首页 > 最新文献

Motivation and Emotion最新文献

英文 中文
Social dynamics in interpersonal emotion regulation: A theoretical framework for understanding direct and indirect other-based processes 人际情绪调节的社会动力:理解基于他人的直接和间接过程的理论框架
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-23 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10065-2

Abstract

Interpersonal emotion regulation involves having emotions changed in a social context. While some research has used the term to refer to instances where others are used to alter one’s own emotions (intrinsic), other research refers to goal-directed actions aimed at modifying others’ emotional responses (extrinsic). We argue that the self-other distinction should be applied not only to the target (who has their emotion regulated) but also to the means (whether the agent uses themselves or others to achieve the regulation). Based on this, we propose interpersonal emotion regulation can take place when an agent changes a target’s emotions by affecting a third party’s emotion who will shift the emotion of the target in turn (direct other-based interpersonal ER) or by impacting a third party’s emotion (indirect other-based interpersonal ER). We discuss these processes and the conditions that lead to their emergence reconciling findings from different fields and suggesting new research venues.

摘要 人际情绪调节涉及在社会环境中改变情绪。一些研究用这个词来指利用他人来改变自己的情绪(内在),而另一些研究则指旨在改变他人情绪反应的目标导向行动(外在)。我们认为,自我与他人的区别不仅应适用于目标(谁的情绪受到调节),还应适用于手段(行为主体是利用自己还是他人来实现调节)。在此基础上,我们提出人际情绪调节可以发生在代理人通过影响第三方的情绪来改变目标对象的情绪,而第三方的情绪又会反过来改变目标对象的情绪(直接的基于他人的人际情绪调节),或者通过影响第三方的情绪来改变目标对象的情绪(间接的基于他人的人际情绪调节)。我们讨论了这些过程以及导致其出现的条件,调和了不同领域的研究结果,并提出了新的研究方向。
{"title":"Social dynamics in interpersonal emotion regulation: A theoretical framework for understanding direct and indirect other-based processes","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10065-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10065-2","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Interpersonal emotion regulation involves having emotions changed in a social context. While some research has used the term to refer to instances where others are used to alter one’s own emotions (intrinsic), other research refers to goal-directed actions aimed at modifying others’ emotional responses (extrinsic). We argue that the self-other distinction should be applied not only to the target (who has their emotion regulated) but also to the means (whether the agent uses themselves or others to achieve the regulation). Based on this, we propose interpersonal emotion regulation can take place when an agent changes a target’s emotions by affecting a third party’s emotion who will shift the emotion of the target in turn (direct other-based interpersonal ER) or by impacting a third party’s emotion (indirect other-based interpersonal ER). We discuss these processes and the conditions that lead to their emergence reconciling findings from different fields and suggesting new research venues.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140203190","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spontaneous self-affirmation predicts more meaning and less boredom 自发的自我肯定预示着更多的意义和更少的无聊
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-15 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10060-7

Abstract

Chronic boredom is associated with many negative psychological outcomes, including undermining perceived meaning in life. Meanwhile, emerging research suggests that spontaneous self-affirmation, that is, an inclination to self-affirm, is linked to greater well-being and buffers against psychological threats. We investigated the relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation, perceptions of meaning in life, and boredom proneness with four correlational studies. Study 1a (N = 166) demonstrated that people inclined to self-affirm experience greater perceptions of meaning in life. Study 1b (N = 170) confirmed that spontaneous self-affirmation is associated with lower levels of boredom proneness. Study 2a (N = 214) and Study 2b (N = 105) provided evidence for our central hypothesis, showing that spontaneous self-affirmation predicts lower levels of boredom proneness via greater perceptions of meaning in life. These findings confirm that elevating meaning in life through psychological resources, like spontaneous self-affirmation, may limit boredom. Our work extends the emerging well-being benefits of spontaneous self-affirmation, by demonstrating associations with higher meaning in life and lower boredom proneness.

摘要 长期的无聊与许多负面的心理结果有关,包括破坏对生活意义的感知。同时,新的研究表明,自发的自我肯定,即自我肯定的倾向,与更大的幸福感和缓冲心理威胁有关。我们通过四项相关研究调查了自发的自我肯定、对生活意义的感知和无聊感之间的关系。研究 1a(N = 166)表明,倾向于自我肯定的人更能感受到生活的意义。研究 1b(样本数=170)证实,自发的自我肯定与较低的厌烦情绪相关。研究 2a(N = 214)和研究 2b(N = 105)为我们的中心假设提供了证据,表明自发的自我肯定通过对生活意义的更高感知来预测较低水平的厌倦倾向。这些研究结果证实,通过心理资源(如自发的自我肯定)提升生活的意义可能会减少无聊感。我们的研究通过证明自发自我肯定与提高生活意义和降低无聊感之间的关联,扩展了自发自我肯定带来的新兴幸福感益处。
{"title":"Spontaneous self-affirmation predicts more meaning and less boredom","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10060-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10060-7","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Chronic boredom is associated with many negative psychological outcomes, including undermining perceived meaning in life. Meanwhile, emerging research suggests that spontaneous self-affirmation, that is, an inclination to self-affirm, is linked to greater well-being and buffers against psychological threats. We investigated the relationship between spontaneous self-affirmation, perceptions of meaning in life, and boredom proneness with four correlational studies. Study 1a (N = 166) demonstrated that people inclined to self-affirm experience greater perceptions of meaning in life. Study 1b (N = 170) confirmed that spontaneous self-affirmation is associated with lower levels of boredom proneness. Study 2a (N = 214) and Study 2b (N = 105) provided evidence for our central hypothesis, showing that spontaneous self-affirmation predicts lower levels of boredom proneness via greater perceptions of meaning in life. These findings confirm that elevating meaning in life through psychological resources, like spontaneous self-affirmation, may limit boredom. Our work extends the emerging well-being benefits of spontaneous self-affirmation, by demonstrating associations with higher meaning in life and lower boredom proneness.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140147226","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Anticipated affective action-effects as controlling factors of spatially oriented pointing movement 作为空间定向指向运动控制因素的预期情感动作效应
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-08 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10061-6
Lola Lachaud, Carole Ferrel, Vincent Dru, Sylvie Vernazza-Martin

Ideomotor theories admit that anticipated action-effects are the basis of the action selection and initiation, but there is no common understanding of the role of anticipated affective outcomes in motor organization. This study provides substantial information that shows that anticipated affective action-effects (positive/negative) combined with the direction in which a movement is performed (rightward/leftward) significantly impact its organization. Sixty-four right-handed subjects performed right and left pointing movements on a graphic tablet to trigger the appearance of a positive or negative visual outcome. Results indicate a compatibility effect between the direction of the movement and the valence of the action-effect that facilitates cognitive processes involved in movement organization. Pointing movements were faster triggered and executed when the movement was directed to the right combined with a positive outcome and when the movement was directed to the left with a negative one. Such findings are consistent with Casasanto’s body-specificity hypothesis, which stipulates an association of the dominant hand side with positive features, and with ideomotor theory where perception and action share a common representation. This experiment confirms that voluntary movements’ organization is built through an action representation, including affective predictions.

意念运动理论承认,预期的动作效果是动作选择和启动的基础,但对于预期的情感结果在动作组织中的作用却没有共识。本研究提供的大量信息表明,预期的情感动作效果(积极/消极)与执行动作的方向(向右/向左)相结合,会对动作组织产生重大影响。64 名右撇子受试者在图形平板电脑上做左右指向动作,以触发积极或消极视觉结果的出现。结果表明,动作的方向和动作效果的价值之间存在相容效应,这种相容效应促进了动作组织的认知过程。当指向右侧的动作与积极的结果结合在一起时,以及指向左侧的动作与消极的结果结合在一起时,指向动作会更快地被触发和执行。这些发现与卡萨桑托的身体特异性假说和意念运动理论相吻合,前者认为优势手侧与积极特征相关联,而后者则认为感知和动作具有共同的表征。本实验证实,自主运动的组织是通过动作表征(包括情感预测)建立起来的。
{"title":"Anticipated affective action-effects as controlling factors of spatially oriented pointing movement","authors":"Lola Lachaud, Carole Ferrel, Vincent Dru, Sylvie Vernazza-Martin","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10061-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10061-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Ideomotor theories admit that anticipated action-effects are the basis of the action selection and initiation, but there is no common understanding of the role of anticipated affective outcomes in motor organization. This study provides substantial information that shows that anticipated affective action-effects (positive/negative) combined with the direction in which a movement is performed (rightward/leftward) significantly impact its organization. Sixty-four right-handed subjects performed right and left pointing movements on a graphic tablet to trigger the appearance of a positive or negative visual outcome. Results indicate a compatibility effect between the direction of the movement and the valence of the action-effect that facilitates cognitive processes involved in movement organization. Pointing movements were faster triggered and executed when the movement was directed to the right combined with a positive outcome and when the movement was directed to the left with a negative one. Such findings are consistent with Casasanto’s body-specificity hypothesis, which stipulates an association of the dominant hand side with positive features, and with ideomotor theory where perception and action share a common representation. This experiment confirms that voluntary movements’ organization is built through an action representation, including affective predictions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-03-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140071934","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Basic psychological needs as an explanation of body image and eating pathways to adolescent adjustment: a prospective study testing an integrated self-determination theory model 以基本心理需求解释青少年适应身体形象和饮食的途径:检验综合自我决定理论模型的前瞻性研究
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10063-4
Shi Yu, Ziyi Zoey He, Tianxiang Cui, Jinbo He
{"title":"Basic psychological needs as an explanation of body image and eating pathways to adolescent adjustment: a prospective study testing an integrated self-determination theory model","authors":"Shi Yu, Ziyi Zoey He, Tianxiang Cui, Jinbo He","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10063-4","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10063-4","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140437146","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Seizing and freezing to life outcomes: Need for cognitive closure intensifies affective reactions to major events 对生活结果的把握和冻结:对认知封闭的需求会加剧对重大事件的情感反应
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-23 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10058-1
Seung Eun Cha, Xyle Ku, Soeun Sarah Lee, Incheol Choi

Research shows that significant well-being disparities emerge between individuals who experience major negative events and those who undergo major positive events. However, such differences may vary among individuals. Drawing from the theory of the need for cognitive closure (NFC), we theorize and test whether NFC, which captures seizing and freezing on salient information to reach a fast conclusion, intensifies the differences in affective well-being between those who experience negative or positive events. Across three studies (total N = 2,399), we provide converging evidence that supports our theoretical claim. We first found that participants with high (vs. low) NFC show a greater affective well-being gap between those who recalled their past negative and positive major events (Study 1). We also discovered consistent patterns when participants were provided with negative or positive major event scenarios (Study 2). Lastly, we further substantiated our findings by utilizing a longitudinal study of the 20th presidential election in South Korea (Study 3). Over a 6-week period, the well-being gaps between the supporters of the election winner and the runner-up were more pronounced among individuals with higher NFC. Overall, our findings have implications for identifying a novel psychological trait that influences the affective well-being gaps following significant events in one’s life.

研究表明,经历重大消极事件的人与经历重大积极事件的人之间会出现明显的幸福感差异。然而,这种差异可能因人而异。根据认知封闭需要(NFC)理论,我们推测并检验了 NFC(捕捉并冻结突出信息以快速得出结论)是否会加剧经历消极或积极事件的人之间的情感幸福感差异。通过三项研究(总人数 = 2,399),我们提供了支持我们理论主张的趋同证据。我们首先发现,NFC 高(与 NFC 低)的参与者在回忆过去的消极和积极重大事件时,会表现出更大的情感幸福感差距(研究 1)。我们还发现,当参与者被提供消极或积极的重大事件情景时,他们的模式也是一致的(研究 2)。最后,我们通过对韩国第 20 届总统选举的纵向研究进一步证实了我们的发现(研究 3)。在为期 6 周的研究中,在 NFC 较高的个体中,大选冠军支持者与亚军支持者之间的幸福感差距更为明显。总之,我们的研究结果对确定一种新的心理特征具有重要意义,这种心理特征会影响人在经历人生重大事件后的情感幸福感差距。
{"title":"Seizing and freezing to life outcomes: Need for cognitive closure intensifies affective reactions to major events","authors":"Seung Eun Cha, Xyle Ku, Soeun Sarah Lee, Incheol Choi","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10058-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10058-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research shows that significant well-being disparities emerge between individuals who experience major negative events and those who undergo major positive events. However, such differences may vary among individuals. Drawing from the theory of the need for cognitive closure (NFC), we theorize and test whether NFC, which captures seizing and freezing on salient information to reach a fast conclusion, intensifies the differences in affective well-being between those who experience negative or positive events. Across three studies (total <i>N</i> = 2,399), we provide converging evidence that supports our theoretical claim. We first found that participants with high (vs. low) NFC show a greater affective well-being gap between those who recalled their past negative and positive major events (Study 1). We also discovered consistent patterns when participants were provided with negative or positive major event scenarios (Study 2). Lastly, we further substantiated our findings by utilizing a longitudinal study of the 20th presidential election in South Korea (Study 3). Over a 6-week period, the well-being gaps between the supporters of the election winner and the runner-up were more pronounced among individuals with higher NFC. Overall, our findings have implications for identifying a novel psychological trait that influences the affective well-being gaps following significant events in one’s life.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139947445","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Interplay of intrinsic motivation and well-being at school 内在动力与学校幸福感的相互作用
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-18 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10057-2

Abstract

Feeling well at school is crucial for both young people’s healthy self-development and their intrinsic learning motivation. Both concepts, subjective well-being and intrinsic motivation, are central to self-determination theory, but their reciprocal relationship is not studied very well. We hypothesized that the concepts affect each other directly: Students who are intrinsically motivated feel well at school due to positive emotions and the strive for understanding the learning content, which is an intrinsic motivation immanent. Beyond, students who feel well at school are intrinsically motivated, because subjective well-being represents an intrinsic value of the learning situation. This hypothesis is tested using a sample of 773 secondary school students (mean age: 12.7 years, SD = 1.8), who were answering questionnaires at the beginning (T1), at mid (T2) and at the end (T3) of a school year. The data were analyzed using a cross-lagged panel model. The results gave evidence on cross-lagged effects and an indirect effect from subjective well-being to intrinsic motivation to subjective well-being.

摘要 在学校感觉良好对青少年的健康自我发展和内在学习动机都至关重要。主观幸福感和内在学习动机这两个概念都是自我决定理论的核心,但它们之间的相互关系却没有得到很好的研究。我们假设这两个概念会直接相互影响:具有内在动机的学生由于积极的情绪和努力理解学习内容而在学校感觉良好,这是一种内在的内在动机。此外,在学校感觉良好的学生也具有内在动机,因为主观幸福感代表了学习情境的内在价值。本假设以 773 名中学生(平均年龄:12.7 岁,SD = 1.8)为样本进行了检验,这些学生分别在学年开始(T1)、中期(T2)和结束(T3)时回答了问卷。数据采用交叉滞后面板模型进行分析。结果证明了交叉滞后效应,以及从主观幸福感到内在动机再到主观幸福感的间接效应。
{"title":"Interplay of intrinsic motivation and well-being at school","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10057-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10057-2","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Feeling well at school is crucial for both young people’s healthy self-development and their intrinsic learning motivation. Both concepts, subjective well-being and intrinsic motivation, are central to self-determination theory, but their reciprocal relationship is not studied very well. We hypothesized that the concepts affect each other directly: Students who are intrinsically motivated feel well at school due to positive emotions and the strive for understanding the learning content, which is an intrinsic motivation immanent. Beyond, students who feel well at school are intrinsically motivated, because subjective well-being represents an intrinsic value of the learning situation. This hypothesis is tested using a sample of 773 secondary school students (mean age: 12.7 years, SD = 1.8), who were answering questionnaires at the beginning (T1), at mid (T2) and at the end (T3) of a school year. The data were analyzed using a cross-lagged panel model. The results gave evidence on cross-lagged effects and an indirect effect from subjective well-being to intrinsic motivation to subjective well-being.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139910059","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sounds boring: the causal effect of boredom on self-administration of aversive stimuli in the presence of a positive alternative 听起来很无聊:在有积极替代品的情况下,无聊对厌恶刺激自我施用的因果效应
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-13 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10053-y

Abstract

Previous studies demonstrated that boredom leads to increased self-administration of painful electric stimulation, a proxy for non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI). However, in these experimental studies, participants had no behavioral alternatives besides electric stimulation to break the monotony. A first aim in the current experimental study was to examine whether boredom still leads to self-administering aversive stimuli when positive behavioral alternatives are present. This boredom effect was furthermore compared to an anger induction. The second aim was to examine whether history of NSSI and negative urgency (i.e., the tendency to engage in impulsive action in response to negative emotions) positively moderate the link between boredom and self-administered aversive stimuli. In a between-subjects design using college students (N = 129), participants were randomly assigned to one of three emotion induction writing tasks (i.e., boredom, anger, neutral), during which frequency of self-administered positive (chirping birds) and aversive (screaming pig) sounds was measured. The latter was used as a proxy for NSSI behavior. Results showed that boredom led to increased selection of aversive sounds compared to the neutral and anger conditions, despite the presence of a positive alternative (i.e., positive sounds). No difference in frequency of selecting the aversive sounds was observed between the anger and neutral condition. Neither history of NSSI nor negative urgency moderated the effect of condition on self-administered aversive stimuli. The current results tentatively support a causal and specific link between boredom and NSSI, and warrant further examination of the role of boredom in maladaptive behaviors such as NSSI.

摘要 以前的研究表明,无聊会导致更多的人自我使用痛苦的电刺激,这是非自杀性自伤(NSSI)的一种表现形式。然而,在这些实验研究中,参与者除了电刺激外没有其他行为选择来打破单调的生活。本实验研究的第一个目的是研究当存在积极的行为替代方案时,无聊是否仍会导致自我施用厌恶刺激。此外,还将这种无聊效应与愤怒诱导进行了比较。第二个目的是研究NSSI史和消极紧迫感(即对消极情绪做出反应而采取冲动行动的倾向)是否会对无聊和自我施用厌恶刺激之间的联系起到积极的调节作用。在以大学生(129 人)为研究对象的主体间设计中,被试被随机分配到三种情绪诱导写作任务(即无聊、愤怒、中性)中的一种,在此期间,被试测量了自发的积极声音(鸟鸣)和厌恶声音(猪叫)的频率。后者被用来替代 NSSI 行为。结果显示,与中性和愤怒条件相比,尽管存在积极的替代选择(即积极的声音),但无聊会导致选择厌恶声音的频率增加。在愤怒和中性条件下,选择厌恶声音的频率没有差异。NSSI史和消极紧迫感都没有调节条件对自控厌恶刺激的影响。目前的研究结果初步支持了无聊与NSSI之间的因果和特定联系,值得进一步研究无聊在NSSI等适应不良行为中的作用。
{"title":"Sounds boring: the causal effect of boredom on self-administration of aversive stimuli in the presence of a positive alternative","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10053-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10053-y","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Previous studies demonstrated that boredom leads to increased self-administration of painful electric stimulation, a proxy for non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI). However, in these experimental studies, participants had no behavioral alternatives besides electric stimulation to break the monotony. A first aim in the current experimental study was to examine whether boredom still leads to self-administering aversive stimuli when positive behavioral alternatives are present. This boredom effect was furthermore compared to an anger induction. The second aim was to examine whether history of NSSI and negative urgency (i.e., the tendency to engage in impulsive action in response to negative emotions) positively moderate the link between boredom and self-administered aversive stimuli. In a between-subjects design using college students (N = 129), participants were randomly assigned to one of three emotion induction writing tasks (i.e., boredom, anger, neutral), during which frequency of self-administered positive (chirping birds) and aversive (screaming pig) sounds was measured. The latter was used as a proxy for NSSI behavior. Results showed that boredom led to increased selection of aversive sounds compared to the neutral and anger conditions, despite the presence of a positive alternative (i.e., positive sounds). No difference in frequency of selecting the aversive sounds was observed between the anger and neutral condition. Neither history of NSSI nor negative urgency moderated the effect of condition on self-administered aversive stimuli. The current results tentatively support a causal and specific link between boredom and NSSI, and warrant further examination of the role of boredom in maladaptive behaviors such as NSSI.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139752872","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Intact modulation of response vigor in major depressive disorder 重度抑郁障碍中反应活力的完整调节
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-024-10059-0
Shirel Bakbani-Elkayam, Tohar Dolev-Amit, Eitan Hemed, Sigal Zilcha-Mano, Baruch Eitam

Blunted motivation is a core symptom of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Although the empirical picture is mixed, cognitive processes that can be collectively referred to as reward processing have been found to be consistently muted in MDD; most notably, reward sensitivity and reinforcement learning. Works on the modulation of response vigor in individuals with MDD have examined various types of reward, but recent research has shown that in the general population, response vigor is not modulated by type of reward on tasks that are highly similar to those used in these experiments. The present study implemented a form of non-reward related reinforcement which has repeatedly been shown to modulate response vigor in the general population. It investigated whether modulation of response vigor by this type of reinforcement would be effective in individuals with MDD. Clinically depressed individuals (N = 121; 76 post-exclusion) engaged in a task in which their responses led to predictable and immediate sensorimotor effects, or no such effects. Response vigor increased when responses led to sensorimotor effects, which was comparable to the increase found in the general population. These findings support the utility of isolating the computations leading to different reinforcement types and suggest that motivational deficits in MDD may be specific to the type of reward (i.e., hedonically or otherwise explicitly desired stimuli). These results contribute to the literature by suggesting that the reinforcement from sensorimotor predictability stems from processes devolved to motor control, whereas reinforcement from rewards may depend on more general-purpose processes.

动机减退是重度抑郁症(MDD)的一个核心症状。虽然实证研究结果不一,但人们发现,在 MDD 患者中,统称为奖赏加工的认知过程一直处于失调状态;其中最明显的是奖赏敏感性和强化学习。有关 MDD 患者反应强度调节的研究已经对各种类型的奖赏进行了研究,但最近的研究表明,在与这些实验高度相似的任务中,普通人群的反应强度并不受奖赏类型的调节。本研究采用了一种与奖励无关的强化方式,这种强化方式在普通人群中被反复证明可以调节反应强度。本研究调查了这种强化形式对反应强度的调节是否对患有精神抑郁症的人有效。临床抑郁症患者(121 人;76 人在排除后)参与了一项任务,在这项任务中,他们的反应会导致可预测的即时感官运动效应或无此类效应。当反应导致感觉运动效应时,反应强度会增加,这与在普通人群中发现的增幅相当。这些发现支持了将导致不同强化类型的计算分离出来的实用性,并表明 MDD 的动机缺陷可能与奖励类型(即享乐或其他明确期望的刺激)有关。这些结果表明,感知运动可预测性带来的强化源于下放到运动控制的过程,而奖励带来的强化可能依赖于更通用的过程,从而为相关文献做出了贡献。
{"title":"Intact modulation of response vigor in major depressive disorder","authors":"Shirel Bakbani-Elkayam, Tohar Dolev-Amit, Eitan Hemed, Sigal Zilcha-Mano, Baruch Eitam","doi":"10.1007/s11031-024-10059-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-024-10059-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Blunted motivation is a core symptom of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Although the empirical picture is mixed, cognitive processes that can be collectively referred to as reward processing have been found to be consistently muted in MDD; most notably, reward sensitivity and reinforcement learning. Works on the modulation of response vigor in individuals with MDD have examined various types of reward, but recent research has shown that in the general population, response vigor is not modulated by type of reward on tasks that are highly similar to those used in these experiments. The present study implemented a form of non-reward related reinforcement which has repeatedly been shown to modulate response vigor in the general population. It investigated whether modulation of response vigor by this type of reinforcement would be effective in individuals with MDD. Clinically depressed individuals (N = 121; 76 post-exclusion) engaged in a task in which their responses led to predictable and immediate sensorimotor effects, or no such effects. Response vigor increased when responses led to sensorimotor effects, which was comparable to the increase found in the general population. These findings support the utility of isolating the computations leading to different reinforcement types and suggest that motivational deficits in MDD may be specific to the type of reward (i.e., hedonically or otherwise explicitly desired stimuli). These results contribute to the literature by suggesting that the reinforcement from sensorimotor predictability stems from processes devolved to motor control, whereas reinforcement from rewards may depend on more general-purpose processes.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139752853","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men 减少色盲男性对食物的恐惧和反感
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w
Isabel Gauthier, Bunmi Olatunji
{"title":"Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men","authors":"Isabel Gauthier, Bunmi Olatunji","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139800075","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men 减少色盲男性对食物的恐惧和反感
IF 4.8 3区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w
Isabel Gauthier, Bunmi Olatunji
{"title":"Reduced food neophobia and food disgust in colorblind men","authors":"Isabel Gauthier, Bunmi Olatunji","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10055-w","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139859911","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Motivation and Emotion
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1