首页 > 最新文献

Cognitive Science最新文献

英文 中文
Predictability and Variation in Language Are Differentially Affected by Learning and Production 语言的可预测性和变异性受学习和制作的不同影响。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13435
Aislinn Keogh, Simon Kirby, Jennifer Culbertson

General principles of human cognition can help to explain why languages are more likely to have certain characteristics than others: structures that are difficult to process or produce will tend to be lost over time. One aspect of cognition that is implicated in language use is working memory—the component of short-term memory used for temporary storage and manipulation of information. In this study, we consider the relationship between working memory and regularization of linguistic variation. Regularization is a well-documented process whereby languages become less variable (on some dimension) over time. This process has been argued to be driven by the behavior of individual language users, but the specific mechanism is not agreed upon. Here, we use an artificial language learning experiment to investigate whether limitations in working memory during either language learning or language production drive regularization behavior. We find that taxing working memory during production results in the loss of all types of variation, but the process by which random variation becomes more predictable is better explained by learning biases. A computational model offers a potential explanation for the production effect using a simple self-priming mechanism.

人类认知的一般原则有助于解释为什么语言比其他语言更有可能具有某些特征:难以处理或生成的结构往往会随着时间的推移而丢失。与语言使用有关的认知的一个方面是工作记忆--短时记忆中用于临时存储和处理信息的部分。在本研究中,我们将考虑工作记忆与语言变异规则化之间的关系。正则化是一个有据可查的过程,随着时间的推移,语言(在某些维度上)的变异会越来越小。这一过程被认为是由语言使用者的个人行为所驱动的,但具体机制尚未达成一致。在此,我们利用人工语言学习实验来研究在语言学习或语言生产过程中工作记忆的限制是否会驱动正则化行为。我们发现,在语言生产过程中对工作记忆的限制会导致所有类型变异的丧失,但学习偏差能更好地解释随机变异变得更可预测的过程。一个计算模型利用一个简单的自激励机制为生产效应提供了一个潜在的解释。
{"title":"Predictability and Variation in Language Are Differentially Affected by Learning and Production","authors":"Aislinn Keogh,&nbsp;Simon Kirby,&nbsp;Jennifer Culbertson","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13435","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13435","url":null,"abstract":"<p>General principles of human cognition can help to explain why languages are more likely to have certain characteristics than others: structures that are difficult to process or produce will tend to be lost over time. One aspect of cognition that is implicated in language use is working memory—the component of short-term memory used for temporary storage and manipulation of information. In this study, we consider the relationship between working memory and regularization of linguistic variation. Regularization is a well-documented process whereby languages become less variable (on some dimension) over time. This process has been argued to be driven by the behavior of individual language users, but the specific mechanism is not agreed upon. Here, we use an artificial language learning experiment to investigate whether limitations in working memory during either language learning or language production drive regularization behavior. We find that taxing working memory during production results in the loss of all types of variation, but the process by which random variation becomes more predictable is better explained by learning biases. A computational model offers a potential explanation for the production effect using a simple self-priming mechanism.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13435","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140337306","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Unraveling Temporal Dynamics of Multidimensional Statistical Learning in Implicit and Explicit Systems: An X-Way Hypothesis 揭示隐式和显式系统中多维统计学习的时间动态:X-Way 假设
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-02 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13437
Stephen Man-Kit Lee, Nicole Sin Hang Law, Shelley Xiuli Tong

Statistical learning enables humans to involuntarily process and utilize different kinds of patterns from the environment. However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying the simultaneous acquisition of multiple regularities from different perceptual modalities remain unclear. A novel multidimensional serial reaction time task was developed to test 40 participants’ ability to learn simple first-order and complex second-order relations between uni-modal visual and cross-modal audio-visual stimuli. Using the difference in reaction times between sequenced and random stimuli as the index of domain-general statistical learning, a significant difference and dissociation of learning occurred between the initial and final learning phases. Furthermore, we used a negative and positive occurrence-frequency-and-reaction-time correlation to indicate implicit and explicit learning, respectively, and found that learning simple uni-modal patterns involved an implicit-to-explicit segue, while acquiring complex cross-modal patterns required an explicit-to-implicit segue, resulting in a X-shape crossing of regularity learning. Thus, we propose an X-way hypothesis to elucidate the dynamic interplay between the implicit and explicit systems at two distinct stages when acquiring various regularities in a multidimensional probability space.

统计学习使人类能够不由自主地处理和利用环境中的各种模式。然而,从不同感知模式中同时获得多种规律性的认知机制仍不清楚。研究人员开发了一种新颖的多维序列反应时间任务,以测试40名参与者学习单模态视觉刺激和跨模态视听刺激之间简单的一阶关系和复杂的二阶关系的能力。以顺序刺激和随机刺激之间反应时间的差异作为领域一般统计学习的指标,发现初始学习阶段和最终学习阶段之间存在显著差异和学习分离。此外,我们用出现频率和反应时间的负相关和正相关分别表示内隐学习和外显学习,发现学习简单的单模态模式需要内隐到外显的转换,而学习复杂的跨模态模式则需要外显到内隐的转换,从而形成了规律性学习的X型交叉。因此,我们提出了 "X-路 "假说,以阐明在多维概率空间中获得各种规则性时,内隐系统和外显系统在两个不同阶段的动态相互作用。
{"title":"Unraveling Temporal Dynamics of Multidimensional Statistical Learning in Implicit and Explicit Systems: An X-Way Hypothesis","authors":"Stephen Man-Kit Lee,&nbsp;Nicole Sin Hang Law,&nbsp;Shelley Xiuli Tong","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13437","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13437","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Statistical learning enables humans to involuntarily process and utilize different kinds of patterns from the environment. However, the cognitive mechanisms underlying the simultaneous acquisition of multiple regularities from different perceptual modalities remain unclear. A novel multidimensional serial reaction time task was developed to test 40 participants’ ability to learn simple first-order and complex second-order relations between uni-modal visual and cross-modal audio-visual stimuli. Using the difference in reaction times between sequenced and random stimuli as the index of domain-general statistical learning, a significant difference and dissociation of learning occurred between the initial and final learning phases. Furthermore, we used a negative and positive occurrence-frequency-and-reaction-time correlation to indicate implicit and explicit learning, respectively, and found that learning simple uni-modal patterns involved an implicit-to-explicit segue, while acquiring complex cross-modal patterns required an explicit-to-implicit segue, resulting in a X-shape crossing of regularity learning. Thus, we propose an X-way hypothesis to elucidate the dynamic interplay between the implicit and explicit systems at two distinct stages when acquiring various regularities in a multidimensional probability space.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-04-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13437","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140337308","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Correction to “Introducing meta-analysis in the evaluation of computational models of infant language development” 对 "在婴儿语言发展计算模型评估中引入元分析 "的更正。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13434

Cruz Blandón, M. A., Cristia, A., Räsänen, O. (2023). Introducing meta-analysis in the evaluation of computational models of infant language development. Cognitive Science, 47(7), e13307. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13307

On page 15, a citation to Bunce et al. (2021; pre-print) inaccurately attributes an estimate of 5.82 h of daily infant speech exposure to their study.

Bunce et al. (2021) did not directly report on infants’ daily speech exposure. Instead, our estimate of 5.82 h of speech per day was derived from their data as follows: we first calculated the average rates of target-child-directed speech (TCDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS) per hour across the five languages studied (Table 2 in Bunce et al., 2021). The sum of these average rates—3.72 min per hour for TCDS and 10.84 min per hour for ADS—was then multiplied by 24 h to estimate full-day exposure, yielding 5.82 h per day.

However, this estimate excludes speech directed at other children but heard by the target child, accounting for an additional 4.61 min per hour as reported in the supplementary material of Bunce et al. (2021). Additionally, the estimate assumes the long-form recordings analyzed are representative of a full 24-h day, likely overestimating language exposure by including nighttime, when infants and their caregivers are typically asleep. The long-form recordings analyzed by Bunce et al. (2021) and the actual language input to infants is likely biased toward the waking hours of adults and children in the language environments studied. The estimate of 2124 h of speech heard per year presented in our paper is thus on the upper end of the likely input scale but remains within plausible bounds. For context, Hart and Risley (1995) report 45 million words heard by the age of 4 in families of the professional class, equivalent to about 937.5 h of speech (assuming an average word duration of 0.3 s), but this estimate is only for child-directed speech (CDS). Bunce et al. (2021) found that infants exposed to North-American English hear twice as much ADS as CDS, and our simulations aimed to account for all speech a learner hears.

Cruz Blandón, M. A., Cristia, A., Räsänen, O. (2023).在婴儿语言发展计算模型评估中引入元分析。认知科学》,47(7),e13307。https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13307On 第 15 页,引用了 Bunce 等人(2021 年;预印本)的研究,不准确地将婴儿每天 5.82 小时的语言接触估计归因于他们的研究。Bunce 等人(2021 年)并没有直接报告婴儿每天的语言接触。Bunce 等人(2021 年)并没有直接报告婴儿每天的言语接触量,相反,我们对每天 5.82 小时言语接触量的估计是根据他们的数据得出的,具体如下:我们首先计算了所研究的五种语言每小时目标儿童引导言语 (TCDS) 和成人引导言语 (ADS) 的平均比率(Bunce 等人,2021 年,表 2)。这些平均比率的总和--TCDS 为每小时 3.72 分钟,ADS 为每小时 10.84 分钟--然后乘以 24 小时来估算全天的暴露时间,得出每天 5.82 小时。然而,这一估算不包括针对其他儿童但目标儿童听到的言语,如 Bunce 等人(2021 年)的补充材料中所报告的,每小时还需 4.61 分钟。此外,该估算假定所分析的长篇录音代表了一天 24 小时的全部时间,而将夜间包括在内可能会高估语言接触时间,因为夜间婴儿及其看护人通常都在睡觉。Bunce 等人(2021 年)分析的长式录音和婴儿实际的语言输入可能偏重于所研究语言环境中成人和儿童的清醒时间。因此,我们在论文中提出的每年听到 2124 小时语言的估计值处于可能输入规模的上限,但仍在合理范围之内。哈特和里斯利(Hart and Risley,1995 年)报告说,在专业阶层的家庭中,4 岁前听到的单词数为 4500 万,相当于约 937.5 小时的语音(假设平均单词持续时间为 0.3 秒),但这一估计值仅针对儿童引导的语音(CDS)。Bunce 等人(2021 年)发现,接触北美英语的婴儿听到的 ADS 是 CDS 的两倍。
{"title":"Correction to “Introducing meta-analysis in the evaluation of computational models of infant language development”","authors":"","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13434","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13434","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Cruz Blandón, M. A., Cristia, A., Räsänen, O. (2023). Introducing meta-analysis in the evaluation of computational models of infant language development. <i>Cognitive Science</i>, <i>47</i>(7), e13307. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13307</p><p>On page 15, a citation to Bunce et al. (2021; pre-print) inaccurately attributes an estimate of 5.82 h of daily infant speech exposure to their study.</p><p>Bunce et al. (2021) did not directly report on infants’ daily speech exposure. Instead, our estimate of 5.82 h of speech per day was derived from their data as follows: we first calculated the average rates of target-child-directed speech (TCDS) and adult-directed speech (ADS) per hour across the five languages studied (Table 2 in Bunce et al., 2021). The sum of these average rates—3.72 min per hour for TCDS and 10.84 min per hour for ADS—was then multiplied by 24 h to estimate full-day exposure, yielding 5.82 h per day.</p><p>However, this estimate excludes speech directed at other children but heard by the target child, accounting for an additional 4.61 min per hour as reported in the supplementary material of Bunce et al. (2021). Additionally, the estimate assumes the long-form recordings analyzed are representative of a full 24-h day, likely overestimating language exposure by including nighttime, when infants and their caregivers are typically asleep. The long-form recordings analyzed by Bunce et al. (2021) and the actual language input to infants is likely biased toward the waking hours of adults and children in the language environments studied. The estimate of 2124 h of speech heard per year presented in our paper is thus on the upper end of the likely input scale but remains within plausible bounds. For context, Hart and Risley (1995) report 45 million words heard by the age of 4 in families of the professional class, equivalent to about 937.5 h of speech (assuming an average word duration of 0.3 s), but this estimate is only for child-directed speech (CDS). Bunce et al. (2021) found that infants exposed to North-American English hear twice as much ADS as CDS, and our simulations aimed to account for all speech a learner hears.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13434","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140294932","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Looking at Mental Images: Eye-Tracking Mental Simulation During Retrospective Causal Judgment 观察心理图像:回溯因果判断时的眼动心理模拟
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-26 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13426
Kristina Krasich, Kevin O'Neill, Felipe De Brigard

How do people evaluate causal relationships? Do they just consider what actually happened, or do they also consider what could have counterfactually happened? Using eye tracking and Gaussian process modeling, we investigated how people mentally simulated past events to judge what caused the outcomes to occur. Participants played a virtual ball-shooting game and then—while looking at a blank screen—mentally simulated (a) what actually happened, (b) what counterfactually could have happened, or (c) what caused the outcome to happen. Our findings showed that participants moved their eyes in patterns consistent with the actual or counterfactual events that they mentally simulated. When simulating what caused the outcome to occur, participants moved their eyes consistent with simulations of counterfactual possibilities. These results favor counterfactual theories of causal reasoning, demonstrate how eye movements can reflect simulation during this reasoning and provide a novel approach for investigating retrospective causal reasoning and counterfactual thinking.

人们如何评价因果关系?他们是只考虑实际发生的情况,还是也考虑可能发生的反事实情况?通过眼动跟踪和高斯过程建模,我们研究了人们是如何在头脑中模拟过去的事件来判断结果发生的原因的。参与者玩了一个虚拟的射球游戏,然后在看着空白屏幕的同时,在头脑中模拟(a)实际发生的事情,(b)反事实可能发生的事情,或者(c)导致结果发生的原因。我们的研究结果表明,参与者的视线移动模式与他们在头脑中模拟的实际事件或反事实事件一致。在模拟导致结果发生的原因时,参与者的视线移动与反事实可能性的模拟一致。这些结果有利于因果推理的反事实理论,证明了眼球运动如何反映这种推理过程中的模拟,并为研究回溯性因果推理和反事实思维提供了一种新方法。
{"title":"Looking at Mental Images: Eye-Tracking Mental Simulation During Retrospective Causal Judgment","authors":"Kristina Krasich,&nbsp;Kevin O'Neill,&nbsp;Felipe De Brigard","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13426","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13426","url":null,"abstract":"<p>How do people evaluate causal relationships? Do they just consider what actually happened, or do they also consider what could have counterfactually happened? Using eye tracking and Gaussian process modeling, we investigated how people mentally simulated past events to judge what caused the outcomes to occur. Participants played a virtual ball-shooting game and then—while looking at a blank screen—mentally simulated (a) what actually happened, (b) what counterfactually could have happened, or (c) what caused the outcome to happen. Our findings showed that participants moved their eyes in patterns consistent with the actual or counterfactual events that they mentally simulated. When simulating what caused the outcome to occur, participants moved their eyes consistent with simulations of counterfactual possibilities. These results favor counterfactual theories of causal reasoning, demonstrate how eye movements can reflect simulation during this reasoning and provide a novel approach for investigating retrospective causal reasoning and counterfactual thinking.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13426","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289218","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Embodying Similarity and Difference: The Effect of Listing and Contrasting Gestures During U.S. Political Speech 体现相似与差异:美国政治演讲中列举和对比手势的效果。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13428
Icy (Yunyi) Zhang, Tina Izad, Erica A. Cartmill

Public speakers like politicians carefully craft their words to maximize the clarity, impact, and persuasiveness of their messages. However, these messages can be shaped by more than words. Gestures play an important role in how spoken arguments are perceived, conceptualized, and remembered by audiences. Studies of political speech have explored the ways spoken arguments are used to persuade audiences and cue applause. Studies of politicians’ gestures have explored the ways politicians illustrate different concepts with their hands, but have not focused on gesture's potential as a tool of persuasion. Our paper combines these traditions to ask first, how politicians gesture when using spoken rhetorical devices aimed at persuading audiences, and second, whether these gestures influence the ways their arguments are perceived. Study 1 examined two rhetorical devices—contrasts and lists—used by three politicians during U.S. presidential debates and asked whether the gestures produced during contrasts and lists differ. Gestures produced during contrasts were more likely to involve changes in hand location, and gestures produced during lists were more likely to involve changes in trajectory. Study 2 used footage from the same debates in an experiment to ask whether gesture influenced the way people perceived the politicians’ arguments. When participants had access to gestural information, they perceived contrasted items as more different from one another and listed items as more similar to one another than they did when they only had access to speech. This was true even when participants had access to only gesture (in muted videos). We conclude that gesture is effective at communicating concepts of similarity and difference and that politicians (and likely other speakers) take advantage of gesture's persuasive potential.

像政治家这样的公众演讲者会仔细斟酌他们的言辞,以最大限度地提高信息的清晰度、影响力和说服力。然而,塑造这些信息的不仅仅是语言。手势在受众如何感知、概念化和记忆口语论点方面发挥着重要作用。对政治演讲的研究探讨了口语论证如何用来说服听众和引起掌声。对政治家手势的研究探讨了政治家用手说明不同概念的方式,但并未关注手势作为说服工具的潜力。我们的论文结合了这些传统,首先探究政治家在使用旨在说服听众的口头修辞手段时是如何做出手势的,其次探究这些手势是否会影响人们对其论点的理解。研究 1 考察了三位政治家在美国总统辩论中使用的两种修辞手段--对比和列表,并询问对比和列表时的手势是否有所不同。对比时的手势更可能涉及手部位置的变化,而列表时的手势更可能涉及轨迹的变化。研究 2 在实验中使用了同样的辩论片段,以探究手势是否会影响人们对政治家论点的感知。当参与者可以获得手势信息时,他们会认为对比项目之间的差异更大,而列出的项目之间的相似度则更高。即使参与者只能看到手势(在静音视频中),情况也是如此。我们的结论是,手势能有效传达相似性和差异性的概念,政治家(也可能是其他演讲者)会利用手势的说服潜力。
{"title":"Embodying Similarity and Difference: The Effect of Listing and Contrasting Gestures During U.S. Political Speech","authors":"Icy (Yunyi) Zhang,&nbsp;Tina Izad,&nbsp;Erica A. Cartmill","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13428","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13428","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Public speakers like politicians carefully craft their words to maximize the clarity, impact, and persuasiveness of their messages. However, these messages can be shaped by more than words. Gestures play an important role in how spoken arguments are perceived, conceptualized, and remembered by audiences. Studies of political speech have explored the ways spoken arguments are used to persuade audiences and cue applause. Studies of politicians’ gestures have explored the ways politicians illustrate different concepts with their hands, but have not focused on gesture's potential as a tool of persuasion. Our paper combines these traditions to ask first, how politicians gesture when using spoken rhetorical devices aimed at persuading audiences, and second, whether these gestures influence the ways their arguments are perceived. Study 1 examined two rhetorical devices—<i>contrasts</i> and <i>lists</i>—used by three politicians during U.S. presidential debates and asked whether the gestures produced during contrasts and lists differ. Gestures produced during contrasts were more likely to involve changes in hand location, and gestures produced during lists were more likely to involve changes in trajectory. Study 2 used footage from the same debates in an experiment to ask whether gesture influenced the way people perceived the politicians’ arguments. When participants had access to gestural information, they perceived contrasted items as more different from one another and listed items as more similar to one another than they did when they only had access to speech. This was true even when participants had access to only gesture (in muted videos). We conclude that gesture is effective at communicating concepts of similarity and difference and that politicians (and likely other speakers) take advantage of gesture's persuasive potential.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13428","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289217","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Can Infants Retain Statistically Segmented Words and Mappings Across a Delay? 婴儿能否跨越延迟保留统计分段的单词和映射?
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13433
Ferhat Karaman, Jill Lany, Jessica F. Hay

Infants are sensitive to statistics in spoken language that aid word-form segmentation and immediate mapping to referents. However, it is not clear whether this sensitivity influences the formation and retention of word-referent mappings across a delay, two real-world challenges that learners must overcome. We tested how the timing of referent training, relative to familiarization with transitional probabilities (TPs) in speech, impacts English-learning 23-month-olds’ ability to form and retain word-referent mappings. In Experiment 1, we tested infants’ ability to retain TP information across a 10-min delay and use it in the service of word learning. Infants successfully mapped high-TP but not low-TP words to referents. In Experiment 2, infants readily mapped the same words even when they were unfamiliar. In Experiment 3, high- and low-TP word-referent mappings were trained immediately after familiarization, and infants readily remembered these associations 10 min later. In sum, although 23-month-old infants do not need strong statistics to map word forms to referents immediately, or to remember those mappings across a delay, infants are nevertheless sensitive to these statistics in the speech stream, and they influence mapping after a delay. These findings suggest that, by 23 months of age, sensitivity to statistics in speech may impact infants’ language development by leading word forms with low coherence to be poorly mapped following even a short period of consolidation.

婴儿对口语中有助于词形分割和立即映射到参照物的统计数据非常敏感。然而,这种敏感性是否会影响单词-参照物映射的形成和延迟保留,目前尚不清楚,而这正是学习者必须克服的两个现实挑战。我们测试了相对于熟悉语音中的过渡概率(TPs)而言,指代训练的时机如何影响 23 个月大的英语学习者形成和保持单词-指代映射的能力。在实验 1 中,我们测试了婴儿在延迟 10 分钟后保留 TP 信息并将其用于单词学习的能力。婴儿成功地将高TP词(而非低TP词)映射到了参照物上。在实验 2 中,即使是不熟悉的词,婴儿也能很容易地将其与相同的词对应起来。在实验 3 中,高词性和低词性单词与参照物的映射在熟悉后立即进行了训练,10 分钟后婴儿很容易记住这些联想。总之,虽然 23 个月大的婴儿不需要强大的统计量就能立即将词形映射到参照物上,也不需要在延迟后记住这些映射,但婴儿对语流中的这些统计量很敏感,它们会在延迟后影响映射。这些研究结果表明,到 23 个月大时,对语音中统计信息的敏感性可能会影响婴儿的语言发展,导致连贯性较低的词形在经过短时间的巩固后也不能很好地映射。
{"title":"Can Infants Retain Statistically Segmented Words and Mappings Across a Delay?","authors":"Ferhat Karaman,&nbsp;Jill Lany,&nbsp;Jessica F. Hay","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13433","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13433","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Infants are sensitive to statistics in spoken language that aid word-form segmentation and immediate mapping to referents. However, it is not clear whether this sensitivity influences the formation and retention of word-referent mappings across a delay, two real-world challenges that learners must overcome. We tested how the timing of referent training, relative to familiarization with transitional probabilities (TPs) in speech, impacts English-learning 23-month-olds’ ability to form and retain word-referent mappings. In Experiment 1, we tested infants’ ability to retain TP information across a 10-min delay and use it in the service of word learning. Infants successfully mapped high-TP but not low-TP words to referents. In Experiment 2, infants readily mapped the same words even when they were unfamiliar. In Experiment 3, high- and low-TP word-referent mappings were trained immediately after familiarization, and infants readily remembered these associations 10 min later. In sum, although 23-month-old infants do not need strong statistics to map word forms to referents immediately, or to remember those mappings across a delay, infants are nevertheless sensitive to these statistics in the speech stream, and they influence mapping after a delay. These findings suggest that, by 23 months of age, sensitivity to statistics in speech may impact infants’ language development by leading word forms with low coherence to be poorly mapped following even a short period of consolidation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289215","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Computational Modeling of the Segmentation of Sentence Stimuli From an Infant Word-Finding Study 从婴儿找词研究中建立句子刺激分割计算模型
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-25 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13427
Daniel Swingley, Robin Algayres

Computational models of infant word-finding typically operate over transcriptions of infant-directed speech corpora. It is now possible to test models of word segmentation on speech materials, rather than transcriptions of speech. We propose that such modeling efforts be conducted over the speech of the experimental stimuli used in studies measuring infants' capacity for learning from spoken sentences. Correspondence with infant outcomes in such experiments is an appropriate benchmark for models of infants. We demonstrate such an analysis by applying the DP-Parser model of Algayres and colleagues to auditory stimuli used in infant psycholinguistic experiments by Pelucchi and colleagues. The DP-Parser model takes speech as input, and creates multiple overlapping embeddings from each utterance. Prospective words are identified as clusters of similar embedded segments. This allows segmentation of each utterance into possible words, using a dynamic programming method that maximizes the frequency of constituent segments. We show that DP-Parse mimics American English learners' performance in extracting words from Italian sentences, favoring the segmentation of words with high syllabic transitional probability. This kind of computational analysis over actual stimuli from infant experiments may be helpful in tuning future models to match human performance.

婴儿找词的计算模型通常是在婴儿引导的语音库转录本上运行的。现在,我们有可能在语音材料而不是语音转录本上测试单词分段模型。我们建议在测量婴儿从口语句子中学习能力的研究中,对实验刺激的语音进行这种建模工作。与婴儿在此类实验中的结果相对应,是婴儿模型的适当基准。我们通过将 Algayres 及其同事的 DP-Parser 模型应用于 Pelucchi 及其同事的婴儿心理语言学实验中使用的听觉刺激,来演示这种分析。DP-Parser 模型将语音作为输入,并从每个语音中创建多个重叠嵌入。前瞻性词语被识别为相似嵌入片段的集群。这样就可以使用动态编程方法将每个语段分割成可能的词语,从而最大限度地提高组成语段的频率。我们的研究表明,DP-Parse 模拟了美式英语学习者从意大利语句子中提取单词的表现,有利于分割音节过渡概率高的单词。这种对婴儿实验中的实际刺激进行的计算分析可能有助于调整未来的模型,使之与人类的表现相匹配。
{"title":"Computational Modeling of the Segmentation of Sentence Stimuli From an Infant Word-Finding Study","authors":"Daniel Swingley,&nbsp;Robin Algayres","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13427","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13427","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Computational models of infant word-finding typically operate over transcriptions of infant-directed speech corpora. It is now possible to test models of word segmentation on speech materials, rather than transcriptions of speech. We propose that such modeling efforts be conducted over the speech of the experimental stimuli used in studies measuring infants' capacity for learning from spoken sentences. Correspondence with infant outcomes in such experiments is an appropriate benchmark for models of infants. We demonstrate such an analysis by applying the DP-Parser model of Algayres and colleagues to auditory stimuli used in infant psycholinguistic experiments by Pelucchi and colleagues. The DP-Parser model takes speech as input, and creates multiple overlapping embeddings from each utterance. Prospective words are identified as clusters of similar embedded segments. This allows segmentation of each utterance into possible words, using a dynamic programming method that maximizes the frequency of constituent segments. We show that DP-Parse mimics American English learners' performance in extracting words from Italian sentences, favoring the segmentation of words with high syllabic transitional probability. This kind of computational analysis over actual stimuli from infant experiments may be helpful in tuning future models to match human performance.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13427","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140289216","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Temporal Gestures in Different Temporal Perspectives 不同时空视角下的时态手势
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13425
Emir Akbuğa, Tilbe Göksun

Temporal perspectives allow us to place ourselves and temporal events on a timeline, making it easier to conceptualize time. This study investigates how we take different temporal perspectives in our temporal gestures. We asked participants (n = 36) to retell temporal scenarios written in the Moving-Ego, Moving-Time, and Time-Reference-Point perspectives in spontaneous and encouraged gesture conditions. Participants took temporal perspectives mostly in similar ways regardless of the gesture condition. Perspective comparisons showed that temporal gestures of our participants resonated better with the Ego- (i.e., Moving-Ego and Moving-Time) versus Time-Reference-Point distinction instead of the classical Moving-Ego versus Moving-Time contrast. Specifically, participants mostly produced more Moving-Ego and Time-Reference-Point gestures for the corresponding scenarios and speech; however, the Moving-Time perspective was not adopted more than the others in any condition. Similarly, the Moving-Time gestures did not favor an axis over the others, whereas Moving-Ego gestures were mostly sagittal and Time-Reference-Point gestures were mostly lateral. These findings suggest that we incorporate temporal perspectives into our temporal gestures to a considerable extent; however, the classical Moving-Ego and Moving-Time classification may not hold for temporal gestures.

时间视角使我们能够将自己和时间事件置于时间轴上,从而更容易地将时间概念化。本研究探讨了我们如何在时间手势中采用不同的时间视角。我们要求参与者(n = 36)在自发和鼓励的手势条件下复述以 "移动-自我"、"移动-时间 "和 "时间-参照点 "视角撰写的时间情景。无论手势条件如何,参与者采用的时间视角大多相似。视角比较显示,参与者的时间手势在 "自我"(即 "移动-自我 "和 "移动-时间")与 "时间-参照-点 "的区别上产生了更好的共鸣,而不是传统的 "移动-自我 "与 "移动-时间 "的对比。具体来说,在相应的情景和言语中,参与者大多会做出更多的 "移动-自我 "和 "时间-参照点 "手势;但是,在任何情况下,"移动-时间 "视角都没有被更多地采用。同样,"移动-时间 "手势并不比其他手势更偏向于某一轴线,而 "移动-自我 "手势大多是矢状的,"时间-参照点 "手势大多是横向的。这些研究结果表明,我们在相当程度上将时间视角融入了我们的时间手势中;然而,经典的 "移动-自我 "和 "移动-时间 "分类可能并不适用于时间手势。
{"title":"Temporal Gestures in Different Temporal Perspectives","authors":"Emir Akbuğa,&nbsp;Tilbe Göksun","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13425","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13425","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Temporal perspectives allow us to place ourselves and temporal events on a timeline, making it easier to conceptualize time. This study investigates how we take different temporal perspectives in our temporal gestures. We asked participants (<i>n</i> = 36) to retell temporal scenarios written in the Moving-Ego, Moving-Time, and Time-Reference-Point perspectives in spontaneous and encouraged gesture conditions. Participants took temporal perspectives mostly in similar ways regardless of the gesture condition. Perspective comparisons showed that temporal gestures of our participants resonated better with the Ego- (i.e., Moving-Ego and Moving-Time) versus Time-Reference-Point distinction instead of the classical Moving-Ego versus Moving-Time contrast. Specifically, participants mostly produced more Moving-Ego and Time-Reference-Point gestures for the corresponding scenarios and speech; however, the Moving-Time perspective was not adopted more than the others in any condition. Similarly, the Moving-Time gestures did not favor an axis over the others, whereas Moving-Ego gestures were mostly sagittal and Time-Reference-Point gestures were mostly lateral. These findings suggest that we incorporate temporal perspectives into our temporal gestures to a considerable extent; however, the classical Moving-Ego and Moving-Time classification may not hold for temporal gestures.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13425","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140159273","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Evaluating the Relative Importance of Wordhood Cues Using Statistical Learning 利用统计学习评估词性线索的相对重要性。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13429
Elizabeth Pankratz, Simon Kirby, Jennifer Culbertson

Identifying wordlike units in language is typically done by applying a battery of criteria, though how to weight these criteria with respect to one another is currently unknown. We address this question by investigating whether certain criteria are also used as cues for learning an artificial language—if they are, then perhaps they can be relied on more as trustworthy top-down diagnostics. The two criteria for grammatical wordhood that we consider are a unit's free mobility and its internal immutability. These criteria also map to two cognitive mechanisms that could underlie successful statistical learning: learners might orient themselves around the low transitional probabilities at unit boundaries, or they might seek chunks with high internal transitional probabilities. We find that each criterion has its own facilitatory effect, and learning is best where they both align. This supports the battery-of-criteria approach to diagnosing wordhood, and also suggests that the mechanism behind statistical learning may not be a question of either/or; perhaps the two mechanisms do not compete, but mutually reinforce one another.

识别语言中的词类单位通常是通过一系列标准来完成的,但如何权衡这些标准,目前还不得而知。为了解决这个问题,我们研究了某些标准是否也可用作学习人工语言的线索--如果是的话,那么这些标准作为自上而下的诊断标准也许更值得信赖。我们考虑的语法词性的两个标准是单位的自由流动性和内部不变性。这些标准也映射出两种认知机制,它们可能是成功的统计学习的基础:学习者可能会围绕单元边界的低过渡概率进行定位,或者他们可能会寻找具有高内部过渡概率的语块。我们发现,每种标准都有各自的促进作用,而学习效果最好的是两者都一致的地方。这支持了诊断词性的电池标准方法,同时也表明统计学习背后的机制可能不是一个非此即彼的问题;也许这两种机制并不相互竞争,而是相互促进。
{"title":"Evaluating the Relative Importance of Wordhood Cues Using Statistical Learning","authors":"Elizabeth Pankratz,&nbsp;Simon Kirby,&nbsp;Jennifer Culbertson","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13429","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13429","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Identifying wordlike units in language is typically done by applying a battery of criteria, though how to weight these criteria with respect to one another is currently unknown. We address this question by investigating whether certain criteria are also used as cues for learning an artificial language—if they are, then perhaps they can be relied on more as trustworthy top-down diagnostics. The two criteria for grammatical wordhood that we consider are a unit's free mobility and its internal immutability. These criteria also map to two cognitive mechanisms that could underlie successful statistical learning: learners might orient themselves around the low transitional probabilities at unit boundaries, or they might seek chunks with high internal transitional probabilities. We find that each criterion has its own facilitatory effect, and learning is best where they both align. This supports the battery-of-criteria approach to diagnosing wordhood, and also suggests that the mechanism behind statistical learning may not be a question of either/or; perhaps the two mechanisms do not compete, but mutually reinforce one another.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13429","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140144389","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recursive Numeral Systems Optimize the Trade-off Between Lexicon Size and Average Morphosyntactic Complexity 递归数字系统在词典规模和平均形态句法复杂性之间实现了最佳权衡。
IF 2.5 2区 心理学 Q2 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.1111/cogs.13424
Milica Denić, Jakub Szymanik

Human languages vary in terms of which meanings they lexicalize, but this variation is constrained. It has been argued that languages are under two competing pressures: the pressure to be simple (e.g., to have a small lexicon) and to allow for informative (i.e., precise) communication, and that which meanings get lexicalized may be explained by languages finding a good way to trade off between these two pressures. However, in certain semantic domains, languages can reach very high levels of informativeness even if they lexicalize very few meanings in that domain. This is due to productive morphosyntax and compositional semantics, which may allow for construction of meanings which are not lexicalized. Consider the semantic domain of natural numbers: many languages lexicalize few natural number meanings as monomorphemic expressions, but can precisely convey very many natural number meanings using morphosyntactically complex numerals. In such semantic domains, lexicon size is not in direct competition with informativeness. What explains which meanings are lexicalized in such semantic domains? We will propose that in such cases, languages need to solve a different kind of trade-off problem: the trade-off between the pressure to lexicalize as few meanings as possible (i.e, to minimize lexicon size) and the pressure to produce as morphosyntactically simple utterances as possible (i.e, to minimize average morphosyntactic complexity of utterances). To support this claim, we will present a case study of 128 natural languages' numeral systems, and show computationally that they achieve a near-optimal trade-off between lexicon size and average morphosyntactic complexity of numerals. This study in conjunction with previous work on communicative efficiency suggests that languages' lexicons are shaped by a trade-off between not two but three pressures: be simple, be informative, and minimize average morphosyntactic complexity of utterances.

人类语言在词汇化意义方面各不相同,但这种变化是受限制的。有人认为,语言面临着两种相互竞争的压力:一是简单的压力(例如,词汇量小),二是信息交流的压力(即,精确)。然而,在某些语义领域,语言的信息量可以达到很高的水平,即使在该领域词汇化的意义很少。这要归功于生产性形态句法和组合语义学,它们可以构造出没有被词汇化的意义。考虑一下自然数的语义域:许多语言将极少数自然数的意义词法化为单体表达式,但却能用形态句法复杂的数字精确地表达极多的自然数意义。在这种语义领域,词库规模与信息量并不直接竞争。在这种语义域中,哪些意义被词法化了呢?我们将提出,在这种情况下,语言需要解决一个不同类型的权衡问题:在将尽可能少的意义词汇化的压力(即尽可能缩小词库规模)和产生尽可能形态句法简单的语篇的压力(即尽可能降低语篇的平均形态句法复杂性)之间进行权衡。为了支持这一观点,我们将对 128 种自然语言的数字系统进行案例研究,并通过计算证明它们在词库大小和数字的平均形态句法复杂性之间实现了近乎最佳的权衡。这项研究与之前关于交际效率的研究相结合,表明语言的词典是在三种压力的权衡下形成的:简单、信息量大、语篇的平均形态句法复杂度最小。
{"title":"Recursive Numeral Systems Optimize the Trade-off Between Lexicon Size and Average Morphosyntactic Complexity","authors":"Milica Denić,&nbsp;Jakub Szymanik","doi":"10.1111/cogs.13424","DOIUrl":"10.1111/cogs.13424","url":null,"abstract":"<p>Human languages vary in terms of which meanings they lexicalize, but this variation is constrained. It has been argued that languages are under two competing pressures: the pressure to be simple (e.g., to have a small lexicon) and to allow for informative (i.e., precise) communication, and that which meanings get lexicalized may be explained by languages finding a good way to trade off between these two pressures. However, in certain semantic domains, languages can reach very high levels of informativeness even if they lexicalize very few meanings in that domain. This is due to productive morphosyntax and compositional semantics, which may allow for construction of meanings which are not lexicalized. Consider the semantic domain of natural numbers: many languages lexicalize few natural number meanings as monomorphemic expressions, but can precisely convey very many natural number meanings using morphosyntactically complex numerals. In such semantic domains, lexicon size is not in direct competition with informativeness. What explains which meanings are lexicalized in such semantic domains? We will propose that in such cases, languages need to solve a different kind of trade-off problem: the trade-off between the pressure to lexicalize as few meanings as possible (i.e, to minimize lexicon size) and the pressure to produce as morphosyntactically simple utterances as possible (i.e, to minimize average morphosyntactic complexity of utterances). To support this claim, we will present a case study of 128 natural languages' numeral systems, and show computationally that they achieve a near-optimal trade-off between lexicon size and average morphosyntactic complexity of numerals. This study in conjunction with previous work on communicative efficiency suggests that languages' lexicons are shaped by a trade-off between not two but <i>three</i> pressures: be simple, be informative, and minimize average morphosyntactic complexity of utterances.</p>","PeriodicalId":48349,"journal":{"name":"Cognitive Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.5,"publicationDate":"2024-03-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/cogs.13424","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140144390","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Cognitive Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1