Pub Date : 2023-12-20DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10052-z
Christopher Mlynski, Swantje Mueller, Christopher M. Napolitano, Veronika Job
While there is anecdotal evidence and some scientific support for the value of having multiple paths to reach one’s life goals, recent work concerning backup plans argues that their mere availability undermines commitment to and performance in the originally chosen path. In this paper, we evaluated this phenomenon amongst college students (N = 345) entering their first term with an already available family-based alternative life path. As expected, entering into college with an available family-based alternative life path led to a decrease in study commitment over the first semester and indirectly predicted lower end-of-semester grades through this reduction in commitment. However, results indicate that this only occurred when students reported experiencing an action crisis at the end of their first semester. If students did not report having an action crisis, an available family-based alternative life path did not influence study commitment and predicted a higher end-of-semester GPA. Ultimately, findings highlight the major role action crisis plays in the influence an alternative life path has on path trajectory.
{"title":"A backup plan for life? Alternative Life paths facilitate disengagement in an action crisis","authors":"Christopher Mlynski, Swantje Mueller, Christopher M. Napolitano, Veronika Job","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10052-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10052-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While there is anecdotal evidence and some scientific support for the value of having multiple paths to reach one’s life goals, recent work concerning backup plans argues that their mere availability undermines commitment to and performance in the originally chosen path. In this paper, we evaluated this phenomenon amongst college students (<i>N</i> = 345) entering their first term with an already available family-based alternative life path. As expected, entering into college with an available family-based alternative life path led to a decrease in study commitment over the first semester and indirectly predicted lower end-of-semester grades through this reduction in commitment. However, results indicate that this only occurred when students reported experiencing an action crisis at the end of their first semester. If students did not report having an action crisis, an available family-based alternative life path did not influence study commitment and predicted a higher end-of-semester GPA. Ultimately, findings highlight the major role action crisis plays in the influence an alternative life path has on path trajectory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"7 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138817415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-17DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10048-9
Muireann K. O’Dea, Eric R. Igou, Wijnand A. P. van Tilburg
Boredom signals a lack of meaning. Gratitude promotes feelings of meaning in life. We proposed accordingly that gratitude, by engendering meaning, shields against boredom. Specifically, we hypothesized that gratitude prevents boredom by increasing perceptions of meaning in life. We tested this hypothesis in five studies (N = 954). Study 1 revealed that grateful people are less prone to boredom. Studies 2a and 2b demonstrated that grateful people are less prone to boredom, and this relationship is statistically mediated by elevated meaning in life. Study 3 found that dispositional gratitude also predicted less state boredom in response to a behavioural task, via heightened perceptions of meaning in life. In Study 4, experimentally induced gratitude reduced boredom through increased perceptions of meaning in life. The findings demonstrate gratitude’s role in effectively reducing and preventing boredom by boosting the feeling that life is meaningful.
{"title":"Preventing boredom with gratitude: The role of meaning in life","authors":"Muireann K. O’Dea, Eric R. Igou, Wijnand A. P. van Tilburg","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10048-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10048-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Boredom signals a lack of meaning. Gratitude promotes feelings of meaning in life. We proposed accordingly that gratitude, by engendering meaning, shields against boredom. Specifically, we hypothesized that gratitude prevents boredom by increasing perceptions of meaning in life. We tested this hypothesis in five studies (N = 954). Study 1 revealed that grateful people are less prone to boredom. Studies 2a and 2b demonstrated that grateful people are less prone to boredom, and this relationship is statistically mediated by elevated meaning in life. Study 3 found that dispositional gratitude also predicted less state boredom in response to a behavioural task, via heightened perceptions of meaning in life. In Study 4, experimentally induced gratitude reduced boredom through increased perceptions of meaning in life. The findings demonstrate gratitude’s role in effectively reducing and preventing boredom by boosting the feeling that life is meaningful.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"100 11","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138693235","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-16DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10054-x
Abstract
Research on attention allocation in OCD has exclusively used obsession-provoking threat stimuli, showing sustained attention over neutral ones. Recently, however, a study contrasting neutral stimuli with stimuli depicting the completion of compulsive acts (end-states stimuli), which is associated with a reduced anxiety (negative reinforcement), showed similar results. Yet, as relative to neutral stimuli end-state stimuli were also OCD-related, these results may still reflect obsession-related distress rather than relief brought on by stimuli signaling compulsion completion. Also, end-states stimuli were not systematically validated. In Study 1, participants rated the subjective discomfort experienced when viewing traditional obsession-provoking threat pictures and novel end-states pictures. We first compared participants with high (HOC) and low (LOC) levels of obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and then clinically diagnosed OCD participants and matched healthy controls. In study 2, we compared gaze patterns of HOC and LOC participants while freely viewing 2-by-2 matrices directly contrasting two threat and two end-state stimuli. Study 1 showed a larger difference in experienced discomfort when viewing the end-state stimuli, compared to threat stimuli, for both OC groups compared with their respected control groups. Study 2 showed that while LOC participants demonstrated no difference in dwell time on threat vs. end-state pictures, the HOC group allocated more attention toward the latter. Both groups were more prone to fixate first the threat pictures. Task reliability was high. Attention allocation in OCD may also be affected by cues signaling the completion of compulsive acts.
摘要 有关强迫症注意力分配的研究只使用了引起强迫症的威胁刺激,结果显示持续注意力高于中性刺激。然而,最近的一项研究将中性刺激与描述强迫行为完成过程的刺激(终结状态刺激)进行了对比,显示出了类似的结果,后者与焦虑减少(负强化)有关。然而,由于相对于中性刺激而言,终结状态刺激也与强迫症有关,因此这些结果可能仍然反映了与强迫症有关的痛苦,而不是暗示强迫行为完成的刺激所带来的缓解。此外,终结状态刺激也没有经过系统的验证。在研究 1 中,参与者对观看传统的强迫症威胁图片和新颖的终结状态图片时所体验到的主观不适感进行了评分。我们首先比较了强迫症状程度高(HOC)和程度低(LOC)的参与者,然后比较了临床诊断为强迫症的参与者和匹配的健康对照组。在研究 2 中,我们比较了 HOC 和 LOC 参与者在自由观看 2×2 矩阵时的注视模式,该矩阵直接对比了两个威胁刺激和两个结束状态刺激。研究 1 显示,与受尊重的对照组相比,OC 组和 LOC 组在观看终结状态刺激时所体验到的不适感差异更大。研究 2 显示,虽然 LOC 参与者在观看威胁图片和结束状态图片时的停留时间没有差异,但 HOC 组对后者的注意力分配更多。两组受试者都更容易将注意力首先集中在威胁图片上。任务的可靠性很高。强迫症患者的注意力分配也可能会受到强迫行为完成的信号线索的影响。
{"title":"A sight for sore eyes? Attention allocation to obsession-provoking stimuli and stimuli reflecting compulsion completion","authors":"","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10054-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10054-x","url":null,"abstract":"<h3>Abstract</h3> <p>Research on attention allocation in OCD has exclusively used obsession-provoking threat stimuli, showing sustained attention over neutral ones. Recently, however, a study contrasting neutral stimuli with stimuli depicting the completion of compulsive acts (end-states stimuli), which is associated with a reduced anxiety (negative reinforcement), showed similar results. Yet, as relative to neutral stimuli end-state stimuli were also OCD-related, these results may still reflect obsession-related distress rather than relief brought on by stimuli signaling compulsion completion. Also, end-states stimuli were not systematically validated. In Study <span>1</span>, participants rated the subjective discomfort experienced when viewing traditional obsession-provoking threat pictures and novel end-states pictures. We first compared participants with high (HOC) and low (LOC) levels of obsessive-compulsive symptoms, and then clinically diagnosed OCD participants and matched healthy controls. In study <span>2</span>, we compared gaze patterns of HOC and LOC participants while freely viewing 2-by-2 matrices directly contrasting two threat and two end-state stimuli. Study <span>1</span> showed a larger difference in experienced discomfort when viewing the end-state stimuli, compared to threat stimuli, for both OC groups compared with their respected control groups. Study <span>2</span> showed that while LOC participants demonstrated no difference in dwell time on threat vs. end-state pictures, the HOC group allocated more attention toward the latter. Both groups were more prone to fixate first the threat pictures. Task reliability was high. Attention allocation in OCD may also be affected by cues signaling the completion of compulsive acts.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"81 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138679736","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-13DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10051-0
Gerben A. van Kleef, Maybritt Larsen, Eftychia Stamkou
Teachers, parents, and other feedback providers commonly express positive emotions to stimulate learning. When students’ performance is below expectations, however, feedback providers may be inclined to express negative emotions. How these different emotional styles shape students’ development remains poorly understood. Here we investigate the effects of music teachers’ positive versus negative emotional expressions on their students’ musical performance. We draw on emotions as social information (EASI) theory, which postulates that the effects of emotional expressions depend on targets’ information-processing motivation (which determines whether feedback is extracted from emotional expressions) and agreeableness (which determines the perceived appropriateness of positive vs. negative expressions). We followed music teachers and students during regular learning sessions. One week before the sessions, we assessed students’ dispositional information-processing motivation and agreeableness. Immediately after the sessions, students reported on their teachers’ emotional expressions during the session, and teachers rated the performance of students on two musical tasks. An outside expert evaluated recordings of a subset of these performances. Consistent with the EASI framework, students who were confronted with stronger positive emotional expressions of their teachers performed better to the extent that they were lower on information-processing motivation and higher on agreeableness. Conversely, students who were confronted with stronger negative emotional expressions performed better to the degree that they were higher on information-processing motivation and lower on agreeableness. These findings indicate that both positive and negative emotional expressions of teachers can benefit students’ performance, depending on the student’s personality. We discuss implications for feedback, emotions and education.
{"title":"How music teachers’ emotional expressions shape students’ performance: “C’est le ton qui fait la musique”","authors":"Gerben A. van Kleef, Maybritt Larsen, Eftychia Stamkou","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10051-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10051-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Teachers, parents, and other feedback providers commonly express positive emotions to stimulate learning. When students’ performance is below expectations, however, feedback providers may be inclined to express negative emotions. How these different emotional styles shape students’ development remains poorly understood. Here we investigate the effects of music teachers’ positive versus negative emotional expressions on their students’ musical performance. We draw on emotions as social information (EASI) theory, which postulates that the effects of emotional expressions depend on targets’ information-processing motivation (which determines whether feedback is extracted from emotional expressions) and agreeableness (which determines the perceived appropriateness of positive vs. negative expressions). We followed music teachers and students during regular learning sessions. One week before the sessions, we assessed students’ dispositional information-processing motivation and agreeableness. Immediately after the sessions, students reported on their teachers’ emotional expressions during the session, and teachers rated the performance of students on two musical tasks. An outside expert evaluated recordings of a subset of these performances. Consistent with the EASI framework, students who were confronted with stronger positive emotional expressions of their teachers performed better to the extent that they were lower on information-processing motivation and higher on agreeableness. Conversely, students who were confronted with stronger negative emotional expressions performed better to the degree that they were higher on information-processing motivation and lower on agreeableness. These findings indicate that both positive and negative emotional expressions of teachers can benefit students’ performance, depending on the student’s personality. We discuss implications for feedback, emotions and education.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"9 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138629871","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-08DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10050-1
Steffen Wild, Sebastian Rahn, Thomas Meyer
Research shows that in university education programs, students’ motivation decreases over time, which is associated with indicators of reduced academic success, such as student dropout rate. Consequently, researchers have analyzed motivation change and explored the options available to universities to maintain a high level of motivation among students. Using Person-environment fit theory, our research suggests that perceived support offered by lecturers and instructional quality influence students’ subject interest. We conducted a longitudinal design of 823 participants from Baden-Wuerttemberg Cooperative State University and estimated a latent change score model using data collected between the participants’ first and second academic years. Our findings suggest that perceived support from lecturers mitigated the decrease in subject interest. Moreover, our results support the hypothesis that universities can attenuate the decreasing change of subject interest from students. Our findings are contextualized with reference to contemporary research in the field and we offer practical suggestions for maintaining high motivation among students.
{"title":"Factors mitigating the decline of motivation during the first academic year: a latent change score analysis","authors":"Steffen Wild, Sebastian Rahn, Thomas Meyer","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10050-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10050-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Research shows that in university education programs, students’ motivation decreases over time, which is associated with indicators of reduced academic success, such as student dropout rate. Consequently, researchers have analyzed motivation change and explored the options available to universities to maintain a high level of motivation among students. Using Person-environment fit theory, our research suggests that perceived support offered by lecturers and instructional quality influence students’ subject interest. We conducted a longitudinal design of 823 participants from Baden-Wuerttemberg Cooperative State University and estimated a latent change score model using data collected between the participants’ first and second academic years. Our findings suggest that perceived support from lecturers mitigated the decrease in subject interest. Moreover, our results support the hypothesis that universities can attenuate the decreasing change of subject interest from students. Our findings are contextualized with reference to contemporary research in the field and we offer practical suggestions for maintaining high motivation among students.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"252 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138562769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-05DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10049-8
Katherine Muenks, Yiqiu Yan
Recent work has found that undergraduate students’ perceptions of their peers’ intelligence mindsets can either promote or hinder their sense of belonging, motivation, and challenge-seeking behaviors in their courses. In the present exploratory set of studies, we examine what specific behaviors signal to students that their peers have either growth or fixed mindsets. Using open-ended (Study 1) and closed-ended (Studies 2–3) measures, we identify five categories of behavior that signal peers’ mindsets to students: verbal or explicit messages, how much effort their peers exert, their peers’ willingness to help, how competitive their peers are, and the extent to which their peers are self-deprecating. In Study 3, we also find that students’ perceptions of their peers’ growth mindsets in a specific STEM course are associated with higher belonging, and lower emotional cost, self-handicapping, and procrastination in that course, replicating and extending previous work. Implications for college STEM instructors are discussed.
{"title":"Do my peers have a fixed or growth mindset? Exploring the behaviors associated with undergraduate STEM students’ perceptions of their peers’ mindsets about intelligence","authors":"Katherine Muenks, Yiqiu Yan","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10049-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10049-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Recent work has found that undergraduate students’ perceptions of their peers’ intelligence mindsets can either promote or hinder their sense of belonging, motivation, and challenge-seeking behaviors in their courses. In the present exploratory set of studies, we examine what specific behaviors signal to students that their peers have either growth or fixed mindsets. Using open-ended (Study 1) and closed-ended (Studies 2–3) measures, we identify five categories of behavior that signal peers’ mindsets to students: verbal or explicit messages, how much effort their peers exert, their peers’ willingness to help, how competitive their peers are, and the extent to which their peers are self-deprecating. In Study 3, we also find that students’ perceptions of their peers’ growth mindsets in a specific STEM course are associated with higher belonging, and lower emotional cost, self-handicapping, and procrastination in that course, replicating and extending previous work. Implications for college STEM instructors are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"19 1","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138524150","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-12-05DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10047-w
Verity Y. Q. Lua, Wei Ming Ooi, A’isyah Najib, Christine Tan, Nadyanna M. Majeed, Angela K.-y. Leung, Andree Hartanto
While the extent to which individuals engage in and enjoy cognitive abilities, commonly known as need for cognition (NFC), has been suggested to promote adaptive behaviors associated with well-being, there has not been a systematic examination of the strength of the relationship between NFC and well-being. This meta-analysis sought to examine the association between NFC and well-being. Based on 108 effect sizes extracted from 52 samples (50 records), a small to medium positive relationship (r = .20, 95% CI [.16, .23], p < .001) between NFC and well-being was observed. Sub-group analyses revealed that NFC was associated with different aspects of well-being, including life satisfaction, positive affect, negative affect, purpose in life, self-acceptance, personal growth, environmental mastery, positive relations with others, autonomy, depression, anxiety and stress (|r|s = [.07, .45]). Exploratory moderation analyses showed that age moderated the relationship between NFC and well-being, whereby the positive relationship was stronger in younger samples. The gender proportion of the sample also moderated the relationship between NFC and well-being for certain specific measures of well-being, whereby the positive relationship between NFC and well-being was stronger among females.
虽然个人参与和享受认知能力的程度,通常被称为认知需要(NFC),已经被认为可以促进与幸福感相关的适应性行为,但还没有对NFC和幸福感之间关系的强度进行系统的检查。这项荟萃分析试图检验NFC和幸福感之间的关系。基于从52个样本(50条记录)中提取的108个效应量,中小型正相关(r =。20, 95% ci[。[16, .23], p < .001])。亚组分析显示,NFC与幸福感的不同方面相关,包括生活满意度、积极影响、消极影响、生活目标、自我接纳、个人成长、环境掌控、与他人的积极关系、自主性、抑郁、焦虑和压力(|r|s = [s])。07年,.45])。探索性调节分析表明,年龄调节了NFC和幸福感之间的关系,因此,在年轻的样本中,正相关关系更强。对于某些特定的幸福感测量,样本的性别比例也调节了NFC和幸福感之间的关系,即NFC和幸福感之间的正相关关系在女性中更强。
{"title":"Think your way to happiness? Investigating the role of need for cognition in well-being through a three-level meta-analytic approach","authors":"Verity Y. Q. Lua, Wei Ming Ooi, A’isyah Najib, Christine Tan, Nadyanna M. Majeed, Angela K.-y. Leung, Andree Hartanto","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10047-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10047-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p>While the extent to which individuals engage in and enjoy cognitive abilities, commonly known as need for cognition (NFC), has been suggested to promote adaptive behaviors associated with well-being, there has not been a systematic examination of the strength of the relationship between NFC and well-being. This meta-analysis sought to examine the association between NFC and well-being. Based on 108 effect sizes extracted from 52 samples (50 records), a small to medium positive relationship (<i>r</i> = .20, 95% CI [.16, .23], <i>p</i> < .001) between NFC and well-being was observed. Sub-group analyses revealed that NFC was associated with different aspects of well-being, including life satisfaction, positive affect, negative affect, purpose in life, self-acceptance, personal growth, environmental mastery, positive relations with others, autonomy, depression, anxiety and stress (|<i>r|</i>s = [.07, .45]). Exploratory moderation analyses showed that age moderated the relationship between NFC and well-being, whereby the positive relationship was stronger in younger samples. The gender proportion of the sample also moderated the relationship between NFC and well-being for certain specific measures of well-being, whereby the positive relationship between NFC and well-being was stronger among females.</p>","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"30 8","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":4.8,"publicationDate":"2023-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138524142","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-16DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10046-x
Huanhuan Zhao, Heyun Zhang, Zhufang Wang, Wen He
{"title":"Does mortality salience make youths more materialistic? The role of future orientation and awe","authors":"Huanhuan Zhao, Heyun Zhang, Zhufang Wang, Wen He","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10046-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10046-x","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"41 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136115607","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-15DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10041-2
Paton Pak Chun Yam, Fan Huang, Xudong Luo, Brian Parkinson
{"title":"Aligning with the agent of justice: Schadenfreude following punishment of trust violators","authors":"Paton Pak Chun Yam, Fan Huang, Xudong Luo, Brian Parkinson","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10041-2","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10041-2","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"72 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136184718","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
Pub Date : 2023-10-11DOI: 10.1007/s11031-023-10044-z
Marta Viola, Arie W. Kruglanski, Molly Ellenberg, Antonio Pierro
Abstract In line with Significance Quest Theory (SQT, Kruglanski et al., 2022) and Costly Signaling Theory (CST, Zahavi, 1995), the present research aims to investigate the relationship between individual differences in ambition and support for costly (in terms of investment of personal resources) aid programs. Consistent with SQT, which holds that the quest for significance is a universal need that may lead to any type (e.g., violent or prosocial) of extreme behavior in order to satisfy it, we hypothesized that ambitious (vs. less ambitious) people are more motivated to engage in resource-intensive aid programs. In four studies (Total N = 744), both correlational (Studies 1 and 4) and experimental (Studies 2 and 3), we found a significant positive relationship between levels of ambition and support for resource-intensive aid programs; this relationship was mediated by difficulty perceived as important, i.e., the attribution of high value to difficult tasks and goals (Study 4).
摘要本研究根据重要性探索理论(SQT, Kruglanski et al., 2022)和代价信号理论(CST, Zahavi, 1995),旨在探讨个体野心差异与支持代价高昂(以个人资源投入为标准)援助计划之间的关系。SQT认为,对意义的追求是一种普遍的需求,可能导致任何类型的极端行为(例如,暴力或亲社会)来满足它,与此一致,我们假设雄心勃勃(与不那么雄心勃勃)的人更有动力参与资源密集型援助计划。在相关研究(研究1和4)和实验研究(研究2和3)的四项研究(总N = 744)中,我们发现抱负水平与对资源密集型援助计划的支持之间存在显著的正相关关系;这种关系被认为是重要的困难所中介,即对困难任务和目标的高价值归因(研究4)。
{"title":"Ambitious people are more prone to support resource-intensive aid programs","authors":"Marta Viola, Arie W. Kruglanski, Molly Ellenberg, Antonio Pierro","doi":"10.1007/s11031-023-10044-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-023-10044-z","url":null,"abstract":"Abstract In line with Significance Quest Theory (SQT, Kruglanski et al., 2022) and Costly Signaling Theory (CST, Zahavi, 1995), the present research aims to investigate the relationship between individual differences in ambition and support for costly (in terms of investment of personal resources) aid programs. Consistent with SQT, which holds that the quest for significance is a universal need that may lead to any type (e.g., violent or prosocial) of extreme behavior in order to satisfy it, we hypothesized that ambitious (vs. less ambitious) people are more motivated to engage in resource-intensive aid programs. In four studies (Total N = 744), both correlational (Studies 1 and 4) and experimental (Studies 2 and 3), we found a significant positive relationship between levels of ambition and support for resource-intensive aid programs; this relationship was mediated by difficulty perceived as important, i.e., the attribution of high value to difficult tasks and goals (Study 4).","PeriodicalId":48282,"journal":{"name":"Motivation and Emotion","volume":"198 1","pages":"0"},"PeriodicalIF":0.0,"publicationDate":"2023-10-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"136210342","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}