首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
Large Language Models: A Historical and Sociocultural Perspective 大语言模型:历史与社会文化视角。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13430
Eugene Yu Ji

This letter explores the intricate historical and contemporary links between large language models (LLMs) and cognitive science through the lens of information theory, statistical language models, and socioanthropological linguistic theories. The emergence of LLMs highlights the enduring significance of information-based and statistical learning theories in understanding human communication. These theories, initially proposed in the mid-20th century, offered a visionary framework for integrating computational science, social sciences, and humanities, which nonetheless was not fully fulfilled at that time. The subsequent development of sociolinguistics and linguistic anthropology, especially since the 1970s, provided critical perspectives and empirical methods that both challenged and enriched this framework. This letter proposes that two pivotal concepts derived from this development, metapragmatic function and indexicality, offer a fruitful theoretical perspective for integrating the semantic, textual, and pragmatic, contextual dimensions of communication, an amalgamation that contemporary LLMs have yet to fully achieve. The author believes that contemporary cognitive science is at a crucial crossroads, where fostering interdisciplinary dialogues among computational linguistics, social linguistics and linguistic anthropology, and cognitive and social psychology is in particular imperative. Such collaboration is vital to bridge the computational, cognitive, and sociocultural aspects of human communication and human−AI interaction, especially in the era of large language and multimodal models and human-centric Artificial Intelligence (AI).

这封信通过信息论、统计语言模型和社会人类学语言理论的视角,探讨了大型语言模型(LLMs)与认知科学之间错综复杂的历史和当代联系。大型语言模型的出现凸显了以信息为基础的统计学习理论在理解人类交流方面的持久意义。这些理论最初是在 20 世纪中期提出的,为整合计算科学、社会科学和人文科学提供了一个富有远见的框架,但在当时并未完全实现。随后,社会语言学和语言人类学的发展,尤其是 20 世纪 70 年代以来的发展,提供了批判性的视角和实证方法,既挑战了这一框架,也丰富了这一框架。这封信提出,从这一发展中衍生出的两个关键概念--元语用功能和索引性--为整合交际的语义、文本和语用、语境维度提供了富有成效的理论视角,而当代语言学硕士尚未完全实现这一整合。作者认为,当代认知科学正处于一个关键的十字路口,促进计算语言学、社会语言学和语言人类学以及认知和社会心理学之间的跨学科对话尤为必要。这种合作对于沟通人类交流和人机交互的计算、认知和社会文化方面至关重要,尤其是在大型语言和多模态模型以及以人为本的人工智能(AI)时代。
{"title":"Large Language Models: A Historical and Sociocultural Perspective","authors":"Eugene Yu Ji","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13430","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13430","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This letter explores the intricate historical and contemporary links between large language models (LLMs) and cognitive science through the lens of information theory, statistical language models, and socioanthropological linguistic theories. The emergence of LLMs highlights the enduring significance of information-based and statistical learning theories in understanding human communication. These theories, initially proposed in the mid-20th century, offered a visionary framework for integrating computational science, social sciences, and humanities, which nonetheless was not fully fulfilled at that time. The subsequent development of sociolinguistics and linguistic anthropology, especially since the 1970s, provided critical perspectives and empirical methods that both challenged and enriched this framework. This letter proposes that two pivotal concepts derived from this development, metapragmatic function and indexicality, offer a fruitful theoretical perspective for integrating the semantic, textual, and pragmatic, contextual dimensions of communication, an amalgamation that contemporary LLMs have yet to fully achieve. The author believes that contemporary cognitive science is at a crucial crossroads, where fostering interdisciplinary dialogues among computational linguistics, social linguistics and linguistic anthropology, and cognitive and social psychology is in particular imperative. Such collaboration is vital to bridge the computational, cognitive, and sociocultural aspects of human communication and human−AI interaction, especially in the era of large language and multimodal models and human-centric Artificial Intelligence (AI).</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140159272","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Emotional Content of Children's Writing: A Data-Driven Approach 儿童写作的情感内容:数据驱动法。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13423
Yuzhen Dong, Yaling Hsiao, Nicola Dawson, Nilanjana Banerji, Kate Nation

Emotion is closely associated with language, but we know very little about how children express emotion in their own writing. We used a large-scale, cross-sectional, and data-driven approach to investigate emotional expression via writing in children of different ages, and whether it varies for boys and girls. We first used a lexicon-based bag-of-words approach to identify emotional content in a large corpus of stories (N>100,000) written by 7- to 13-year-old children. Generalized Additive Models were then used to model changes in sentiment across age and gender. Two other machine learning approaches (BERT and TextBlob) validated and extended these analyses, converging on the finding that positive sentiments in children's writing decrease with age. These findings echo reports from previous studies showing a decrease in mood and an increased use of negative emotion words with age. We also found that stories by girls contained more positive sentiments than stories by boys. Our study shows the utility of large-scale data-driven approaches to reveal the content and nature of children's writing. Future experimental work should build on these observations to understand the likely complex relationships between written language and emotion, and how these change over development.

情感与语言密切相关,但我们对儿童如何在自己的写作中表达情感却知之甚少。我们采用了大规模、横断面和数据驱动的方法,研究不同年龄段儿童通过写作表达情感的情况,以及男孩和女孩在这方面是否存在差异。首先,我们使用基于词库的词袋法来识别 7 至 13 岁儿童所写的大量故事语料库(N>100,000)中的情感内容。然后使用广义加性模型(Generalized Additive Models)对不同年龄和性别的情感变化进行建模。另外两种机器学习方法(BERT 和 TextBlob)验证并扩展了这些分析,最终得出结论:儿童写作中的积极情绪会随着年龄的增长而减少。这些发现与之前的研究报告相呼应,后者表明随着年龄的增长,儿童的情绪会下降,负面情绪词语的使用会增加。我们还发现,女孩的故事比男孩的故事包含更多的积极情绪。我们的研究表明,大规模数据驱动方法对揭示儿童写作的内容和性质很有帮助。未来的实验工作应以这些观察结果为基础,以了解书面语言与情感之间可能存在的复杂关系,以及这些关系如何随着发展而变化。
{"title":"The Emotional Content of Children's Writing: A Data-Driven Approach","authors":"Yuzhen Dong,&nbsp;Yaling Hsiao,&nbsp;Nicola Dawson,&nbsp;Nilanjana Banerji,&nbsp;Kate Nation","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13423","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13423","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Emotion is closely associated with language, but we know very little about how children express emotion in their own writing. We used a large-scale, cross-sectional, and data-driven approach to investigate emotional expression via writing in children of different ages, and whether it varies for boys and girls. We first used a lexicon-based bag-of-words approach to identify emotional content in a large corpus of stories (<i>N</i>&gt;100,000) written by 7- to 13-year-old children. Generalized Additive Models were then used to model changes in sentiment across age and gender. Two other machine learning approaches (<i>BERT</i> and <i>TextBlob</i>) validated and extended these analyses, converging on the finding that positive sentiments in children's writing decrease with age. These findings echo reports from previous studies showing a decrease in mood and an increased use of negative emotion words with age. We also found that stories by girls contained more positive sentiments than stories by boys. Our study shows the utility of large-scale data-driven approaches to reveal the content and nature of children's writing. Future experimental work should build on these observations to understand the likely complex relationships between written language and emotion, and how these change over development.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13423","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140144391","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inducing Novel Sound–Taste Correspondences via an Associative Learning Task 通过联想学习任务诱发新的声味对应关系
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13421
Francisco Barbosa Escobar, Qian Janice Wang

The interest in crossmodal correspondences, including those involving sounds and involving tastes, has experienced rapid growth in recent years. However, the mechanisms underlying these correspondences are not well understood. In the present study (N = 302), we used an associative learning paradigm, based on previous literature using simple sounds with no consensual taste associations (i.e., square and triangle wave sounds at 200 Hz) and taste words (i.e., sweet and bitter), to test the influence of two potential mechanisms in establishing sound–taste correspondences and investigate whether either learning mechanism could give rise to new and long-lasting associations. Specifically, we examined an emotional mediation account (i.e., using sad and happy emoji facial expressions) and a transitive path (i.e., sound-taste correspondence being mediated by color, using red and black colored squares). The results revealed that the associative learning paradigm mapping the triangle wave tone with a happy emoji facial expression induced a novel crossmodal correspondence between this sound and the word sweet. Importantly, we found that this novel association was still present two months after the experimental learning paradigm. None of the other mappings, emotional or transitive, gave rise to any significant associations between sound and taste. These findings provide evidence that new crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes can be created by leveraging the affective connection between both dimensions, helping elucidate the mechanisms underlying these associations. Moreover, these findings reveal that these associations can last for several weeks after the experimental session through which they were induced.

近年来,人们对跨模态对应(包括涉及声音和味道的对应)的兴趣迅速增长。然而,人们对这些对应关系的内在机制还不甚了解。在本研究(N = 302)中,我们在以往文献的基础上使用了一个联想学习范式,利用没有一致味觉联想的简单声音(即 200 Hz 的方波和三角波声音)和味觉词(即甜和苦)来测试建立声音-味觉对应的两种潜在机制的影响,并研究这两种学习机制是否都能产生新的、持久的联想。具体来说,我们研究了情感中介(即使用悲伤和快乐的表情符号)和传递路径(即声音-味道对应由颜色中介,使用红色和黑色的彩色方块)。研究结果表明,将三角波音调与快乐表情符号进行映射的联想学习范式诱导了这种声音与甜味一词之间的新型跨模态对应关系。重要的是,我们发现这种新联想在实验学习范式进行两个月后仍然存在。其他的映射,无论是情感映射还是转义映射,都没有在声音和味道之间产生任何显著的关联。这些发现证明,声音和味道之间新的跨模态对应关系可以通过利用这两个维度之间的情感联系而产生,有助于阐明这些关联的内在机制。此外,这些研究结果还表明,这些联想可以在诱发联想的实验环节结束后持续数周之久。
{"title":"Inducing Novel Sound–Taste Correspondences via an Associative Learning Task","authors":"Francisco Barbosa Escobar,&nbsp;Qian Janice Wang","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13421","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13421","url":null,"abstract":"<p>The interest in crossmodal correspondences, including those involving sounds and involving tastes, has experienced rapid growth in recent years. However, the mechanisms underlying these correspondences are not well understood. In the present study (<i>N</i> = 302), we used an associative learning paradigm, based on previous literature using simple sounds with no consensual taste associations (i.e., square and triangle wave sounds at 200 Hz) and taste words (i.e., sweet and bitter), to test the influence of two potential mechanisms in establishing sound–taste correspondences and investigate whether either learning mechanism could give rise to new and long-lasting associations. Specifically, we examined an emotional mediation account (i.e., using sad and happy emoji facial expressions) and a transitive path (i.e., sound-taste correspondence being mediated by color, using red and black colored squares). The results revealed that the associative learning paradigm mapping the triangle wave tone with a happy emoji facial expression induced a novel crossmodal correspondence between this sound and the word sweet. Importantly, we found that this novel association was still present two months after the experimental learning paradigm. None of the other mappings, emotional or transitive, gave rise to any significant associations between sound and taste. These findings provide evidence that new crossmodal correspondences between sounds and tastes can be created by leveraging the affective connection between both dimensions, helping elucidate the mechanisms underlying these associations. Moreover, these findings reveal that these associations can last for several weeks after the experimental session through which they were induced.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13421","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140159271","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Probing the Representational Structure of Regular Polysemy via Sense Analogy Questions: Insights from Contextual Word Vectors 通过意义类比问题探究常规多义词的表征结构:从语境词向量中获得的启示。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13416
Jiangtian Li, Blair C. Armstrong

Regular polysemes are sets of ambiguous words that all share the same relationship between their meanings, such as CHICKEN and LOBSTER both referring to an animal or its meat. To probe how a distributional semantic model, here exemplified by bidirectional encoder representations from transformers (BERT), represents regular polysemy, we analyzed whether its embeddings support answering sense analogy questions similar to “is the mapping between CHICKEN (as an animal) and CHICKEN (as a meat) similar to that which maps between LOBSTER (as an animal) to LOBSTER (as a meat)?” We did so using the LRcos model, which combines a logistic regression classifier of different categories (e.g., animal vs. meat) with a measure of cosine similarity. We found that (a) the model was sensitive to the shared structure within a given regular relationship; (b) the shared structure varies across different regular relationships (e.g., animal/meat vs. location/organization), potentially reflective of a “regularity continuum;” (c) some high-order latent structure is shared across different regular relationships, suggestive of a similar latent structure across different types of relationships; and (d) there is a lack of evidence for the aforementioned effects being explained by meaning overlap. Lastly, we found that both components of the LRcos model made important contributions to accurate responding and that a variation of this method could yield an accuracy boost of 10% in answering sense analogy questions. These findings enrich previous theoretical work on regular polysemy with a computationally explicit theory and methods, and provide evidence for an important organizational principle for the mental lexicon and the broader conceptual knowledge system.

正则多义词是指词义之间具有相同关系的模棱两可的词组,例如 CHICKEN 和 LOBSTER 都指一种动物或其肉。为了探究分布式语义模型(这里以来自转换器的双向编码器表示法(BERT)为例)如何表示规则多义词,我们分析了其嵌入是否支持回答类似于 "CHICKEN(作为一种动物)和CHICKEN(作为一种肉)之间的映射是否类似于LOBSTER(作为一种动物)和LOBSTER(作为一种肉)之间的映射 "这样的意义类比问题?我们使用了 LRcos 模型,该模型将不同类别(如动物与肉类)的逻辑回归分类器与余弦相似度测量相结合。我们发现:(a) 该模型对特定常规关系中的共享结构很敏感;(b) 共享结构在不同的常规关系(如动物/肉类与地点/组织)中各不相同,可能反映了 "常规性连续体";(c) 一些高阶潜在结构在不同的常规关系中是共享的,表明不同类型的关系中存在相似的潜在结构;(d) 缺乏证据表明上述效应可以用意义重叠来解释。最后,我们发现 LRcos 模型的两个组成部分都对准确回答做出了重要贡献,而且这种方法的变体可以使回答意义类比问题的准确率提高 10%。这些研究结果丰富了之前关于规则多义词的理论研究,提供了一种计算明确的理论和方法,并为心理词典和更广泛的概念知识系统的一个重要组织原则提供了证据。
{"title":"Probing the Representational Structure of Regular Polysemy via Sense Analogy Questions: Insights from Contextual Word Vectors","authors":"Jiangtian Li,&nbsp;Blair C. Armstrong","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13416","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13416","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Regular polysemes are sets of ambiguous words that all share the same relationship between their meanings, such as CHICKEN and LOBSTER both referring to an animal or its meat. To probe how a distributional semantic model, here exemplified by bidirectional encoder representations from transformers (BERT), represents regular polysemy, we analyzed whether its embeddings support answering sense analogy questions similar to “is the mapping between CHICKEN (as an animal) and CHICKEN (as a meat) similar to that which maps between LOBSTER (as an animal) to LOBSTER (as a meat)?” We did so using the LRcos model, which combines a logistic regression classifier of different categories (e.g., animal vs. meat) with a measure of cosine similarity. We found that (a) the model was sensitive to the shared structure within a given regular relationship; (b) the shared structure varies across different regular relationships (e.g., animal/meat vs. location/organization), potentially reflective of a “regularity continuum;” (c) some high-order latent structure is shared across different regular relationships, suggestive of a similar latent structure across different types of relationships; and (d) there is a lack of evidence for the aforementioned effects being explained by meaning overlap. Lastly, we found that both components of the LRcos model made important contributions to accurate responding and that a variation of this method could yield an accuracy boost of 10% in answering sense analogy questions. These findings enrich previous theoretical work on regular polysemy with a computationally explicit theory and methods, and provide evidence for an important organizational principle for the mental lexicon and the broader conceptual knowledge system.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13416","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140121128","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Enriching Thinking Through Discourse 通过话语丰富思维
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13420
Deanna Kuhn, Sybille Bruun, Caroline Geithner

Great effort is invested in identifying ways to change people's minds on an issue. A first priority should perhaps be enriching their thinking about the issue. With a goal of enriching their thinking, we studied the views of community adults on the DACA issue—young adults who entered the United States illegally as children. A dialogic method was employed, offering dual benefits in providing participants the opportunity to further develop their own ideas and to consider differing ideas. Yet, participants engaged in dialog only vicariously by observing the talk of a pair of actors who held opposing positions on DACA. The effect on participants’ thinking was greatest in the condition in which they viewed a dialog between the two actors, rather than a comparison condition in which the actors individually expressed their positions. In control conditions, no presentation was observed. Probing questions included in all conditions encouraged a participant to examine and clarify for themselves their own position, potentially enriching it. This condition proved unsuccessful in enriching thinking; participants’ justifications for their own positions in fact became simpler and less qualified. In contrast, observing a video of a like-minded and opposing other did enrich observers’ thinking, yet to a greater degree in the dialogic than nondialogic condition. The findings thus suggest observed dialog as a promising practical approach in promoting deeper thinking.

在确定如何改变人们对某一问题的看法方面,我们付出了巨大的努力。首要任务或许应该是丰富他们对问题的思考。以丰富他们的思维为目标,我们研究了社区成年人对 DACA 问题的看法--这些成年人在童年时非法进入美国。我们采用了对话法,这种方法有双重好处,既能让参与者进一步发展自己的观点,又能让他们考虑不同的观点。然而,参与者只是通过观察一对在 DACA 问题上持相反立场的行动者的谈话来参与对话。对参与者思维的影响最大的是他们观看两位演员对话的条件,而不是演员单独表达立场的对比条件。在对照条件下,没有观察到任何陈述。在所有条件中都包含了探究性问题,鼓励参与者审视并澄清自己的立场,从而丰富自己的立场。事实证明,这种条件在丰富思维方面并不成功;事实上,参与者为自己的立场提出的理由变得更加简单和不那么有说服力。与此相反,观察志同道合者和反对者的视频确实丰富了观察者的思维,但在对话条件下比非对话条件下的程度更高。因此,研究结果表明,观察对话是促进深入思考的一种很有前途的实用方法。
{"title":"Enriching Thinking Through Discourse","authors":"Deanna Kuhn,&nbsp;Sybille Bruun,&nbsp;Caroline Geithner","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13420","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13420","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Great effort is invested in identifying ways to change people's minds on an issue. A first priority should perhaps be enriching their thinking about the issue. With a goal of enriching their thinking, we studied the views of community adults on the DACA issue—young adults who entered the United States illegally as children. A dialogic method was employed, offering dual benefits in providing participants the opportunity to further develop their own ideas and to consider differing ideas. Yet, participants engaged in dialog only vicariously by observing the talk of a pair of actors who held opposing positions on DACA. The effect on participants’ thinking was greatest in the condition in which they viewed a dialog between the two actors, rather than a comparison condition in which the actors individually expressed their positions. In control conditions, no presentation was observed. Probing questions included in all conditions encouraged a participant to examine and clarify for themselves their own position, potentially enriching it. This condition proved unsuccessful in enriching thinking; participants’ justifications for their own positions in fact became simpler and less qualified. In contrast, observing a video of a like-minded and opposing other did enrich observers’ thinking, yet to a greater degree in the dialogic than nondialogic condition. The findings thus suggest observed dialog as a promising practical approach in promoting deeper thinking.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140121126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncertain Facts or Uncertain Values? Testing the Distinction Between Empirical and Normative Uncertainty in Moral Judgments 不确定的事实还是不确定的价值观?检验道德判断中经验不确定性与规范不确定性的区别。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13422
Maximilian Theisen, Markus Germar

People can be uncertain in their moral judgments. Philosophers have argued that such uncertainty can either refer to the underlying empirical facts (empirical uncertainty) or to the normative evaluation of these facts itself (normative uncertainty). Psychological investigations of this distinction, however, are rare. In this paper, we combined factor-analytical and experimental approaches to show that empirical and normative uncertainty describe two related but different psychological states. In Study 1, we asked N = 265 participants to describe a case of moral uncertainty and to rate different aspects of their uncertainty about this case. Across this wide range of moral scenarios, our items loaded onto three reliable factors: lack of information, unclear consequences, and normative uncertainty. In Study 2, we confirmed this factor structure using predefined stimulus material. N = 402 participants each rated eight scenarios that systematically varied in their degree of uncertainty regarding the consequences of the described actions and in the value conflict that was inherent to them. The empirical uncertainty factors were mainly affected by the introduction of uncertainty regarding consequences, and the normative uncertainty factor was mainly affected by the introduction of value conflict. Our studies provide evidence that the distinction between empirical and normative uncertainty accurately describes a psychological reality. We discuss the relevance of our findings for research on moral judgments and decision-making, and folk metaethics.

人们的道德判断可能是不确定的。哲学家们认为,这种不确定性既可以指基本的经验事实(经验不确定性),也可以指对这些事实本身的规范性评价(规范不确定性)。然而,对这一区别的心理学研究并不多见。在本文中,我们将因素分析法和实验法结合起来,证明经验性不确定性和规范性不确定性描述了两种相关但不同的心理状态。在研究 1 中,我们要求 N = 265 名参与者描述一个道德不确定性案例,并对其不确定性的不同方面进行评分。在这些广泛的道德情景中,我们的项目包含三个可靠的因子:缺乏信息、后果不明确和规范不确定性。在研究 2 中,我们使用预定义的刺激材料证实了这一因子结构。N = 402 名参与者分别对八种情景进行了评分,这些情景在所述行为后果的不确定性程度以及内在价值冲突方面存在系统性差异。经验不确定性因素主要受到后果不确定性的影响,而规范不确定性因素主要受到价值冲突的影响。我们的研究证明,经验不确定性和规范不确定性之间的区别准确地描述了心理现实。我们讨论了我们的研究结果对道德判断和决策以及民间元伦理学研究的意义。
{"title":"Uncertain Facts or Uncertain Values? Testing the Distinction Between Empirical and Normative Uncertainty in Moral Judgments","authors":"Maximilian Theisen,&nbsp;Markus Germar","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13422","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13422","url":null,"abstract":"<p>People can be uncertain in their moral judgments. Philosophers have argued that such uncertainty can either refer to the underlying empirical facts (empirical uncertainty) or to the normative evaluation of these facts itself (normative uncertainty). Psychological investigations of this distinction, however, are rare. In this paper, we combined factor-analytical and experimental approaches to show that empirical and normative uncertainty describe two related but different psychological states. In Study 1, we asked <i>N</i> = 265 participants to describe a case of moral uncertainty and to rate different aspects of their uncertainty about this case. Across this wide range of moral scenarios, our items loaded onto three reliable factors: lack of information, unclear consequences, and normative uncertainty. In Study 2, we confirmed this factor structure using predefined stimulus material. <i>N</i> = 402 participants each rated eight scenarios that systematically varied in their degree of uncertainty regarding the consequences of the described actions and in the value conflict that was inherent to them. The empirical uncertainty factors were mainly affected by the introduction of uncertainty regarding consequences, and the normative uncertainty factor was mainly affected by the introduction of value conflict. Our studies provide evidence that the distinction between empirical and normative uncertainty accurately describes a psychological reality. We discuss the relevance of our findings for research on moral judgments and decision-making, and folk metaethics.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13422","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140121129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Lexical Alignment is Pervasive Across Contexts in Non-WEIRD Adult–Child Interactions 在非 WEIRD 的成人与儿童互动中,词汇对齐在不同语境中普遍存在。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-13 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13417
Adriana Chee Jing Chieng, Camille J. Wynn, Tze Peng Wong, Tyson S Barrett, Stephanie A. Borrie

Lexical alignment, a communication phenomenon where conversational partners adapt their word choices to become more similar, plays an important role in the development of language and social communication skills. While this has been studied extensively in the conversations of preschool-aged children and their parents in Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) communities, research in other pediatric populations is sparse. This study makes significant expansions on the existing literature by focusing on alignment in naturalistic conversations of school-aged children from a non-WEIRD population across multiple conversational tasks and with different types of adult partners. Typically developing children aged 5 to 8 years (n = 45) engaged in four semi-structured conversations that differed by task (problem-solving vs. play-based) and by partner (parent vs. university student), resulting in a corpus of 180 conversations. Lexical alignment scores were calculated and compared to sham conversations, representing alignment occurring at the level of chance. Both children and adults coordinated their conversational utterances by re-using or aligning each other's word choices. This alignment behavior persisted across conversational tasks and partners, although the degree of alignment was moderated by the conversational context. These findings suggest that lexical alignment is a robust phenomenon in conversations between school-age children and adults. Furthermore, this study extends lexical alignment findings to a non-WEIRD culture, suggesting that alignment may be a coordination strategy employed by adults and children across diverse linguistic and cultural backgrounds.

词性一致是一种交流现象,会话伙伴会调整他们的用词选择,使之变得更加相似,这在语言和社会交流技能的发展中起着重要作用。在西方、教育、工业化、富裕和民主(WEIRD)社区,人们对学龄前儿童及其父母的对话进行了广泛的研究,但对其他儿童群体的研究却很少。本研究在现有文献的基础上进行了重大扩展,重点研究了非西方教育、工业、富裕和民主国家(WEIRD)人群中学龄儿童与不同类型成人伙伴在多个对话任务中自然对话的一致性。5 至 8 岁发育正常的儿童(n = 45)参与了四次半结构化对话,对话任务(解决问题与游戏)和对话伙伴(父母与大学生)各不相同,因此产生了 180 个对话语料库。我们计算了词汇一致性得分,并将其与假对话进行了比较,这代表了在偶然水平上出现的一致性。儿童和成人都通过重复使用或对齐对方的选词来协调他们的会话。这种对齐行为在不同的会话任务和伙伴中都持续存在,尽管对齐程度受会话语境的影响。这些研究结果表明,在学龄儿童与成人的会话中,词汇排列是一种强有力的现象。此外,本研究还将词汇对齐的发现扩展到了非 WEIRD 文化中,表明对齐可能是成人和儿童在不同语言和文化背景下使用的一种协调策略。
{"title":"Lexical Alignment is Pervasive Across Contexts in Non-WEIRD Adult–Child Interactions","authors":"Adriana Chee Jing Chieng,&nbsp;Camille J. Wynn,&nbsp;Tze Peng Wong,&nbsp;Tyson S Barrett,&nbsp;Stephanie A. Borrie","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13417","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13417","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Lexical alignment, a communication phenomenon where conversational partners adapt their word choices to become more similar, plays an important role in the development of language and social communication skills. While this has been studied extensively in the conversations of preschool-aged children and their parents in Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic (WEIRD) communities, research in other pediatric populations is sparse. This study makes significant expansions on the existing literature by focusing on alignment in naturalistic conversations of school-aged children from a non-WEIRD population across multiple conversational tasks and with different types of adult partners. Typically developing children aged 5 to 8 years (<i>n</i> = 45) engaged in four semi-structured conversations that differed by task (problem-solving vs. play-based) and by partner (parent vs. university student), resulting in a corpus of 180 conversations. Lexical alignment scores were calculated and compared to sham conversations, representing alignment occurring at the level of chance. Both children and adults coordinated their conversational utterances by re-using or aligning each other's word choices. This alignment behavior persisted across conversational tasks and partners, although the degree of alignment was moderated by the conversational context. These findings suggest that lexical alignment is a robust phenomenon in conversations between school-age children and adults. Furthermore, this study extends lexical alignment findings to a non-WEIRD culture, suggesting that alignment may be a coordination strategy employed by adults and children across diverse linguistic and cultural backgrounds.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140121127","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The Role of Feedback in the Statistical Learning of Language-Like Regularities 反馈在语言类规律统计学习中的作用
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13419
Felicity F. Frinsel, Fabio Trecca, Morten H. Christiansen

In language learning, learners engage with their environment, incorporating cues from different sources. However, in lab-based experiments, using artificial languages, many of the cues and features that are part of real-world language learning are stripped away. In three experiments, we investigated the role of positive, negative, and mixed feedback on the gradual learning of language-like statistical regularities within an active guessing game paradigm. In Experiment 1, participants received deterministic feedback (100%), whereas probabilistic feedback (i.e., 75% or 50%) was introduced in Experiment 2. Finally, Experiment 3 explored the impact of mixed probabilistic feedback (33% positive, 33% negative, 33% no feedback). The results showed that cross-situational learning of words was observed without feedback, but participants were able to learn structural regularities of the miniature language only when feedback was provided. Interestingly, the presence of positive feedback was particularly helpful for the learner, promoting more in-depth learning of the artificial language.

在语言学习过程中,学习者与周围环境打交道,从不同来源获得提示。然而,在使用人工语言的实验室实验中,真实世界语言学习中的许多线索和特征都被剥夺了。在三个实验中,我们研究了积极、消极和混合反馈对在主动猜测游戏范式中逐步学习类似语言的统计规律的作用。在实验 1 中,参与者接受的是确定性反馈(100%),而在实验 2 中则引入了概率反馈(即 75% 或 50%)。最后,实验 3 探讨了混合概率反馈(33% 正面、33% 负面、33% 无反馈)的影响。结果表明,在没有反馈的情况下,参与者可以观察到单词的跨情景学习,但只有在提供反馈的情况下,参与者才能学习到微型语言的结构规律。有趣的是,积极反馈对学习者特别有帮助,能促进他们更深入地学习人工语言。
{"title":"The Role of Feedback in the Statistical Learning of Language-Like Regularities","authors":"Felicity F. Frinsel,&nbsp;Fabio Trecca,&nbsp;Morten H. Christiansen","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13419","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13419","url":null,"abstract":"<p>In language learning, learners engage with their environment, incorporating cues from different sources. However, in lab-based experiments, using artificial languages, many of the cues and features that are part of real-world language learning are stripped away. In three experiments, we investigated the role of positive, negative, and mixed feedback on the gradual learning of language-like statistical regularities within an active guessing game paradigm. In Experiment 1, participants received deterministic feedback (100%), whereas probabilistic feedback (i.e., 75% or 50%) was introduced in Experiment 2. Finally, Experiment 3 explored the impact of mixed probabilistic feedback (33% positive, 33% negative, 33% no feedback). The results showed that cross-situational learning of words was observed without feedback, but participants were able to learn structural regularities of the miniature language only when feedback was provided. Interestingly, the presence of positive feedback was particularly helpful for the learner, promoting more in-depth learning of the artificial language.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140023052","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive Science From the Perspective of Linguistic Diversity 从语言多样性的角度看认知科学。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13418
Yoolim Kim, Annika Tjuka

This letter addresses two issues in language research that are important to cognitive science: the comparability of word meanings across languages and the neglect of an integrated approach to writing systems. The first issue challenges generativist claims by emphasizing the importance of comparability of data, drawing on typologists’ findings about different languages. The second issue addresses the exclusion of diverse writing systems from linguistic investigation and argues for a more extensive study of their effects on language and cognition. We argue for a refocusing of cognitive science research on linguistic diversity in all modalities to develop the most robust understanding of language and its role in human cognition more broadly.

这封信讨论了语言研究中对认知科学非常重要的两个问题:不同语言之间词义的可比性以及对书写系统综合方法的忽视。第一个问题借鉴了类型学家对不同语言的研究成果,强调了数据可比性的重要性,从而挑战了类型学家的主张。第二部分讨论了将不同的书写系统排除在语言学研究之外的问题,并主张更广泛地研究它们对语言和认知的影响。我们主张将认知科学研究的重点重新放在所有模式的语言多样性上,以便更广泛地了解语言及其在人类认知中的作用。
{"title":"Cognitive Science From the Perspective of Linguistic Diversity","authors":"Yoolim Kim,&nbsp;Annika Tjuka","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13418","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13418","url":null,"abstract":"<p>This letter addresses two issues in language research that are important to cognitive science: the comparability of word meanings across languages and the neglect of an integrated approach to writing systems. The first issue challenges generativist claims by emphasizing the importance of comparability of data, drawing on typologists’ findings about different languages. The second issue addresses the exclusion of diverse writing systems from linguistic investigation and argues for a more extensive study of their effects on language and cognition. We argue for a refocusing of cognitive science research on linguistic diversity in all modalities to develop the most robust understanding of language and its role in human cognition more broadly.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13418","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974001","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Integrating Social Cognition Into Domain-General Control: Interactive Activation and Competition for the Control of Action (ICON) 将社会认知融入领域总体控制:行动控制的互动激活与竞争 (ICON)。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13415
Robert Ward, Richard Ramsey

Social cognition differs from general cognition in its focus on understanding, perceiving, and interpreting social information. However, we argue that the significance of domain-general processes for controlling cognition has been historically undervalued in social cognition and social neuroscience research. We suggest much of social cognition can be characterized as specialized feature representations supported by domain-general cognitive control systems. To test this proposal, we develop a comprehensive working model, based on an interactive activation and competition architecture and applied to the control of action. As such, we label the model “ICON” (interactive activation and competition model for the control of action). We used the ICON model to simulate human performance across various laboratory tasks. Our simulations emphasize that many laboratory-based social tasks do not require socially specific control systems, such as those that are argued to rely on neural networks associated with theory-of-mind. Moreover, our model clarifies that perceived disruptions in social cognition, even in what appears to be disruption to the control of social cognition, can stem from deficits in social representation instead. We advocate for a “default stance” in social cognition, where control is usually general, but representation is specific. This study underscores the importance of integrating social cognition within the broader realm of domain-general control processing, offering a unified perspective on task processing.

社会认知不同于一般认知,它侧重于理解、感知和解释社会信息。然而,我们认为,在社会认知和社会神经科学研究中,控制认知的领域一般过程的重要性一直被低估。我们认为,社会认知的大部分特征可以归结为由领域通用认知控制系统支持的专门化特征表征。为了验证这一提议,我们开发了一个基于互动激活和竞争架构的综合工作模型,并将其应用于行动控制。因此,我们将该模型命名为 "ICON"(行动控制的交互激活和竞争模型)。我们使用 ICON 模型模拟了人类在各种实验室任务中的表现。我们的模拟强调,许多基于实验室的社会任务并不需要特定的社会控制系统,例如那些被认为依赖于与心智理论相关的神经网络的任务。此外,我们的模型还阐明,社会认知中的感知干扰,甚至是看似对社会认知控制的干扰,都可能源于社会表征的缺陷。我们主张在社会认知中采取 "默认立场",即控制通常是一般性的,而表征则是特定的。这项研究强调了将社会认知整合到更广泛的领域一般控制处理中的重要性,为任务处理提供了一个统一的视角。
{"title":"Integrating Social Cognition Into Domain-General Control: Interactive Activation and Competition for the Control of Action (ICON)","authors":"Robert Ward,&nbsp;Richard Ramsey","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13415","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13415","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Social cognition differs from general cognition in its focus on understanding, perceiving, and interpreting social information. However, we argue that the significance of domain-general processes for controlling cognition has been historically undervalued in social cognition and social neuroscience research. We suggest much of social cognition can be characterized as specialized feature representations supported by domain-general cognitive control systems. To test this proposal, we develop a comprehensive working model, based on an interactive activation and competition architecture and applied to the control of action. As such, we label the model “ICON” (interactive activation and competition model for the control of action). We used the ICON model to simulate human performance across various laboratory tasks. Our simulations emphasize that many laboratory-based social tasks do not require socially specific control systems, such as those that are argued to rely on neural networks associated with theory-of-mind. Moreover, our model clarifies that perceived disruptions in social cognition, even in what appears to be disruption to the control of social cognition, can stem from deficits in social representation instead. We advocate for a “default stance” in social cognition, where control is usually general, but representation is specific. This study underscores the importance of integrating social cognition within the broader realm of domain-general control processing, offering a unified perspective on task processing.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-02-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13415","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974002","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1