北部6 - 10岁性别符合儿童的相关研究中(年龄M = 7.95, SD = 1.43;54%的女孩,46%的男孩;(77%白人),我们研究了性别本质主义(认为性别是生物性的、离散的、信息性的和不可改变的)和性别认同本质主义(认为性别认同是不可改变的)与对性别不符合儿童的偏见之间的关系。我们还研究了这些关联是否因儿童的文化背景(农村、非多样性、保守与城市、更多样化、自由)而异。我们发现,在两种文化背景下,性别本质主义与偏见之间存在正相关关系。此外,来自农村环境的儿童比来自城市环境的儿童更赞同本质主义,表达了更多的偏见。然而,我们发现儿童的性别认同本质主义不受文化背景的影响,也与偏见无关。
{"title":"Gender essentialism predicts prejudice against gender nonconformity in two cultural contexts","authors":"Rachel D. Fine, Kristina R. Olson, Selin Gülgöz, Rachel Horton, Susan A. Gelman","doi":"10.1111/sode.12720","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12720","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Gender‐nonconforming children face a substantial amount of prejudice, making it important to investigate potential contributing factors. In a correlational study of 253 U.S. Midwestern and Pacific Northwestern 6‐ to 10‐year‐old gender‐conforming children (Age M = 7.95, SD = 1.43; 54% girl, 46% boy; 77% White), we examined how gender essentialism (beliefs that gender is biological, discrete, informative, and immutable) and gender identity essentialism (beliefs that gender identity is immutable) relate to prejudice against gender‐nonconforming children. We also examined whether these associations varied by the child's cultural context (rural, non‐diverse, conservative vs. urban, more diverse, liberal). We found a positive correlation between gender essentialism and prejudice, in both cultural contexts. Additionally, children from the more rural context endorsed more essentialism and expressed more prejudice than did their counterparts from the more urban context. However, we found no differences in children's gender identity essentialism by cultural context and no association with prejudice.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"70 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135888884","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Longitudinal associations between pet relationship quality and socio‐emotional functioning in early adolescence 青少年早期宠物关系质量与社会情感功能的纵向关联
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-18 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12718
Megan K. Mueller, Kristina S. Callina, Amanda M. Richer, Linda Charmaraman
Abstract Adolescence is a key developmental period for socio‐emotional skills, and companion animal relationships may be one potential source of emotional support and resilience during this time. This study used longitudinal data from 940 pet‐owning adolescents, collected over four‐time points, from youth in the Northeastern United States. We assessed whether pet relationship quality (indexed by relationship satisfaction, companionship, and emotional disclosure) predicted trajectories of loneliness, social anxiety, and depression. Results indicated that high satisfaction with a pet relationship was associated with more favorable trajectories, but companionship (i.e., regarding the frequency of interacting with the pet) was not related significantly to socio‐emotional functioning. High levels of disclosure to a pet were linked with less favorable trajectories for loneliness and depression, but not related to social anxiety. These results suggest that a pet relationship can, in some cases, be associated positively with socio‐emotional development, but that there is significant complexity in these associations. Families, educators, and practitioners should take a nuanced approach to understanding individual adolescent‐pet relationships as a contextual asset for specific youth.
青春期是社会情感技能发展的关键时期,伴侣动物关系可能是这一时期情感支持和适应能力的潜在来源。这项研究使用了940名养宠物的青少年的纵向数据,收集了四个时间点,来自美国东北部的青少年。我们评估了宠物关系质量(以关系满意度、陪伴和情感表露为指标)是否预测了孤独、社交焦虑和抑郁的轨迹。结果表明,对宠物关系的高满意度与更有利的轨迹相关,但陪伴(即与宠物互动的频率)与社会情感功能没有显著相关性。对宠物的高程度披露与孤独感和抑郁的不利轨迹有关,但与社交焦虑无关。这些结果表明,在某些情况下,宠物关系可能与社会情感发展呈正相关,但这些关系存在显著的复杂性。家庭、教育工作者和从业者应该采取细致入微的方法来理解个别青少年与宠物的关系,作为特定青少年的背景资产。
{"title":"Longitudinal associations between pet relationship quality and socio‐emotional functioning in early adolescence","authors":"Megan K. Mueller, Kristina S. Callina, Amanda M. Richer, Linda Charmaraman","doi":"10.1111/sode.12718","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12718","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Adolescence is a key developmental period for socio‐emotional skills, and companion animal relationships may be one potential source of emotional support and resilience during this time. This study used longitudinal data from 940 pet‐owning adolescents, collected over four‐time points, from youth in the Northeastern United States. We assessed whether pet relationship quality (indexed by relationship satisfaction, companionship, and emotional disclosure) predicted trajectories of loneliness, social anxiety, and depression. Results indicated that high satisfaction with a pet relationship was associated with more favorable trajectories, but companionship (i.e., regarding the frequency of interacting with the pet) was not related significantly to socio‐emotional functioning. High levels of disclosure to a pet were linked with less favorable trajectories for loneliness and depression, but not related to social anxiety. These results suggest that a pet relationship can, in some cases, be associated positively with socio‐emotional development, but that there is significant complexity in these associations. Families, educators, and practitioners should take a nuanced approach to understanding individual adolescent‐pet relationships as a contextual asset for specific youth.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"256 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135888781","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social Development 社会发展
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-16 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12616
Social DevelopmentVolume 32, Issue 4 p. 1115-1116 ISSUE INFORMATIONFree Access Social Development First published: 16 October 2023 https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12616AboutPDF ToolsRequest permissionExport citationAdd to favoritesTrack citation ShareShare Give accessShare full text accessShare full-text accessPlease review our Terms and Conditions of Use and check box below to share full-text version of article.I have read and accept the Wiley Online Library Terms and Conditions of UseShareable LinkUse the link below to share a full-text version of this article with your friends and colleagues. Learn more.Copy URL Share a linkShare onEmailFacebookTwitterLinkedInRedditWechat No abstract is available for this article. Volume32, Issue4November 2023Pages 1115-1116 RelatedInformation
社会发展第32卷第4期p. 1115-1116问题信息免费访问社会发展首次出版:2023年10月16日https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12616AboutPDF ToolsRequest permissionExport citationAdd to favoritesTrack citation ShareShare给予accessShare全文accessShare全文accessShare请查看我们的使用条款和条件,并在下面的复选框中分享文章的全文版本。我已经阅读并接受了Wiley在线图书馆使用共享链接的条款和条件,请使用下面的链接与您的朋友和同事分享本文的全文版本。学习更多的知识。复制URL共享链接共享一个emailfacebooktwitterlinkedinreddit微信本文无摘要vol . 32, issue 4十一月2023页1115-1116
{"title":"Social Development","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/sode.12616","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12616","url":null,"abstract":"Social DevelopmentVolume 32, Issue 4 p. 1115-1116 ISSUE INFORMATIONFree Access Social Development First published: 16 October 2023 https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12616AboutPDF ToolsRequest permissionExport citationAdd to favoritesTrack citation ShareShare Give accessShare full text accessShare full-text accessPlease review our Terms and Conditions of Use and check box below to share full-text version of article.I have read and accept the Wiley Online Library Terms and Conditions of UseShareable LinkUse the link below to share a full-text version of this article with your friends and colleagues. Learn more.Copy URL Share a linkShare onEmailFacebookTwitterLinkedInRedditWechat No abstract is available for this article. Volume32, Issue4November 2023Pages 1115-1116 RelatedInformation","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"34 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136078892","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Family separation from military service and children's externalizing symptoms: Exploring moderation by non‐military spouse employment, family financial stress, marital quality, and the parenting alliance 家庭与兵役分离和儿童外化症状:探索非军人配偶就业、家庭经济压力、婚姻质量和养育联盟的调节作用
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-11 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12713
Sabrina M. Richardson, Jacqueline C. Pflieger, Elizabeth Hisle‐Gorman, Ernestine C. Briggs, John A. Fairbank, Valerie A. Stander
Abstract Military separation is a well‐documented vulnerability point for service members, yet little is known regarding how children fare across this transition. The current study examined 909 military‐connected children from the Millennium Cohort Family Study (Wave 1 M age = 3.88 years, SD = .095) across a 3‐year period to explore whether separation predicted child externalizing symptoms over and above Wave 1 externalizing levels, by comparing separated versus not separated military families over time. We also explored if non‐military spouse employment, financial stress, marital quality, or parenting alliance moderated the relation of separation with child externalizing. Data were collected via a parent‐reported online questionnaire and administrative military records. Results showed that separation was unrelated to externalizing. However, moderation analyses suggested that for those who separated, non‐military spouses’ employment prior to separation was related to less externalizing, whereas the parenting alliance was related to less externalizing only for families who remained in the military. Recommendations include assistance with spouse employment prior to military separation and parenting support throughout military service.
军事分离是服务人员的一个有据可考的脆弱性点,但很少有人知道儿童如何在这种过渡中度过。本研究对来自千禧年队列家庭研究(第1波M年龄= 3.88岁,SD = 0.095)的909名与军队有联系的儿童进行了为期3年的调查,通过比较离散和未离散的军人家庭,探讨分离是否能预测超过第1波外化水平的儿童外化症状。我们还探讨了非军人配偶的就业、经济压力、婚姻质量或养育联盟是否会调节分离与孩子外化的关系。数据通过家长报告的在线问卷和行政军事记录收集。结果表明,分离与外化无关。然而,适度分析表明,对于那些分居的家庭,非军人配偶在分居前的就业与较少的外化有关,而养育联盟只与较少的外化有关,这些外化只与留在军队的家庭有关。建议包括在军人离职前协助配偶就业以及在服兵役期间为子女提供支助。
{"title":"Family separation from military service and children's externalizing symptoms: Exploring moderation by non‐military spouse employment, family financial stress, marital quality, and the parenting alliance","authors":"Sabrina M. Richardson, Jacqueline C. Pflieger, Elizabeth Hisle‐Gorman, Ernestine C. Briggs, John A. Fairbank, Valerie A. Stander","doi":"10.1111/sode.12713","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12713","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Military separation is a well‐documented vulnerability point for service members, yet little is known regarding how children fare across this transition. The current study examined 909 military‐connected children from the Millennium Cohort Family Study (Wave 1 M age = 3.88 years, SD = .095) across a 3‐year period to explore whether separation predicted child externalizing symptoms over and above Wave 1 externalizing levels, by comparing separated versus not separated military families over time. We also explored if non‐military spouse employment, financial stress, marital quality, or parenting alliance moderated the relation of separation with child externalizing. Data were collected via a parent‐reported online questionnaire and administrative military records. Results showed that separation was unrelated to externalizing. However, moderation analyses suggested that for those who separated, non‐military spouses’ employment prior to separation was related to less externalizing, whereas the parenting alliance was related to less externalizing only for families who remained in the military. Recommendations include assistance with spouse employment prior to military separation and parenting support throughout military service.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"49 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136212823","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Early childhood predictors of early school‐age academic skills and resilience among children living in poverty 贫困儿童早期学龄学习技能和适应能力的早期儿童预测因素
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-10-02 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12715
Daniel Ewon Choe, Santiago Barreda, Chardée A. Galán, Frances Gardner, Melvin N. Wilson, Thomas J. Dishion, Daniel S. Shaw
Abstract This longitudinal study of low‐income families tested neighborhood‐, family‐, and child‐centered promotive factors in early childhood, responses to an early family intervention, and their interactions as predictors of school‐entry levels of and early school‐age gains in academic skills. Using a racially‐diverse, low‐income sample ( n = 527) from a randomized controlled trial of the Family Check‐Up (FCU) intervention and Bayesian multilevel regression modeling, we tested whether neighborhood cohesion, positive mother–child engagement, and child self‐regulation in early childhood (ages 2–5 years) and their interactions with FCU group assignment predicted the intercept and slope of academic skills across child age 5, 7.5, and 8.5 years. Higher positive mother–child engagement and child self‐regulation predicted higher academic skills at school entry. An interaction between the FCU intervention and positive mother–child engagement predicted gains in academic skills compared to national norms. The findings suggest the FCU intervention leveraged positive mother–child engagement in early childhood to promote academic skills, offering a potential avenue from which to prevent income achievement gaps before school entry.
本研究对低收入家庭进行了纵向研究,测试了儿童早期以社区、家庭和儿童为中心的促进因素,对早期家庭干预的反应,以及它们之间的相互作用,作为入学水平和早期学龄期学业技能提高的预测因素。使用来自家庭检查(FCU)干预的随机对照试验的种族多样化、低收入样本(n = 527)和贝叶斯多水平回归模型,我们测试了邻里凝聚力、积极的母婴参与和儿童早期(2-5岁)的自我调节及其与FCU组分配的相互作用是否预测了5岁、7.5岁和8.5岁儿童学业技能的截距和斜率。更高的积极的母子参与和儿童自我调节预示着更高的入学学习技能。与国家标准相比,FCU干预和积极的母婴参与之间的相互作用预测了学业技能的提高。研究结果表明,FCU的干预措施在儿童早期利用了积极的母婴参与来提高学业技能,为防止入学前的收入成就差距提供了一条潜在途径。
{"title":"Early childhood predictors of early school‐age academic skills and resilience among children living in poverty","authors":"Daniel Ewon Choe, Santiago Barreda, Chardée A. Galán, Frances Gardner, Melvin N. Wilson, Thomas J. Dishion, Daniel S. Shaw","doi":"10.1111/sode.12715","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12715","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract This longitudinal study of low‐income families tested neighborhood‐, family‐, and child‐centered promotive factors in early childhood, responses to an early family intervention, and their interactions as predictors of school‐entry levels of and early school‐age gains in academic skills. Using a racially‐diverse, low‐income sample ( n = 527) from a randomized controlled trial of the Family Check‐Up (FCU) intervention and Bayesian multilevel regression modeling, we tested whether neighborhood cohesion, positive mother–child engagement, and child self‐regulation in early childhood (ages 2–5 years) and their interactions with FCU group assignment predicted the intercept and slope of academic skills across child age 5, 7.5, and 8.5 years. Higher positive mother–child engagement and child self‐regulation predicted higher academic skills at school entry. An interaction between the FCU intervention and positive mother–child engagement predicted gains in academic skills compared to national norms. The findings suggest the FCU intervention leveraged positive mother–child engagement in early childhood to promote academic skills, offering a potential avenue from which to prevent income achievement gaps before school entry.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"8 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"135899984","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Children's and adolescents’ evaluations of wealth‐related STEM inequality 儿童和青少年对与财富相关的STEM不平等的评价
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12710
Luke McGuire, Christina Marlow, Adam J. Hoffman, Angelina Joy, Fidelia Law, Adam Hartstone‐Rose, Adam Rutland, Mark Winterbottom, Frances Balkwill, Karen P. Burns, Laurence Butler, Grace Fields, Kelly Lynn Mulvey
Abstract The fields of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) are rife with inequalities and under‐representation that have their roots in childhood. While researchers have focused on gender and race/ethnicity as two key dimensions of inequality, less attention has been paid to wealth. To this end, and drawing from the Social Reasoning Development approach, we examined children's and adolescents’ perceptions of STEM ability and access to opportunities as a function of wealth, as well as their desire to rectify such inequalities. Participants ( n = 234: early childhood, n = 70, mean age = 6.33, SD = .79; middle childhood, n = 92, mean age = 8.90, SD = .83 and early adolescence, n = 62, mean age = 12.00; SD = 1.16) in the U.K. (64% White British) and U.S. (40% White/European American) read about two characters, one high‐wealth and one low‐wealth. In early childhood, participants reported that the high‐wealth character would have greater STEM ability and were just as likely to invite either character to take part in a STEM opportunity. By middle childhood, participants were more likely to report equal STEM abilities for both characters and to seek to rectify inequalities by inviting the low‐wealth character to take part in a STEM opportunity. However, older participants reported that peers would still prefer to invite the high‐wealth character. These findings also varied by ethnic group status, with minority status participants rectifying inequalities at a younger age than majority status participants. Together these findings document that children are aware of STEM inequalities based on wealth and, with age, will increasingly seek to rectify these inequalities.
科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)领域充斥着源于童年的不平等和代表性不足。虽然研究人员将性别和种族/民族作为不平等的两个关键维度,但对财富的关注却很少。为此,我们借鉴社会推理发展方法,研究了儿童和青少年对STEM能力和获得机会的看法,以及他们纠正这种不平等的愿望。参与者(n = 234):早期儿童,n = 70,平均年龄= 6.33,SD = 0.79;童年中期,n = 92,平均年龄= 8.90,SD = 0.83;青春期早期,n = 62,平均年龄= 12.00;SD = 1.16),英国(64%的英国白人)和美国(40%的白人/欧洲裔美国人)阅读了两个角色,一个是高财富阶层,一个是低财富阶层。在儿童早期,参与者报告说,高财富的角色会有更大的STEM能力,并且很可能邀请其中任何一个角色参加STEM机会。到了童年中期,参与者更有可能报告两个角色的STEM能力相同,并通过邀请低财富角色参加STEM机会来寻求纠正不平等。然而,年长的参与者报告说,同伴们仍然更愿意邀请富有的角色。这些发现也因种族地位而异,少数族裔参与者比多数族裔参与者更早纠正不平等。这些调查结果表明,儿童意识到基于财富的STEM不平等,随着年龄的增长,他们将越来越多地寻求纠正这些不平等。
{"title":"Children's and adolescents’ evaluations of wealth‐related STEM inequality","authors":"Luke McGuire, Christina Marlow, Adam J. Hoffman, Angelina Joy, Fidelia Law, Adam Hartstone‐Rose, Adam Rutland, Mark Winterbottom, Frances Balkwill, Karen P. Burns, Laurence Butler, Grace Fields, Kelly Lynn Mulvey","doi":"10.1111/sode.12710","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12710","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The fields of science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) are rife with inequalities and under‐representation that have their roots in childhood. While researchers have focused on gender and race/ethnicity as two key dimensions of inequality, less attention has been paid to wealth. To this end, and drawing from the Social Reasoning Development approach, we examined children's and adolescents’ perceptions of STEM ability and access to opportunities as a function of wealth, as well as their desire to rectify such inequalities. Participants ( n = 234: early childhood, n = 70, mean age = 6.33, SD = .79; middle childhood, n = 92, mean age = 8.90, SD = .83 and early adolescence, n = 62, mean age = 12.00; SD = 1.16) in the U.K. (64% White British) and U.S. (40% White/European American) read about two characters, one high‐wealth and one low‐wealth. In early childhood, participants reported that the high‐wealth character would have greater STEM ability and were just as likely to invite either character to take part in a STEM opportunity. By middle childhood, participants were more likely to report equal STEM abilities for both characters and to seek to rectify inequalities by inviting the low‐wealth character to take part in a STEM opportunity. However, older participants reported that peers would still prefer to invite the high‐wealth character. These findings also varied by ethnic group status, with minority status participants rectifying inequalities at a younger age than majority status participants. Together these findings document that children are aware of STEM inequalities based on wealth and, with age, will increasingly seek to rectify these inequalities.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"31 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136061648","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Domain‐general or specific: How is children's understanding of deception socialized? 领域-一般或特定:儿童对欺骗的理解是如何社会化的?
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-22 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12712
Jedediah W. P. Allen, Demet Kara
Abstract The current study investigated parenting influences on children's understanding of lie‐telling in eight different social situations. These social situations clustered into two broad categories that have been assumed in the literature: first, self‐oriented lies that were generally told to benefit the self (e.g., to avoid punishment or gain status); and second, socio‐culturally‐oriented lies that were told for more social reasons (e.g., to create positive affect, maintain modesty or politeness). Two types of parenting variables were also measured. The first concerned more general parenting practices and have been studied in the literature; while the second was about more specific parental deceptive behaviors like lying to your child for their compliance. Participants included 141 Turkish parent‐child dyads aged 7, 9, and 11. All children judged the self‐oriented and the socio‐cultural lies as inappropriate but more so for the self‐oriented ones; further, the socio‐cultural lies were judged less negatively with age. While general parenting practices did not predict children's judgments, for parental deceptive practices, there were strong negative relationships between parents’ use of threatening lies (e.g., “come with me or I'll leave you here”) and children's judgments for both self‐ and socio‐cultural lie types. The two main conclusions are that specific parental deceptive practices are more relevant than general parenting for understanding children's judgments about lie‐telling situations. Second, the assumed categorization of lies into “self‐” and “social‐” seems generally valid when using a plurality of situations. Finally, some issues related to the morality of lie‐telling are discussed.
摘要本研究调查了八种不同社会情境下父母教养对儿童说谎理解的影响。这些社会情境被归纳为文献中假设的两大类:第一,自我导向的谎言,通常是为了让自己受益(例如,为了避免惩罚或获得地位);第二种是社会文化导向的谎言,这些谎言更多是出于社会原因(例如,创造积极的影响,保持谦虚或礼貌)。还测量了两种类型的养育变量。第一种涉及更普遍的育儿实践,已经在文献中进行了研究;而第二个是关于更具体的父母欺骗行为,比如为了孩子的服从而撒谎。参与者包括141对年龄分别为7岁、9岁和11岁的土耳其夫妻。所有的孩子都认为自我导向和社会文化导向的谎言是不合适的,但自我导向的谎言更不合适;此外,随着年龄的增长,人们对社会文化谎言的负面评价也会减少。虽然一般的育儿实践不能预测孩子的判断,但对于父母的欺骗行为,父母使用威胁性谎言(例如,“跟我来,否则我就把你留在这里”)与孩子对自我和社会文化谎言类型的判断之间存在强烈的负相关。两个主要结论是,特定的父母欺骗行为比一般的父母行为更能理解孩子对说谎情况的判断。其次,假设谎言分为“自我-”和“社会-”,在使用多种情况时似乎普遍有效。最后,讨论了与说谎道德有关的一些问题。
{"title":"Domain‐general or specific: How is children's understanding of deception socialized?","authors":"Jedediah W. P. Allen, Demet Kara","doi":"10.1111/sode.12712","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12712","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract The current study investigated parenting influences on children's understanding of lie‐telling in eight different social situations. These social situations clustered into two broad categories that have been assumed in the literature: first, self‐oriented lies that were generally told to benefit the self (e.g., to avoid punishment or gain status); and second, socio‐culturally‐oriented lies that were told for more social reasons (e.g., to create positive affect, maintain modesty or politeness). Two types of parenting variables were also measured. The first concerned more general parenting practices and have been studied in the literature; while the second was about more specific parental deceptive behaviors like lying to your child for their compliance. Participants included 141 Turkish parent‐child dyads aged 7, 9, and 11. All children judged the self‐oriented and the socio‐cultural lies as inappropriate but more so for the self‐oriented ones; further, the socio‐cultural lies were judged less negatively with age. While general parenting practices did not predict children's judgments, for parental deceptive practices, there were strong negative relationships between parents’ use of threatening lies (e.g., “come with me or I'll leave you here”) and children's judgments for both self‐ and socio‐cultural lie types. The two main conclusions are that specific parental deceptive practices are more relevant than general parenting for understanding children's judgments about lie‐telling situations. Second, the assumed categorization of lies into “self‐” and “social‐” seems generally valid when using a plurality of situations. Finally, some issues related to the morality of lie‐telling are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"92 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136061364","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How bad is it to eat an intelligent chicken? Children's judgments of eating animals are less ‘self‐serving’ than adults 吃一只聪明的鸡有多糟糕?与成年人相比,孩子们对吃动物的判断不那么“自私”
4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2023-09-14 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12709
Heather Henseler Kozachenko, Jared Piazza
Abstract Research shows that adult meat eaters strategically distort or disregard information about animals (e.g., their intelligence) that is problematic for meat consumption. However, the development of such behaviours is not well understood. Two studies tested whether primary‐school‐age children exhibit motivated use of information about food animals as adults do ( N = 148 children, 410 adults). Using experimental methods that manipulated participants’ perceptions of the intelligence (high vs. low) of food animals versus non‐food animals (Study 1) and the perspective taken (self vs. other; Study 2), it was found that, compared to adult omnivores, children tend to hold stronger moral views about the wrongness of harming animals to use as food. Only adults exhibited motivated non‐use of intelligence information and self‐other distinctions in their moral‐concern judgments. Children's judgments of eating animals did not exhibit the strategic, self‐serving processes characteristic of adult meat eaters. Psychological explanations for these developmental differences are discussed.
研究表明,成年肉食者策略性地歪曲或无视动物的信息(例如,它们的智力),这对肉类消费有问题。然而,这种行为的发展并没有得到很好的理解。两项研究测试了小学适龄儿童是否会像成年人一样主动使用有关食用动物的信息(N = 148名儿童,410名成年人)。使用实验方法操纵参与者对食用动物与非食用动物的智力感知(高与低)(研究1)和所采取的视角(自我与他人;研究2)发现,与成年杂食动物相比,儿童倾向于对伤害动物作为食物的错误持有更强烈的道德观。只有成年人在道德关注判断中表现出动机性不使用智力信息和自我-他人差异。儿童对食用动物的判断并没有表现出成年肉食者所特有的策略性、自私自利的过程。对这些发展差异的心理学解释进行了讨论。
{"title":"How bad is it to eat an intelligent chicken? Children's judgments of eating animals are less ‘self‐serving’ than adults","authors":"Heather Henseler Kozachenko, Jared Piazza","doi":"10.1111/sode.12709","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12709","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract Research shows that adult meat eaters strategically distort or disregard information about animals (e.g., their intelligence) that is problematic for meat consumption. However, the development of such behaviours is not well understood. Two studies tested whether primary‐school‐age children exhibit motivated use of information about food animals as adults do ( N = 148 children, 410 adults). Using experimental methods that manipulated participants’ perceptions of the intelligence (high vs. low) of food animals versus non‐food animals (Study 1) and the perspective taken (self vs. other; Study 2), it was found that, compared to adult omnivores, children tend to hold stronger moral views about the wrongness of harming animals to use as food. Only adults exhibited motivated non‐use of intelligence information and self‐other distinctions in their moral‐concern judgments. Children's judgments of eating animals did not exhibit the strategic, self‐serving processes characteristic of adult meat eaters. Psychological explanations for these developmental differences are discussed.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"358 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-09-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"134911045","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1