首页 > 最新文献

Social Development最新文献

英文 中文
Loneliness in Indonesian adolescents: Associations with quantity and quality of friendship and status within and between peer groups 印度尼西亚青少年的孤独感:友谊的数量和质量与同龄群体内部和之间的地位的关系
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-11 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12771
Keqin Zhang, Urip Purwono, Doran C. French
Loneliness is a perceived discrepancy between desired and experienced social relationships that may arise from lack of intimate attachment to another person (e.g., friend) or lack of involvement in larger networks (e.g., peer groups). This study assessed how multiple aspects of friendship and peer group involvement were associated with Indonesian adolescents’ self‐reported loneliness. Participants were 754 twelfth‐grade Indonesian students (413 girls, Mage = 16.5 years). Reciprocated friendships were calculated from within‐grade nominations and both dyadic members rated the quality of their friendship. Peer groups were generated from Social Cognitive Mapping (SCM) and within‐group centrality and group visibility were determined using SCM. Group popularity was derived using peer nominations. A Structural Equation Model revealed that multiple aspects of friendship and group involvement, except for friend‐rated friendship quality, were concurrently associated with loneliness. An interaction between within‐group centrality and group popularity was found for boys, suggesting that being central in a group may be more important for boys in low‐status than those in high‐status groups. The results revealed that quantity and quality of friendship as well as position in the peer group and status of the peer group were associated with loneliness. The comparable associations of loneliness with aspects of friendship and group involvement may be a function of Indonesian culture in which both group involvement and friendships are valued.
孤独感是一种期望的社会关系与经历的社会关系之间存在差异的感觉,这种差异可能源于缺乏与他人(如朋友)的亲密依恋,或缺乏对更大网络(如同伴团体)的参与。本研究评估了友谊和朋辈群体参与的多个方面与印尼青少年自我报告的孤独感之间的关系。研究对象为 754 名印度尼西亚十二年级学生(413 名女生,年龄 = 16.5 岁)。互惠友谊是根据年级内的提名计算得出的,双向成员对其友谊的质量进行评分。同辈群体由社会认知图谱(Social Cognitive Mapping,SCM)生成,群体内中心度和群体可见度由 SCM 确定。群体受欢迎程度是通过同伴提名得出的。结构方程模型显示,除朋友评价的友谊质量外,友谊和群体参与的多个方面同时与孤独感相关。研究发现,男孩在群体中的中心地位与群体受欢迎程度之间存在交互作用,这表明,对于地位较低的男孩来说,在群体中处于中心地位可能比在地位较高的群体中更为重要。研究结果表明,友谊的数量和质量以及在同伴群体中的位置和同伴群体的地位都与孤独感有关。孤独感与友谊和群体参与方面的相似性可能是印尼文化的一种功能,在印尼文化中,群体参与和友谊都很重要。
{"title":"Loneliness in Indonesian adolescents: Associations with quantity and quality of friendship and status within and between peer groups","authors":"Keqin Zhang, Urip Purwono, Doran C. French","doi":"10.1111/sode.12771","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12771","url":null,"abstract":"Loneliness is a perceived discrepancy between desired and experienced social relationships that may arise from lack of intimate attachment to another person (e.g., friend) or lack of involvement in larger networks (e.g., peer groups). This study assessed how multiple aspects of friendship and peer group involvement were associated with Indonesian adolescents’ self‐reported loneliness. Participants were 754 twelfth‐grade Indonesian students (413 girls, Mage = 16.5 years). Reciprocated friendships were calculated from within‐grade nominations and both dyadic members rated the quality of their friendship. Peer groups were generated from Social Cognitive Mapping (SCM) and within‐group centrality and group visibility were determined using SCM. Group popularity was derived using peer nominations. A Structural Equation Model revealed that multiple aspects of friendship and group involvement, except for friend‐rated friendship quality, were concurrently associated with loneliness. An interaction between within‐group centrality and group popularity was found for boys, suggesting that being central in a group may be more important for boys in low‐status than those in high‐status groups. The results revealed that quantity and quality of friendship as well as position in the peer group and status of the peer group were associated with loneliness. The comparable associations of loneliness with aspects of friendship and group involvement may be a function of Indonesian culture in which both group involvement and friendships are valued.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"30 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207694","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Developmental differences in young children's implied use of cognitive resources in their self‐regulation strategies 幼儿在自我调节策略中暗示使用认知资源的发展差异
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12769
Gabrielle Sky Cardwell, Pamela M. Cole, Brooke Weaver, Jenna M. Leadbeater, Erika S. Lunkenheimer, Kristin A. Buss, Lisa Gatzke‐Kopp, Nilam Ram
The emergence of self‐regulation during the preschool years is due, in part, to children's development of cognitive resources that can regulate their behavior. However, there is little direct evidence that age influences the extent to which young children's strategies involve such resources. We investigated age differences in the extent that young children's strategies imply cognitive resources. A sample of 154 children (77 girls; M age 45.15 months, range 30–60 months) and mothers from middle‐class families (M income = $89,541) in a small mid‐Atlantic American city (94.2% White). They participated in a 9‐min delayed reward task in which mothers told children they must wait to open a gift; children were reminded every 3 min. The latency, frequency, and average duration of the extent to which children's strategies implied cognitive resources were examined in relation to task time and age. In line with Kopp's framework, results from multilevel models indicated older preschool‐age children engaged strategies implying higher‐level cognitive resources more quickly and frequently, but not longer than younger children. Regardless of age, children engaged cognitive resources more quickly, more often, and longer in the first 3 min of the task than later in the task, suggesting that such engagement was not sustained. The findings are discussed in terms of both the emergence and complexities of regulatory strategy development.
学龄前儿童自我调节能力的出现,部分原因是儿童发展了能够调节其行为的认知资源。然而,几乎没有直接证据表明年龄会影响幼儿的策略在多大程度上涉及这些资源。我们研究了幼儿策略在认知资源方面的年龄差异。样本中有 154 名儿童(77 名女孩;平均年龄 45.15 个月,30-60 个月不等)和母亲来自美国大西洋中部一个小城市的中产阶级家庭(平均收入 = 89,541 美元)(94.2% 为白人)。他们参加了一项 9 分钟的延迟奖励任务,在该任务中,母亲告诉儿童他们必须等待才能打开礼物;每隔 3 分钟提醒儿童一次。我们根据任务时间和年龄,研究了儿童的策略在多大程度上意味着认知资源的延迟时间、频率和平均持续时间。与科普的框架一致,多层次模型的结果表明,年龄较大的学龄前儿童比年龄较小的儿童更快、更频繁地使用暗示高层次认知资源的策略,但持续时间并不长。无论年龄大小,儿童在任务的前 3 分钟比任务的后 3 分钟更快、更频繁、更长时间地使用认知资源,这表明这种使用并不是持续的。本文从调控策略发展的出现和复杂性两方面对研究结果进行了讨论。
{"title":"Developmental differences in young children's implied use of cognitive resources in their self‐regulation strategies","authors":"Gabrielle Sky Cardwell, Pamela M. Cole, Brooke Weaver, Jenna M. Leadbeater, Erika S. Lunkenheimer, Kristin A. Buss, Lisa Gatzke‐Kopp, Nilam Ram","doi":"10.1111/sode.12769","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12769","url":null,"abstract":"The emergence of self‐regulation during the preschool years is due, in part, to children's development of cognitive resources that can regulate their behavior. However, there is little direct evidence that age influences the extent to which young children's strategies involve such resources. We investigated age differences in the extent that young children's strategies imply cognitive resources. A sample of 154 children (77 girls; <jats:italic>M</jats:italic> age 45.15 months, range 30–60 months) and mothers from middle‐class families (<jats:italic>M</jats:italic> income = $89,541) in a small mid‐Atlantic American city (94.2% White). They participated in a 9‐min delayed reward task in which mothers told children they must wait to open a gift; children were reminded every 3 min. The latency, frequency, and average duration of the extent to which children's strategies implied cognitive resources were examined in relation to task time and age. In line with Kopp's framework, results from multilevel models indicated older preschool‐age children engaged strategies implying higher‐level cognitive resources more quickly and frequently, but not longer than younger children. Regardless of age, children engaged cognitive resources more quickly, more often, and longer in the first 3 min of the task than later in the task, suggesting that such engagement was not sustained. The findings are discussed in terms of both the emergence and complexities of regulatory strategy development.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"10 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207689","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Judgements of identity claims vary for monoracial and biracial people 单种族人和双种族人对身份主张的判断各不相同
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12770
Elizabeth A. Quinn‐Jensen, Zoe Liberman
Despite increasing racial diversity in the United States, and the particular growth of multiracial populations, questions about how children perceive others’ (bi)racial identities remain poorly understood. In two preregistered studies, we asked White and racially minoritized American children (N = 157; 4–11‐years old) and White and multiracial adults (N = 226) how acceptable it was for monoracial people (Black or White; Study 1) and/or biracial people (Black–White; Studies 1 and 2) to claim either a monoracial or biracial identity. Consistent with past research with adults, children said that monoracial people should claim (only) the monoracial identity which matched their ancestry. Judgements about biracial identity were more variable. White and multiracial adults (Study 2) reported that biracial targets could claim a racial identity that matched either or both of their parents, with biracial claims being evaluated most positively. Exploratory analyses on children's judgements about biracial people's identity claims (Study 1) revealed different patterns of development for White children and children from minoritized backgrounds. Whereas White children became more likely with age to report that all identity claims were acceptable, children from racially minoritized groups became more likely with age to endorse biracial targets who claimed a biracial identity. These findings suggest that children's own racial background and age may have a larger impact on their perceptions of biracial people's identities, compared to their perceptions of monoracial people's identities.
尽管美国的种族多样性日益增加,多种族人口增长尤为明显,但人们对儿童如何看待他人的(双)种族身份仍然知之甚少。在两项预先登记的研究中,我们询问了白人和种族少数化的美国儿童(人数=157;4-11 岁)以及白人和多种族成人(人数=226)对单种族人(黑人或白人;研究 1)和/或双种族人(黑人-白人;研究 1 和 2)声称自己是单种族人或双种族人的接受程度。与过去对成人的研究一致,儿童认为单种族人应该(只)声称与其祖先相符的单种族身份。对双种族身份的判断则变化较大。白人和多种族成人(研究 2)表示,双种族目标可以声称自己的种族身份与其父母一方或双方相匹配,而双种族声称得到的评价最为积极。对儿童对双种族人身份主张的判断进行的探索性分析(研究 1)显示,白人儿童和来自少数民族背景的儿童有不同的发展模式。随着年龄的增长,白人儿童更有可能认为所有的身份主张都是可以接受的,而来自少数种族群体的儿童则更有可能认可那些声称自己是双种族身份的双种族目标。这些研究结果表明,与对单种族人身份的看法相比,儿童自身的种族背景和年龄可能会对他们对双种族人身份的看法产生更大的影响。
{"title":"Judgements of identity claims vary for monoracial and biracial people","authors":"Elizabeth A. Quinn‐Jensen, Zoe Liberman","doi":"10.1111/sode.12770","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12770","url":null,"abstract":"Despite increasing racial diversity in the United States, and the particular growth of multiracial populations, questions about how children perceive others’ (bi)racial identities remain poorly understood. In two preregistered studies, we asked White and racially minoritized American children (<jats:italic>N </jats:italic>= 157; 4–11‐years old) and White and multiracial adults (<jats:italic>N </jats:italic>= 226) how acceptable it was for monoracial people (Black or White; Study 1) and/or biracial people (Black–White; Studies 1 and 2) to claim either a monoracial or biracial identity. Consistent with past research with adults, children said that monoracial people should claim (only) the monoracial identity which matched their ancestry. Judgements about biracial identity were more variable. White and multiracial adults (Study 2) reported that biracial targets could claim a racial identity that matched either or both of their parents, with biracial claims being evaluated most positively. Exploratory analyses on children's judgements about biracial people's identity claims (Study 1) revealed different patterns of development for White children and children from minoritized backgrounds. Whereas White children became more likely with age to report that all identity claims were acceptable, children from racially minoritized groups became more likely with age to endorse biracial targets who claimed a biracial identity. These findings suggest that children's own racial background and age may have a larger impact on their perceptions of biracial people's identities, compared to their perceptions of monoracial people's identities.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207690","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Paternal activation parenting and growth in children's inhibitory control across early childhood 父亲激活式养育与幼儿期儿童抑制控制能力的增长
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12768
Julia S. Feldman, Melvin N. Wilson, Daniel S. Shaw
Activation parenting (AP) is a parenting construct derived from research and theory on paternal caregiving that includes behaviors that challenge children to approach novel situations, explore their environments, and take physical and socioemotional risks through a balance of encouragement and limit‐setting. Although components of AP have been linked to different domains of children's self‐regulation skills, comprehensive measures of AP and longitudinal research on families from low socioeconomic backgrounds are lacking. These limitations greatly constrain our understanding of the potential benefits of paternal AP for children's self‐regulation development, including the maturation of inhibitory control (IC) in early childhood. Thus, the present study tested associations between paternal AP at age 3 and growth in parent‐reported IC across ages 3–5 in a sample of low‐income, ethnically diverse fathers. Participating fathers (N = 171; 9% Black, 47% White, 8% Latinx, 61% not Latinx; mean household income = $25,145) and their children (51% female) were drawn from the Early Steps Multisite Study. AP during a clean‐up task at child age 3 years, but not a teaching task, was associated with greater growth in IC across ages 3–5 years. Implications of the current findings are presented for understanding associations between paternal AP and children's IC and broader self‐regulation skills with diverse samples of caregivers.
激活型养育(AP)是一种养育方式,它源于有关父亲照料的研究和理论,包括通过鼓励和限制的平衡来挑战儿童接近新情况、探索环境以及承担身体和社会情感风险的行为。虽然 "AP "的组成部分与儿童自我调节技能的不同领域相关,但目前还缺乏对 "AP "的全面测量,也缺乏对社会经济背景较差的家庭的纵向研究。这些局限性极大地限制了我们对父亲自律行为对儿童自我调节能力发展的潜在益处的理解,包括对儿童早期抑制控制(IC)成熟的理解。因此,本研究以低收入、种族多元化的父亲为样本,测试了3岁时父亲的AP与3-5岁时父母报告的IC增长之间的关联。参与研究的父亲(人数=171;9%为黑人,47%为白人,8%为拉丁裔,61%为非拉丁裔;平均家庭收入=25,145 美元)及其子女(51%为女性)均来自 "早期阶梯多站点研究"(Early Steps Multisite Study)。在儿童 3 岁时,清理任务中的 "AP",而非教学任务中的 "AP",与 IC 在 3-5 岁期间的更大增长相关。本研究结果的意义在于,通过不同的照顾者样本,了解父亲的参与与儿童的集成电路和更广泛的自我调节技能之间的关系。
{"title":"Paternal activation parenting and growth in children's inhibitory control across early childhood","authors":"Julia S. Feldman, Melvin N. Wilson, Daniel S. Shaw","doi":"10.1111/sode.12768","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12768","url":null,"abstract":"Activation parenting (AP) is a parenting construct derived from research and theory on paternal caregiving that includes behaviors that challenge children to approach novel situations, explore their environments, and take physical and socioemotional risks through a balance of encouragement and limit‐setting. Although components of AP have been linked to different domains of children's self‐regulation skills, comprehensive measures of AP and longitudinal research on families from low socioeconomic backgrounds are lacking. These limitations greatly constrain our understanding of the potential benefits of paternal AP for children's self‐regulation development, including the maturation of inhibitory control (IC) in early childhood. Thus, the present study tested associations between paternal AP at age 3 and growth in parent‐reported IC across ages 3–5 in a sample of low‐income, ethnically diverse fathers. Participating fathers (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 171; 9% Black, 47% White, 8% Latinx, 61% not Latinx; mean household income = $25,145) and their children (51% female) were drawn from the Early Steps Multisite Study. AP during a clean‐up task at child age 3 years, but not a teaching task, was associated with greater growth in IC across ages 3–5 years. Implications of the current findings are presented for understanding associations between paternal AP and children's IC and broader self‐regulation skills with diverse samples of caregivers.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"16 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Revisiting the seminal studies of attachment formation and reevaluating what it means to become attached 重温关于依恋形成的开创性研究,重新评估依恋的含义
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-09-04 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12765
Harry Freeman
What does it mean to become attached? Three longitudinal studies established the empirical basis for the existing four‐phase model of attachment formation, a model that has remained unmodified and unexamined for over half a century. In this paper, I revisit the research questions, methods, and findings from the seminal studies to reevaluate the current model. The evidence indicates two distinct definitions of attachment onset. In the first two phases of the model, becoming attached is defined by changes in within‐subject behaviors leading to the selection of a discriminated figure. Defined this way, attachment onset is analogous to how bond formation is currently defined in other mammals and how imprinting is understood in birds. In contrast, the third and fourth phases of the model define attachment onset by forming a goal‐corrected relationship. This second definition is human‐centric and relies on secure base behavior as the signature criterion, the same criteria used to classify secure and insecure patterns of attachment. I argue for a narrower definition of attachment by removing goal‐corrected behavior as a criterion and focusing on the normative process of selection. In addition, I integrate contemporary work on pair bonding in humans and other animals to propose new avenues for conceptualizing and studying attachment formation in infancy and beyond in filial and sexual bonds.
依恋意味着什么?三项纵向研究为现有的依恋形成四阶段模型奠定了实证基础,半个多世纪以来,这一模型一直没有得到修改和检验。在本文中,我将重新审视这些开创性研究的研究问题、方法和结果,以重新评估当前的模型。证据表明,依恋开始有两种不同的定义。在该模型的前两个阶段,依恋的产生是由主体内行为的变化所决定的,而主体内行为的变化会导致选择一个受歧视的形象。按照这种定义,依恋的开始类似于目前其他哺乳动物对纽带形成的定义以及鸟类对印记的理解。与此相反,该模型的第三和第四阶段是通过形成一种目标校正关系来定义依恋开始的。第二个定义是以人类为中心的,依赖于安全的基础行为作为标志性标准,这与用于划分安全和不安全依恋模式的标准相同。我主张对依恋进行更狭义的定义,不再将目标校正行为作为标准,而将重点放在规范的选择过程上。此外,我还整合了当代人类和其他动物的配对结合研究成果,为婴儿期及以后的孝道和性关系中依恋的形成的概念化和研究提出了新的途径。
{"title":"Revisiting the seminal studies of attachment formation and reevaluating what it means to become attached","authors":"Harry Freeman","doi":"10.1111/sode.12765","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12765","url":null,"abstract":"What does it mean to become attached? Three longitudinal studies established the empirical basis for the existing four‐phase model of attachment formation, a model that has remained unmodified and unexamined for over half a century. In this paper, I revisit the research questions, methods, and findings from the seminal studies to reevaluate the current model. The evidence indicates two distinct definitions of attachment onset. In the first two phases of the model, becoming attached is defined by changes in within‐subject behaviors leading to the selection of a discriminated figure. Defined this way, attachment onset is analogous to how bond formation is currently defined in other mammals and how imprinting is understood in birds. In contrast, the third and fourth phases of the model define attachment onset by forming a goal‐corrected relationship. This second definition is human‐centric and relies on secure base behavior as the signature criterion, the same criteria used to classify secure and insecure patterns of attachment. I argue for a narrower definition of attachment by removing goal‐corrected behavior as a criterion and focusing on the normative process of selection. In addition, I integrate contemporary work on pair bonding in humans and other animals to propose new avenues for conceptualizing and studying attachment formation in infancy and beyond in filial and sexual bonds.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-09-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207737","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Social reward predicts false belief understanding in Namibian Hai||om children 社会奖励可预测纳米比亚海||om 儿童对错误信念的理解
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-26 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12767
Roman Stengelin, Ljubica Petrović, Maleen Thiele, Robert Hepach, Daniel B. M. Haun
Social motivation is theorized to promote Theory of Mind development in childhood, but research testing this link is scarce and largely limited to urban middle‐class milieus of the Global North. Here, we investigated the link between social motivation (i.e., social reward responsivity) and Theory of Mind (i.e., false belief understanding) among N = 59 Hai||om children (AgeRange = 2.3–8.0 years) from rural Namibia, an indigenous community where children's social experience with peers and adults differs much from urban middle‐class milieus typically sampled in developmental science research. Children's preference for adult, but not peer faces predicted their false belief performance. Moreover, their false belief performance increased with age, showing mastery of the current false belief task by the preschool years. These results accommodate universalist claims on the link between social motivation and Theory of Mind while suggesting a particular contribution of the social reward responsivity to adult social partners.
根据理论,社会动机可促进儿童期心智理论的发展,但检验这种联系的研究却很少,而且主要局限于全球北方的城市中产阶级环境。在这里,我们调查了来自纳米比亚农村的 N = 59 名儿童(年龄范围 = 2.3-8.0 岁)的社交动机(即社交奖赏反应性)与心智理论(即对错误信念的理解)之间的联系。儿童对成人面孔的偏好能预测他们的错误信念表现,而对同伴面孔的偏好不能预测他们的错误信念表现。此外,他们的错误信念表现会随着年龄的增长而提高,这表明他们在学龄前阶段就已经掌握了当前的错误信念任务。这些结果符合关于社会动机与心智理论之间联系的普遍主义主张,同时也表明了社会奖赏反应性对成人社会伙伴的特殊贡献。
{"title":"Social reward predicts false belief understanding in Namibian Hai||om children","authors":"Roman Stengelin, Ljubica Petrović, Maleen Thiele, Robert Hepach, Daniel B. M. Haun","doi":"10.1111/sode.12767","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12767","url":null,"abstract":"Social motivation is theorized to promote Theory of Mind development in childhood, but research testing this link is scarce and largely limited to urban middle‐class milieus of the Global North. Here, we investigated the link between social motivation (i.e., social reward responsivity) and Theory of Mind (i.e., false belief understanding) among <jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 59 Hai||om children (<jats:italic>Age<jats:sub>Range</jats:sub></jats:italic> = 2.3–8.0 years) from rural Namibia, an indigenous community where children's social experience with peers and adults differs much from urban middle‐class milieus typically sampled in developmental science research. Children's preference for adult, but not peer faces predicted their false belief performance. Moreover, their false belief performance increased with age, showing mastery of the current false belief task by the preschool years. These results accommodate universalist claims on the link between social motivation and Theory of Mind while suggesting a particular contribution of the social reward responsivity to adult social partners.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"76 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207692","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Peer attachment in adolescence: What are the individual and relational associated factors? 青春期的同伴依恋:个体和关系的相关因素是什么?
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-23 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12762
Danyka Therriault, Jean‐Pascal Lemelin, Jean Toupin, Michèle Déry
The quality of peer attachment in adolescence is an important determinant of psychosocial adjustment. To date, few clear conclusions can be drawn about the most important factors associated with the quality of peer attachment. This study aimed to identify the most important individual and relational factors associated with peer attachment quality, and to establish their relative contribution. Early adolescents (N = 634, 45.7% girls), 11.29 years on average at the first measurement time, participated in the longitudinal study. Individual and relational factors were measured at T1; parent and peer attachment quality were measured at T2, 2 years later. First, a multiple linear regression analysis was performed using the global score for peer attachment as the outcome variable. Second, a path model, including the three specific attachment dimensions (trust, communication, and alienation), was tested. The invariance of the models by sex was also examined. The analyses showed that peer attachment was associated positively with the adolescent–parent attachment relationship, the adolescent's age, as well as the teacher–student relationship, and associated negatively with the presence of internalizing behavior problems and social rejection. These variables helped explain 21.3% of the total variance. Give or take a few nuances, these same variables were also related to the three specific peer attachment dimensions. Sex differences were observed for the associated factors of these specific dimensions. This study highlights the importance of certain relational variables, in both family and school contexts, as correlates of peer attachment.
青少年时期同伴依恋的质量是社会心理适应的一个重要决定因素。迄今为止,关于与同伴依恋质量相关的最重要因素,还很少有明确的结论。本研究旨在找出与同伴依恋质量相关的最重要的个人和关系因素,并确定它们的相对贡献。参与纵向研究的青少年(人数=634,45.7%为女孩)在首次测量时平均年龄为 11.29 岁。个人和关系因素在第一次测量时进行测量;父母和同伴依恋质量在第二次测量时,即两年后进行测量。首先,以同伴依恋的总体得分作为结果变量,进行了多元线性回归分析。其次,对包括三个具体依恋维度(信任、沟通和疏离)的路径模型进行了测试。此外,还检验了模型在性别方面的不变性。分析表明,同伴依恋与青少年与父母的依恋关系、青少年的年龄以及师生关系呈正相关,而与内化行为问题和社会排斥呈负相关。这些变量解释了 21.3% 的总方差。这些变量也与三个特定的同伴依恋维度有关。在这些特定维度的相关因素中,观察到了性别差异。这项研究强调了家庭和学校环境中某些关系变量作为同伴依恋相关因素的重要性。
{"title":"Peer attachment in adolescence: What are the individual and relational associated factors?","authors":"Danyka Therriault, Jean‐Pascal Lemelin, Jean Toupin, Michèle Déry","doi":"10.1111/sode.12762","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12762","url":null,"abstract":"The quality of peer attachment in adolescence is an important determinant of psychosocial adjustment. To date, few clear conclusions can be drawn about the most important factors associated with the quality of peer attachment. This study aimed to identify the most important individual and relational factors associated with peer attachment quality, and to establish their relative contribution. Early adolescents (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 634, 45.7% girls), 11.29 years on average at the first measurement time, participated in the longitudinal study. Individual and relational factors were measured at T1; parent and peer attachment quality were measured at T2, 2 years later. First, a multiple linear regression analysis was performed using the global score for peer attachment as the outcome variable. Second, a path model, including the three specific attachment dimensions (trust, communication, and alienation), was tested. The invariance of the models by sex was also examined. The analyses showed that peer attachment was associated positively with the adolescent–parent attachment relationship, the adolescent's age, as well as the teacher–student relationship, and associated negatively with the presence of internalizing behavior problems and social rejection. These variables helped explain 21.3% of the total variance. Give or take a few nuances, these same variables were also related to the three specific peer attachment dimensions. Sex differences were observed for the associated factors of these specific dimensions. This study highlights the importance of certain relational variables, in both family and school contexts, as correlates of peer attachment.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"5 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142207693","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Family alliance and infants’ vagal tone: The mediating role of infants’ reactions to unadjusted parental behaviors 家庭联盟与婴儿的迷走神经张力:婴儿对未经调整的父母行为的反应的中介作用
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12761
Valentine Rattaz, Hervé Tissot, Nilo Puglisi, Manuella Epiney, Chantal Razurel, Nicolas Favez
We investigated the influence of family alliance on infants’ vagal tone. Physiological studies have shown that the quality of mother–infant interactions can influence infants’ vagal tone, which is an important indicator of emotion regulation. Although research has shown that family‐level relationships have a unique impact on child development, little is known about the association between the quality of mother–father–infant interactions and infants’ physiological regulation during a family interaction task. We hypothesized that infants in families with a greater family alliance, that is, degree of coordination reached by parents when completing a task, will have higher vagal tone than will infants in families with a lower family alliance. We also hypothesized that this association would be mediated by the amount of intrusive and withdrawn parental behaviors and by the impact of these behaviors on the infant during mother–father–infant interactions. This study included 82 parents with their 3‐month‐old infants. Results showed that family alliance was associated with infants’ vagal tone during triadic interactions and that the impact of intrusive/withdrawn parental behaviors on the interaction partially mediated this association.
我们研究了家庭联盟对婴儿迷走神经张力的影响。生理学研究表明,母婴互动的质量会影响婴儿的迷走神经张力,而迷走神经张力是情绪调节的重要指标。尽管研究表明,家庭层面的关系对儿童的发展有着独特的影响,但人们对母婴互动的质量与婴儿在家庭互动任务中的生理调节之间的关系却知之甚少。我们假设,与家庭联盟度较低的家庭相比,家庭联盟度较高的家庭(即父母在完成任务时达成的协调程度)中的婴儿迷走神经张力较高。我们还假设,在母亲与父亲-婴儿互动过程中,父母的侵入和退缩行为以及这些行为对婴儿的影响会对这种关联产生中介作用。这项研究包括 82 位父母和他们 3 个月大的婴儿。结果表明,家庭联盟与婴儿在三方互动中的迷走神经张力有关,而父母的侵扰/退缩行为对互动的影响部分介导了这种关联。
{"title":"Family alliance and infants’ vagal tone: The mediating role of infants’ reactions to unadjusted parental behaviors","authors":"Valentine Rattaz, Hervé Tissot, Nilo Puglisi, Manuella Epiney, Chantal Razurel, Nicolas Favez","doi":"10.1111/sode.12761","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12761","url":null,"abstract":"We investigated the influence of family alliance on infants’ vagal tone. Physiological studies have shown that the quality of mother–infant interactions can influence infants’ vagal tone, which is an important indicator of emotion regulation. Although research has shown that family‐level relationships have a unique impact on child development, little is known about the association between the quality of mother–father–infant interactions and infants’ physiological regulation during a family interaction task. We hypothesized that infants in families with a greater family alliance, that is, degree of coordination reached by parents when completing a task, will have higher vagal tone than will infants in families with a lower family alliance. We also hypothesized that this association would be mediated by the amount of intrusive and withdrawn parental behaviors and by the impact of these behaviors on the infant during mother–father–infant interactions. This study included 82 parents with their 3‐month‐old infants. Results showed that family alliance was associated with infants’ vagal tone during triadic interactions and that the impact of intrusive/withdrawn parental behaviors on the interaction partially mediated this association.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"53 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141882113","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interactions between relationship support from mothers, fathers, and best friends as related to adolescent adjustment during the transition to high school 母亲、父亲和挚友的关系支持与青少年高中过渡适应之间的相互作用
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12760
Rachel A. Ghosh, Julie C. Bowker, Kenneth H. Rubin
Supportive parent‐adolescent relationships are known to promote adolescent adjustment, but less is known about the interactive roles of supportive relationships with mothers, fathers, and best friends. The current study examined the interactive relations between mother‐adolescent, father‐adolescent, and best friend relationship support on adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems across the high school transition. Participants (N = 368, Mage = 13.60, 47% male, 58% White) reported on their perceived support from mothers, fathers, and best friends (at Time 1 [Grade 8]) and their depressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors (at Times 1 and 2 [Grade 9]). Path models revealed no interaction effects involving support from mothers, fathers, and best friends when predicting externalizing symptoms. However, when predicting T2 depressive symptoms, several compensatory interaction effects were found. Father supportiveness moderated the association between maternal supportiveness and later depressive symptoms, and maternal supportiveness similarly moderated the relation between father supportiveness and depressive symptoms. In both instances, more support from one parent was related to fewer depressive symptoms when youth experienced an unsupportive relationship with the other parent. Best friend support also moderated the associations between both maternal support and paternal support and later depressive symptoms, such that maternal and paternal support were related to fewer subsequent depressive symptoms when youth experienced low and average (but not high) levels of friend support. The findings highlight the importance of considering networks of close relationships in the study of depressive symptoms during adolescence.
众所周知,父母与青少年之间的支持性关系能促进青少年的适应,但人们对母亲、父亲和最好的朋友之间的支持性关系的互动作用却知之甚少。本研究考察了母亲与青少年、父亲与青少年以及好友之间的支持关系对青少年在高中过渡时期的内化和外化问题的互动关系。参与者(人数 = 368,年龄 = 13.60,47% 为男性,58% 为白人)报告了他们从母亲、父亲和最好的朋友那里感知到的支持(时间 1 [8年级])以及他们的抑郁症状和外化行为(时间 1 和 2 [9年级])。路径模型显示,在预测外化症状时,母亲、父亲和最好朋友的支持没有交互效应。然而,在预测 T2 抑郁症状时,发现了几种补偿互动效应。父亲的支持调节了母亲的支持与后期抑郁症状之间的关系,而母亲的支持也同样调节了父亲的支持与抑郁症状之间的关系。在这两种情况下,当青少年与父母中的另一方关系不支持时,父母中的一方提供更多的支持与较少的抑郁症状有关。挚友支持也调节了母亲支持和父亲支持与青少年日后抑郁症状之间的关系,因此,当青少年经历低水平和平均水平(而非高水平)的挚友支持时,母亲和父亲的支持与青少年日后抑郁症状的减少有关。研究结果强调了在研究青少年抑郁症状时考虑亲密关系网络的重要性。
{"title":"Interactions between relationship support from mothers, fathers, and best friends as related to adolescent adjustment during the transition to high school","authors":"Rachel A. Ghosh, Julie C. Bowker, Kenneth H. Rubin","doi":"10.1111/sode.12760","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12760","url":null,"abstract":"Supportive parent‐adolescent relationships are known to promote adolescent adjustment, but less is known about the interactive roles of supportive relationships with mothers, fathers, and best friends. The current study examined the interactive relations between mother‐adolescent, father‐adolescent, and best friend relationship support on adolescent internalizing and externalizing problems across the high school transition. Participants (<jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 368, <jats:italic>M</jats:italic><jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 13.60, 47% male, 58% White) reported on their perceived support from mothers, fathers, and best friends (at Time 1 [Grade 8]) and their depressive symptoms and externalizing behaviors (at Times 1 and 2 [Grade 9]). Path models revealed no interaction effects involving support from mothers, fathers, and best friends when predicting externalizing symptoms. However, when predicting T2 depressive symptoms, several compensatory interaction effects were found. Father supportiveness moderated the association between maternal supportiveness and later depressive symptoms, and maternal supportiveness similarly moderated the relation between father supportiveness and depressive symptoms. In both instances, more support from one parent was related to fewer depressive symptoms when youth experienced an unsupportive relationship with the other parent. Best friend support also moderated the associations between both maternal support and paternal support and later depressive symptoms, such that maternal and paternal support were related to fewer subsequent depressive symptoms when youth experienced low and average (but not high) levels of friend support. The findings highlight the importance of considering networks of close relationships in the study of depressive symptoms during adolescence.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"129 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141864861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parents’ differential trait, mental state, and coping talk about White and Black child storybook characters 父母对白人和黑人儿童故事书角色的不同特质、心理状态和应对谈话
IF 2 4区 心理学 Q3 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-20 DOI: 10.1111/sode.12758
Ashley M. Fraser, Peter J. Reschke, Andrea K. Busby, Emily J. Takamasa, Jennie Jasperson, Bethany Sycamore
Limited literature has examined parents’ unsolicited trait, mental state, and coping talk about media characters by race as they co‐view with their children. We observed 195 US parents describing an illustrated depiction of racialized social exclusion for their child (53% male; Mage = 5.46 years; 60% White) in their home setting. Families discussed a Black child being excluded by White children or a White child being excluded by Black children. Parents’ victim, emotion, personality, and coping talk about the excluded child were coded. Parents were more likely to use victim talk (e.g., “she's being excluded”) and emotion talk (e.g., “she's sad”) when the excluded child was Black and were more likely to use personality talk (e.g., “she's shy”) when the excluded child was White. We further explored parents’ demographic, personal, and neighborhood correlates with their trait, mental state, and coping talk. White parents with greater levels of egalitarian attitudes used more victim talk and coping talk (e.g., “she should go play with them”) about the White excluded child. Being more politically conservative was associated with White parents using more coping talk about the Black excluded child. White parents living in predominantly Black neighborhoods were less likely to use victim talk toward the Black child. BIPOC parents showed more nuanced patterns. These findings suggest that parents’ unsolicited racial messaging varies by racial identification, sociodemographic characteristics, and neighborhood context.
研究父母在与子女共同观看媒体时对媒体人物的主动特质、精神状态和应对方式的文献有限。我们观察了 195 位美国父母在家庭环境中描述其孩子(53% 为男性;年龄 = 5.46 岁;60% 为白人)受到种族化社会排斥的插图。这些家庭讨论了黑人孩子被白人孩子排斥或白人孩子被黑人孩子排斥的情况。对家长关于被排斥儿童的受害者、情绪、个性和应对谈话进行了编码。当被排斥的孩子是黑人时,家长更有可能使用受害者谈话(如 "她被排斥了")和情绪谈话(如 "她很伤心"),而当被排斥的孩子是白人时,家长更有可能使用个性谈话(如 "她很害羞")。我们进一步探讨了父母的人口统计学、个人和邻里关系与其特质、精神状态和应对谈话的相关性。对于被排斥的白人孩子,平等主义态度较强的白人家长使用了更多的受害者言论和应对言论(如 "她应该去和他们一起玩")。政治上更保守的白人父母对黑人受排斥儿童使用更多的应对谈话。居住在以黑人为主的社区的白人家长不太可能对黑人孩子使用受害者谈话。双性恋和变性人家长则表现出更细微的模式。这些研究结果表明,父母主动发出的种族信息因种族认同、社会人口特征和社区环境而异。
{"title":"Parents’ differential trait, mental state, and coping talk about White and Black child storybook characters","authors":"Ashley M. Fraser, Peter J. Reschke, Andrea K. Busby, Emily J. Takamasa, Jennie Jasperson, Bethany Sycamore","doi":"10.1111/sode.12758","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12758","url":null,"abstract":"Limited literature has examined parents’ unsolicited trait, mental state, and coping talk about media characters by race as they co‐view with their children. We observed 195 US parents describing an illustrated depiction of racialized social exclusion for their child (53% male; <jats:italic>M</jats:italic><jats:sub>age</jats:sub> = 5.46 years; 60% White) in their home setting. Families discussed a Black child being excluded by White children or a White child being excluded by Black children. Parents’ victim, emotion, personality, and coping talk about the excluded child were coded. Parents were more likely to use victim talk (e.g., “she's being excluded”) and emotion talk (e.g., “she's sad”) when the excluded child was Black and were more likely to use personality talk (e.g., “she's shy”) when the excluded child was White. We further explored parents’ demographic, personal, and neighborhood correlates with their trait, mental state, and coping talk. White parents with greater levels of egalitarian attitudes used more victim talk and coping talk (e.g., “she should go play with them”) about the White excluded child. Being more politically conservative was associated with White parents using more coping talk about the Black excluded child. White parents living in predominantly Black neighborhoods were less likely to use victim talk toward the Black child. BIPOC parents showed more nuanced patterns. These findings suggest that parents’ unsolicited racial messaging varies by racial identification, sociodemographic characteristics, and neighborhood context.","PeriodicalId":48203,"journal":{"name":"Social Development","volume":"55 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.0,"publicationDate":"2024-07-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141739626","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":4,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Social Development
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1