首页 > 最新文献

Journal of Family Psychology最新文献

英文 中文
Who's doing more and when? Gender, parenting, and housework trajectories. 谁在何时做得更多?性别、养育子女和家务轨迹。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001219
Matthew D Johnson, Michelle Maroto, Nancy L Galambos, Harvey J Krahn

Drawing on five waves of longitudinal survey data (N = 520, 51% female, 39% with a university degree, 90% White), this study examined trajectories of women's and men's contributions to cooking, kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, house cleaning, laundry, and overall housework from Age 25 to 50 years and explored time-invariant (traditional gender role attitudes, homemaker mother, mother and father education assessed at Age 18) and time-varying (raising children at Ages 25, 32, 43, and 50 years) predictors of housework trajectories. Growth curve analyses revealed that women contributed more to all housework tasks than men at Age 25, a gender gap maintained to Age 50. Housework increased to Age 32 and stabilized until Age 43 before declining by Age 50 for women's and men's laundry, women's kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, and overall housework, and men's house cleaning. There was no change in women's and men's trajectory of cooking meals, women's house cleaning, and men's contributions to kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, and overall housework. Traditional gender role attitudes, having a homemaker mother, and mother's and father's education inconsistently predicted women's and men's trajectories. Raising children, however, was consistently linked with within-person fluctuations in housework. When raising children, women contributed more than average to housework, whereas when men were raising children, they contributed less than normal. The results highlight a gendered pattern of housework evident in the twenties and persisting well into midlife, with parenthood widening the gap. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究利用五波纵向调查数据(N = 520,51% 为女性,39% 拥有大学学位,90% 为白人),研究了女性和男性在 25 岁至 50 岁期间在烹饪、厨房清洁、杂货采购、房屋清洁、洗衣和总体家务劳动方面的贡献轨迹,并探讨了家务劳动轨迹的时间不变(传统性别角色态度、家庭主妇母亲、18 岁时评估的母亲和父亲教育程度)和时间变化(25、32、43 和 50 岁时养育子女)预测因素。增长曲线分析表明,女性在 25 岁时比男性在所有家务劳动中贡献更多,这种性别差距一直保持到 50 岁。女性和男性的家务劳动增加到 32 岁,并在 43 岁之前保持稳定,到 50 岁时,女性和男性的洗衣、女性厨房清洁、买菜、整体家务劳动以及男性的家务劳动均有所减少。女性和男性在做饭、女性打扫房间以及男性在厨房清洁、买菜和整体家务方面的贡献轨迹没有变化。传统的性别角色观念、母亲是家庭主妇以及母亲和父亲的教育程度对女性和男性的轨迹预测不一致。然而,养育子女始终与家务劳动的个人波动有关。养育子女时,女性对家务劳动的贡献高于平均水平,而养育子女时,男性对家务劳动的贡献低于正常水平。研究结果凸显了一种性别家务模式,这种模式在 20 多岁时就很明显,一直持续到中年,而为人父母则会扩大这种差距。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Who's doing more and when? Gender, parenting, and housework trajectories.","authors":"Matthew D Johnson, Michelle Maroto, Nancy L Galambos, Harvey J Krahn","doi":"10.1037/fam0001219","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001219","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Drawing on five waves of longitudinal survey data (<i>N</i> = 520, 51% female, 39% with a university degree, 90% White), this study examined trajectories of women's and men's contributions to cooking, kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, house cleaning, laundry, and overall housework from Age 25 to 50 years and explored time-invariant (traditional gender role attitudes, homemaker mother, mother and father education assessed at Age 18) and time-varying (raising children at Ages 25, 32, 43, and 50 years) predictors of housework trajectories. Growth curve analyses revealed that women contributed more to all housework tasks than men at Age 25, a gender gap maintained to Age 50. Housework increased to Age 32 and stabilized until Age 43 before declining by Age 50 for women's and men's laundry, women's kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, and overall housework, and men's house cleaning. There was no change in women's and men's trajectory of cooking meals, women's house cleaning, and men's contributions to kitchen cleaning, grocery shopping, and overall housework. Traditional gender role attitudes, having a homemaker mother, and mother's and father's education inconsistently predicted women's and men's trajectories. Raising children, however, was consistently linked with within-person fluctuations in housework. When raising children, women contributed more than average to housework, whereas when men were raising children, they contributed less than normal. The results highlight a gendered pattern of housework evident in the twenties and persisting well into midlife, with parenthood widening the gap. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177199","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
All in the family: The complementary protective roles of spousal and other family support for Chinese immigrant mothers' life satisfaction over time. 家庭的一切:配偶和其他家庭支持对中国移民母亲生活满意度的长期互补保护作用。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-14 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001212
Yao Sun, Charissa S L Cheah, You Jung Seo, Ana Katrina Aquino, Hatice Gürsoy, Li-Wen Wu

The demand-resources model of stress posits that parenting tasks and expectations of mothers that exceed their resources are likely to tax their psychological well-being. Social and instrumental support from spouse or family may help alleviate the negative effects of parenting stress on mothers' psychological well-being. However, parenting stress and its impact have been less studied among immigrant mothers. Moreover, the sources of family support (i.e., spousal and other family members) might interactively affect mothers' well-being in the face of parenting stress. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine whether support from the spouse and other family members jointly buffers against the long-term psychological effects of parenting stress on Chinese immigrant mothers' life satisfaction. Data were collected from 273 Chinese American mothers at two time points separated by 6 months. A three-way interaction was conducted to examine the complementary protective effect of perceived support mothers received from their spouses and their other family members combined. Results showed that after controlling for the covariates, parenting stress was only longitudinally associated with changes in maternal life satisfaction when support from both spouse and other family members was low. Our findings highlight the complementary protective effects of spousal support and nonspouse family members' support in alleviating parenting stress of mothers and its adverse impact on lowering mothers' life satisfaction 6 months later. Theoretical and applied implications of these findings are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

压力的需求--资源模型认为,如果母亲的育儿任务和期望超出了她们的资源,就有可能给她们的心理健康造成负担。来自配偶或家庭的社会和工具性支持可能有助于减轻育儿压力对母亲心理健康的负面影响。然而,对移民母亲的养育压力及其影响的研究较少。此外,面对育儿压力,家庭支持的来源(即配偶和其他家庭成员)可能会交互影响母亲的幸福感。因此,本研究旨在探讨配偶和其他家庭成员的支持是否共同缓冲了养育压力对华裔移民母亲生活满意度的长期心理影响。本研究收集了 273 位华裔母亲在两个时间点(相隔 6 个月)的数据。通过三方交互作用,研究了母亲从配偶和其他家庭成员那里获得的感知支持的互补保护作用。结果表明,在控制了协变量后,只有当配偶和其他家庭成员的支持都很低时,养育压力才与母亲生活满意度的变化纵向相关。我们的研究结果凸显了配偶支持和非配偶家庭成员支持在减轻母亲养育压力方面的互补保护作用,及其对降低母亲 6 个月后生活满意度的不利影响。本文讨论了这些发现的理论和应用意义。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"All in the family: The complementary protective roles of spousal and other family support for Chinese immigrant mothers' life satisfaction over time.","authors":"Yao Sun, Charissa S L Cheah, You Jung Seo, Ana Katrina Aquino, Hatice Gürsoy, Li-Wen Wu","doi":"10.1037/fam0001212","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001212","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The demand-resources model of stress posits that parenting tasks and expectations of mothers that exceed their resources are likely to tax their psychological well-being. Social and instrumental support from spouse or family may help alleviate the negative effects of parenting stress on mothers' psychological well-being. However, parenting stress and its impact have been less studied among immigrant mothers. Moreover, the sources of family support (i.e., spousal and other family members) might interactively affect mothers' well-being in the face of parenting stress. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine whether support from the spouse and other family members jointly buffers against the long-term psychological effects of parenting stress on Chinese immigrant mothers' life satisfaction. Data were collected from 273 Chinese American mothers at two time points separated by 6 months. A three-way interaction was conducted to examine the complementary protective effect of perceived support mothers received from their spouses and their other family members combined. Results showed that after controlling for the covariates, parenting stress was only longitudinally associated with changes in maternal life satisfaction when support from both spouse and other family members was low. Our findings highlight the complementary protective effects of spousal support and nonspouse family members' support in alleviating parenting stress of mothers and its adverse impact on lowering mothers' life satisfaction 6 months later. Theoretical and applied implications of these findings are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140132909","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Genetic and subjective sensitivity, relationship dynamics, and psychological distress in couples. 夫妻的遗传和主观敏感性、关系动态和心理困扰。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-22 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001229
Samantha M Brown, Galena K Rhoades, Michael Pluess, Elizabeth S Allen, Scott M Stanley

Positive and negative aspects of intimate relationships influence mental health and well-being in couples. According to the environmental sensitivity framework, individuals differ in how strongly they are affected by their environment, with some individuals being more or less sensitive to both negative and positive experiences. The present study examined the longitudinal associations between positive and negative relationship dynamics, including marital satisfaction, positive bonding, and negative communication, and psychological distress as well as the extent to which individual differences in genetic and subjective measures of environmental sensitivity moderated the association between relationship dynamics and psychological distress in a sample of couples in the U.S. Army (n = 238 individuals representing 152 unique couples). Sensitivity was measured by self-report and a polygenic score derived from previous genome-wide association study results. Separate three-level multilevel models were conducted for each relationship dynamic and sensitivity variable. Only for subjective (i.e., self-reported) sensitivity did significant cross-level interactions emerge in predicting psychological distress, whereas no such interactions were found for genetic (i.e., polygenic score) sensitivity. Specifically, lower marital satisfaction and positive bonding were associated with higher psychological distress among subjectively highly sensitive individuals, and higher negative communication was associated with higher psychological distress among subjectively highly sensitive individuals. Findings suggest that both low positive and high negative relationship dynamics may have a greater effect on psychological distress among highly sensitive individuals, which may help to inform tailored intervention to meet the unique needs of couples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

亲密关系的积极和消极方面都会影响夫妻的心理健康和幸福。根据环境敏感性框架,个体受环境影响的程度各不相同,有些个体对负面和正面经历的敏感度有高有低。本研究以美国陆军中的夫妻(n = 238 人,代表 152 对独特的夫妻)为样本,研究了积极和消极关系动态(包括婚姻满意度、积极结合和消极沟通)与心理困扰之间的纵向关联,以及遗传和主观环境敏感度测量的个体差异在多大程度上调节了关系动态与心理困扰之间的关联。敏感性是通过自我报告和根据以前的全基因组关联研究结果得出的多基因评分来测量的。针对每种关系动态和敏感性变量分别建立了三级多层次模型。只有主观(即自我报告)敏感性在预测心理困扰方面出现了显著的跨层次交互作用,而遗传(即多基因评分)敏感性则没有发现这种交互作用。具体而言,在主观高度敏感的个体中,较低的婚姻满意度和积极结合与较高的心理压力相关,而在主观高度敏感的个体中,较高的消极沟通与较高的心理压力相关。研究结果表明,低积极和高消极的关系动态可能对高度敏感人群的心理困扰有更大的影响,这可能有助于提供有针对性的干预措施,以满足夫妻的独特需求。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Genetic and subjective sensitivity, relationship dynamics, and psychological distress in couples.","authors":"Samantha M Brown, Galena K Rhoades, Michael Pluess, Elizabeth S Allen, Scott M Stanley","doi":"10.1037/fam0001229","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001229","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Positive and negative aspects of intimate relationships influence mental health and well-being in couples. According to the environmental sensitivity framework, individuals differ in how strongly they are affected by their environment, with some individuals being more or less sensitive to both negative and positive experiences. The present study examined the longitudinal associations between positive and negative relationship dynamics, including marital satisfaction, positive bonding, and negative communication, and psychological distress as well as the extent to which individual differences in genetic and subjective measures of environmental sensitivity moderated the association between relationship dynamics and psychological distress in a sample of couples in the U.S. Army (<i>n</i> = 238 individuals representing 152 unique couples). Sensitivity was measured by self-report and a polygenic score derived from previous genome-wide association study results. Separate three-level multilevel models were conducted for each relationship dynamic and sensitivity variable. Only for subjective (i.e., self-reported) sensitivity did significant cross-level interactions emerge in predicting psychological distress, whereas no such interactions were found for genetic (i.e., polygenic score) sensitivity. Specifically, lower marital satisfaction and positive bonding were associated with higher psychological distress among subjectively highly sensitive individuals, and higher negative communication was associated with higher psychological distress among subjectively highly sensitive individuals. Findings suggest that both low positive and high negative relationship dynamics may have a greater effect on psychological distress among highly sensitive individuals, which may help to inform tailored intervention to meet the unique needs of couples. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140868353","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Screening parent-adolescent relationships, screen behaviors, tridimensional acculturation, and health among Black immigrant and refugee adolescents during dual pandemics. 筛查父母与青少年的关系、筛查行为、三维文化适应以及双重流行病期间黑人移民和难民青少年的健康状况。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001230
Lauren Eales, Jasmine Banegas, Fernanda Da Silva Cherubini, Salma A Ibrahim, Regina Jihea Ahn, Michelle R Nelson, Roli Dwivedi, Gail M Ferguson

This brief report assesses parent-adolescent relationships, screen behaviors, and tridimensional acculturation as risk and promotive or protective factors for health among Black U.S. immigrant or refugee adolescents during the dual COVID-19 and racism or Whiteness pandemics. Eighty-nine immigrant- or refugee-origin adolescents completed online surveys (72% Somali American, 28% Jamaican American; 45% female; 15% foreign-born; M = 14.11 years). Regression analyses revealed that parental autonomy support, parental restrictive media mediation, and adolescent heritage culture identification were promotive of better screen media use behaviors. Only adolescent media literacy self-efficacy was related to higher screen time. Importantly, screen self-regulation was a better predictor of general health than screen time. Results highlight many parenting strengths in Black immigrant or refugee families and underscore the resilience-promoting power of parent-adolescent relationships. Health implications are discussed to provide guidance for future prevention efforts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本简要报告评估了在 COVID-19 和种族主义或白化双重流行期间,父母与青少年的关系、屏幕行为和三维文化适应性作为美国黑人移民或难民青少年健康的风险和促进或保护因素。89 名移民或难民出身的青少年完成了在线调查(72% 为索马里裔美国人,28% 为牙买加裔美国人;45% 为女性;15% 在国外出生;M = 14.11 岁)。回归分析表明,父母的自主支持、父母对媒体的限制性调解以及青少年对传统文化的认同都有助于改善屏幕媒体的使用行为。只有青少年媒体素养自我效能与较高的屏幕使用时间有关。重要的是,屏幕自我调节比屏幕时间更能预测总体健康状况。研究结果凸显了黑人移民或难民家庭在养育子女方面的许多优势,并强调了父母与青少年关系中促进复原力的力量。研究还讨论了对健康的影响,为今后的预防工作提供指导。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Screening parent-adolescent relationships, screen behaviors, tridimensional acculturation, and health among Black immigrant and refugee adolescents during dual pandemics.","authors":"Lauren Eales, Jasmine Banegas, Fernanda Da Silva Cherubini, Salma A Ibrahim, Regina Jihea Ahn, Michelle R Nelson, Roli Dwivedi, Gail M Ferguson","doi":"10.1037/fam0001230","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001230","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This brief report assesses parent-adolescent relationships, screen behaviors, and tridimensional acculturation as risk and promotive or protective factors for health among Black U.S. immigrant or refugee adolescents during the dual COVID-19 and racism or Whiteness pandemics. Eighty-nine immigrant- or refugee-origin adolescents completed online surveys (72% Somali American, 28% Jamaican American; 45% female; 15% foreign-born; <i>M</i> = 14.11 years). Regression analyses revealed that parental autonomy support, parental restrictive media mediation, and adolescent heritage culture identification were promotive of better screen media use behaviors. Only adolescent media literacy self-efficacy was related to higher screen time. Importantly, screen self-regulation was a better predictor of general health than screen time. Results highlight many parenting strengths in Black immigrant or refugee families and underscore the resilience-promoting power of parent-adolescent relationships. Health implications are discussed to provide guidance for future prevention efforts. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140869985","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The perceived quality of maternal care during childhood shapes attentional bias to infant faces in parents and nonparents. 父母和非父母对儿童时期母亲照顾质量的感知会影响他们对婴儿面部的注意偏差。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-21 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001198
Micol Gemignani, Michele Giannotti, Simona de Falco

While research has shown that the attentional bias to infant faces predicts the quality of infant care, the antecedents of this cognitive process have been less established. In particular, it remains unknown whether the attentional bias to infant faces might be affected by the interplay between different factors, including memories of past experiences of care, adults' sex, and the experience of parenthood. To extend previous results, we examined the attentional bias to infant faces in a mixed sample of parents (n = 99) and nonparents (n = 102), and whether it varied in relation to parental status, sex, the quality of past experiences of care, and the interactions between these factors. A modified go/no-go task was used to compare the effect of adult and infant faces in retaining adults' attention. Memories of past experiences of paternal and maternal care were collected using the short form of the Italian version of the Parental Acceptance-Rejection scale. Results confirmed that infant faces induced greater attentional interference compared to adult faces. A heightened attention to all types of faces was found in parents versus nonparent. Women, as compared to men, were slower in the task performance, and allocated more attention to infant versus adult faces. Consistent with previous evidence, the attentional prioritization of infant faces varied in relation to previous experiences of care with one's own mother; individuals who remembered a more accepting maternal care allocated more attention to infant versus adult faces. Parental status did not modulate this effect, but sex of participants did. Grounded in the interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory (IPARTheory), this study provides new insights for discerning processes that might regulate global adult caregiving. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

虽然研究表明,对婴儿面孔的注意偏向可预测婴儿保育的质量,但这一认知过程的前因却不太确定。特别是,对婴儿面孔的注意偏向是否会受到不同因素(包括对过去照顾经历的记忆、成人的性别和为人父母的经历)之间相互作用的影响,目前仍是未知数。为了扩展之前的研究结果,我们研究了父母(99 人)和非父母(102 人)混合样本中对婴儿面孔的注意偏差,以及这种偏差是否随父母身份、性别、过去照顾经历的质量以及这些因素之间的相互作用而变化。为了比较成人面孔和婴儿面孔在吸引成人注意力方面的效果,我们使用了一个改进的 "去/不去 "任务。此外,还使用意大利语版父母接受-拒绝量表的简表收集了对过去父爱和母爱经历的记忆。结果证实,与成人面孔相比,婴儿面孔引起的注意干扰更大。与非父母亲相比,父母亲对所有类型的面孔都更加关注。与男性相比,女性在完成任务时速度较慢,而且分配给婴儿和成人面孔的注意力更多。与之前的证据相一致的是,婴儿面孔的注意力优先顺序与之前与自己的母亲一起接受照顾的经历有关;记忆中接受母亲照顾较多的个体会将更多注意力分配到婴儿面孔和成人面孔上。父母的身份并不影响这种效应,但参与者的性别却有影响。这项研究以人际父母接受-拒绝理论(IPARTheory)为基础,为辨别可能调节全球成人照料的过程提供了新的见解。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"The perceived quality of maternal care during childhood shapes attentional bias to infant faces in parents and nonparents.","authors":"Micol Gemignani, Michele Giannotti, Simona de Falco","doi":"10.1037/fam0001198","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001198","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>While research has shown that the attentional bias to infant faces predicts the quality of infant care, the antecedents of this cognitive process have been less established. In particular, it remains unknown whether the attentional bias to infant faces might be affected by the interplay between different factors, including memories of past experiences of care, adults' sex, and the experience of parenthood. To extend previous results, we examined the attentional bias to infant faces in a mixed sample of parents (<i>n</i> = 99) and nonparents (<i>n</i> = 102), and whether it varied in relation to parental status, sex, the quality of past experiences of care, and the interactions between these factors. A modified go/no-go task was used to compare the effect of adult and infant faces in retaining adults' attention. Memories of past experiences of paternal and maternal care were collected using the short form of the Italian version of the Parental Acceptance-Rejection scale. Results confirmed that infant faces induced greater attentional interference compared to adult faces. A heightened attention to all types of faces was found in parents versus nonparent. Women, as compared to men, were slower in the task performance, and allocated more attention to infant versus adult faces. Consistent with previous evidence, the attentional prioritization of infant faces varied in relation to previous experiences of care with one's own mother; individuals who remembered a more accepting maternal care allocated more attention to infant versus adult faces. Parental status did not modulate this effect, but sex of participants did. Grounded in the interpersonal acceptance-rejection theory (IPARTheory), this study provides new insights for discerning processes that might regulate global adult caregiving. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177198","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neighborhood selection by parent personality, depression, and coparent support: A two-study replication. 父母性格、抑郁和父母支持的邻里选择:一项两项研究的复制。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-07 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001182
Chia-Feng Chen, Jennifer Weaver, Thomas Schofield

This study addresses the degree to which three selection factors (parent personality, depression, and coparent support) drive movement into neighborhoods and predict changes in youth externalizing behavior. Two studies followed children from birth to age 15 (N = 1,364 for Study 1; N = 4,898 for Study 2). Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage, family income, and youth externalizing behavior were consistently and significantly correlated cross sectionally and longitudinally. Selection factors predicted changes in youth externalizing behavior. Higher family income, mother agreeableness, and perceived support from a coparent predicted movement over time into less disadvantaged neighborhoods. Lower levels of mother impulsivity, neuroticism, and depression also predicted movement over time into less disadvantaged neighborhoods. Neighborhood disadvantage did not predict change in youth externalizing behavior when any of the above selection factors were included in the model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究探讨了三个选择因素(父母个性、抑郁和父母支持)在多大程度上推动了青少年向社区的迁移,并预测了青少年外化行为的变化。两项研究跟踪儿童从出生到15岁(研究1 N = 1364;研究2 N = 4,898)。社区社会经济劣势、家庭收入和青少年外化行为在横断面和纵向上一致且显著相关。选择因素预测青少年外化行为的变化。随着时间的推移,较高的家庭收入、母亲的亲和度和来自父母的支持预示着他们会迁移到不那么贫困的社区。较低的母亲冲动、神经质和抑郁水平也预示着随着时间的推移,孩子会搬到不那么贫困的社区。当模型中包含上述任何一个选择因素时,邻里劣势都不能预测青少年外化行为的变化。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Neighborhood selection by parent personality, depression, and coparent support: A two-study replication.","authors":"Chia-Feng Chen, Jennifer Weaver, Thomas Schofield","doi":"10.1037/fam0001182","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001182","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study addresses the degree to which three selection factors (parent personality, depression, and coparent support) drive movement into neighborhoods and predict changes in youth externalizing behavior. Two studies followed children from birth to age 15 (<i>N</i> = 1,364 for Study 1; <i>N</i> = 4,898 for Study 2). Neighborhood socioeconomic disadvantage, family income, and youth externalizing behavior were consistently and significantly correlated cross sectionally and longitudinally. Selection factors predicted changes in youth externalizing behavior. Higher family income, mother agreeableness, and perceived support from a coparent predicted movement over time into less disadvantaged neighborhoods. Lower levels of mother impulsivity, neuroticism, and depression also predicted movement over time into less disadvantaged neighborhoods. Neighborhood disadvantage did not predict change in youth externalizing behavior when any of the above selection factors were included in the model. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11156791/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138499833","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Circular causality in daily coparenting processes among first-time parents. 首次为人父母者日常共同养育子女过程中的循环因果关系。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001227
Emmi Lindroos, Eija Räikkönen, Kaisa Malinen, Anna K Rönkä

Although coparenting has been widely studied, little is known about the daily processes of coparenting between mothers and fathers in early parenthood. Based on family systems theory and the ecological model of coparenting, we investigated new parents' day-to-day within-family processes of cooperative and tensioned coparenting. Mothers and fathers from 144 Finnish first-time couples completed daily mobile diaries for 7 consecutive days when their firstborn was 4-6 months old. The random-intercept cross-lagged panel model showed three types of within-family processes in daily coparenting, which we named continuity, spread, and shift. Continuity in cooperative coparenting occurred when a parent's previous-day cooperative coparenting positively predicted his or her own cooperative coparenting experiences the next day. We also found that coparenting experiences spread from one spouse to another: A parent's cooperative coparenting on the previous day negatively predicted his or her spouse's experiences of tensioned coparenting the following day. Finally, daily coparenting experiences also shifted from day to day: One parent's experience of tensioned coparenting the previous day positively predicted that parent's cooperative coparenting experiences the next day. No gender differences were found. These findings emphasize that the two daily coparenting dimensions seem to operate partly differently in daily life, as cooperative coparenting was slightly more often a cause and consequence in the observed processes than tensioned coparenting. Therefore, it seems that interventions should focus on enhancing cooperative coparenting. Moreover, the new concepts of continuity, spread, and shift are proposed as better descriptions of the three daily processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

虽然人们对共同养育子女进行了广泛的研究,但对父母在初为父母时的日常共同养育子女过程却知之甚少。基于家庭系统理论和共同养育的生态模式,我们调查了新手父母在家庭内部合作性和紧张性共同养育的日常过程。来自 144 对芬兰初为人父人母的夫妇在他们的第一个孩子 4-6 个月大时,连续 7 天填写了每日移动日记。随机截距交叉滞后面板模型显示了日常共同养育中的三种家庭内部过程,我们将其命名为连续性、扩散性和转移性。当父母前一天的合作共育经历对第二天自己的合作共育经历有正向预测作用时,合作共育就会出现连续性。我们还发现,共同养育经验会从配偶一方扩散到另一方:父母前一天的合作型共同养育经历会对其配偶第二天的紧张型共同养育经历产生负面影响。最后,日常的共同养育经验也会每天发生变化:父母一方前一天的紧张型共同养育经历会对其配偶第二天的合作型共同养育经历产生积极的预测作用。没有发现性别差异。这些发现强调,在日常生活中,这两种日常的亲子关系似乎有部分不同的运作方式,因为在观察到的过程中,合作型亲子关系比紧张型亲子关系更经常地成为原因和结果。因此,干预措施似乎应侧重于加强合作型共同养育。此外,我们还提出了 "连续性"、"扩散 "和 "转移 "这三个新概念,以更好地描述这三个日常生活过程。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Circular causality in daily coparenting processes among first-time parents.","authors":"Emmi Lindroos, Eija Räikkönen, Kaisa Malinen, Anna K Rönkä","doi":"10.1037/fam0001227","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001227","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although coparenting has been widely studied, little is known about the daily processes of coparenting between mothers and fathers in early parenthood. Based on family systems theory and the ecological model of coparenting, we investigated new parents' day-to-day within-family processes of cooperative and tensioned coparenting. Mothers and fathers from 144 Finnish first-time couples completed daily mobile diaries for 7 consecutive days when their firstborn was 4-6 months old. The random-intercept cross-lagged panel model showed three types of within-family processes in daily coparenting, which we named continuity, spread, and shift. <i>Continuity</i> in cooperative coparenting occurred when a parent's previous-day cooperative coparenting positively predicted his or her own cooperative coparenting experiences the next day. We also found that coparenting experiences <i>spread</i> from one spouse to another: A parent's cooperative coparenting on the previous day negatively predicted his or her spouse's experiences of tensioned coparenting the following day. Finally, daily coparenting experiences also <i>shifted</i> from day to day: One parent's experience of tensioned coparenting the previous day positively predicted that parent's cooperative coparenting experiences the next day. No gender differences were found. These findings emphasize that the two daily coparenting dimensions seem to operate partly differently in daily life, as cooperative coparenting was slightly more often a cause and consequence in the observed processes than tensioned coparenting. Therefore, it seems that interventions should focus on enhancing cooperative coparenting. Moreover, the new concepts of continuity, spread, and shift are proposed as better descriptions of the three daily processes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140943783","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Measurement invariance of the Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS) across 10 countries. 10 个国家的亲子关系量表(CRS)测量不变性。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-06-06 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001228
Hervé Tissot, Martijn Van Heel, Mark E Feinberg, Lindsey R Gedaly, Elizabeth Joan Barham, Filip Calders, Elena Camisasca, Thais Ramos de Carvalho, Mustafa Çetin, Cindy-Lee Dennis, Nicolas Favez, Bárbara Figueiredo, Sarah Galdiolo, Maham Khawaja, Diogo Lamela, Rachel M Latham, Na Luo, Clarisse Mosmann, Yasuka Nakamura, Bonamy R Oliver, Tiago Miguel Pinto, Norma Perez-Brena, Isabelle Roskam, Dana Shai, Yoko Takeishi, Karla Van Leeuwen, Michael B Wells, Weiman Xu

The purpose of this study was to assess the factor structure and the measurement invariance of the Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS) across 10 countries based on the seven-factor coparenting model (i.e., Coparenting Agreement, Coparenting Closeness, Exposure to Conflict, Coparenting Support, Endorsement of Partner's Parenting; Division of Labor) proposed by Feinberg (2003). The results of research on coparenting from numerous countries have documented its foundational importance for parent mental health, family relationship quality, child development, and psychopathology. Yet, a cross-country perspective is still lacking. Such a perspective can provide insight into which dimensions of coparenting are universally recognized and which are especially prone to variation. A unique multinational data set, comprised of 15 individual studies collected across 10 countries (Belgium, Brazil, China, Israel, Italy, Japan, Portugal, Switzerland, Turkey, USA) in nine languages was established (N = 9,292; 51.1% mothers). Measurement invariance analyses were conducted. A six-factor structure (original seven factors minus Division of Labor) of the measure was consistent across the different contexts and measurement invariance was achieved at the configural level. There was no support for metric or scalar invariance. These findings provide a basis for the CRS to be used across countries and should inspire future quantitative and qualitative research in cross-country coparenting research to understand what aspects are universal and what aspects of coparenting are linked to specific material, relational, or ideational conditions that underlie high-quality coparenting. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

本研究的目的是根据 Feinberg(2003 年)提出的七因素共同养育模型(即共同养育协议、共同养育亲密度、冲突暴露、共同养育支持、认可伴侣的养育行为;劳动分工),评估 10 个国家的共同养育关系量表(CRS)的因素结构和测量不变性。许多国家对共同养育的研究结果表明,共同养育对父母的心理健康、家庭关系质量、儿童发展和心理病理学具有重要的基础性作用。然而,目前仍缺乏跨国视角。这种视角可以让我们深入了解共同养育的哪些方面得到了普遍认可,哪些方面特别容易发生变化。本研究建立了一个独特的多国数据集,其中包括在 10 个国家(比利时、巴西、中国、以色列、意大利、日本、葡萄牙、瑞士、土耳其、美国)用 9 种语言收集的 15 项单独研究(N = 9292;51.1% 为母亲)。进行了测量不变性分析。在不同的情境下,测量的六因子结构(原始七因子减去劳动分工)是一致的,并且在配置水平上实现了测量不变性。不支持度量或标度不变性。这些发现为 CRS 在不同国家间的使用提供了依据,并应激励未来在跨国共同养育研究中开展定量和定性研究,以了解共同养育的哪些方面具有普遍性,以及共同养育的哪些方面与支撑高质量共同养育的特定物质、关系或意识形态条件相关联。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Measurement invariance of the Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS) across 10 countries.","authors":"Hervé Tissot, Martijn Van Heel, Mark E Feinberg, Lindsey R Gedaly, Elizabeth Joan Barham, Filip Calders, Elena Camisasca, Thais Ramos de Carvalho, Mustafa Çetin, Cindy-Lee Dennis, Nicolas Favez, Bárbara Figueiredo, Sarah Galdiolo, Maham Khawaja, Diogo Lamela, Rachel M Latham, Na Luo, Clarisse Mosmann, Yasuka Nakamura, Bonamy R Oliver, Tiago Miguel Pinto, Norma Perez-Brena, Isabelle Roskam, Dana Shai, Yoko Takeishi, Karla Van Leeuwen, Michael B Wells, Weiman Xu","doi":"10.1037/fam0001228","DOIUrl":"10.1037/fam0001228","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The purpose of this study was to assess the factor structure and the measurement invariance of the Coparenting Relationship Scale (CRS) across 10 countries based on the seven-factor coparenting model (i.e., Coparenting Agreement, Coparenting Closeness, Exposure to Conflict, Coparenting Support, Endorsement of Partner's Parenting; Division of Labor) proposed by Feinberg (2003). The results of research on coparenting from numerous countries have documented its foundational importance for parent mental health, family relationship quality, child development, and psychopathology. Yet, a cross-country perspective is still lacking. Such a perspective can provide insight into which dimensions of coparenting are universally recognized and which are especially prone to variation. A unique multinational data set, comprised of 15 individual studies collected across 10 countries (Belgium, Brazil, China, Israel, Italy, Japan, Portugal, Switzerland, Turkey, USA) in nine languages was established (<i>N</i> = 9,292; 51.1% mothers). Measurement invariance analyses were conducted. A six-factor structure (original seven factors minus Division of Labor) of the measure was consistent across the different contexts and measurement invariance was achieved at the configural level. There was no support for metric or scalar invariance. These findings provide a basis for the CRS to be used across countries and should inspire future quantitative and qualitative research in cross-country coparenting research to understand what aspects are universal and what aspects of coparenting are linked to specific material, relational, or ideational conditions that underlie high-quality coparenting. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141263178","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Bidirectional relations between parents' discipline strategies and children's inhibitory control from early to middle childhood. 从幼儿期到中期,父母的管教策略与儿童的抑制控制之间的双向关系。
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001261
Daphne M Vrantsidis, Naaila Ali, Mahsa Khoei, Sandra A Wiebe

Parents' use of harsh discipline and inductive discipline are theorized to change normatively across the transition from early to middle childhood (ages 5-7 years), with harsh discipline decreasing and inductive discipline increasing. Importantly, within-person support for these changes is lacking. Additionally, these changes are argued to be driven by improvements in children's cognitive and social abilities, including inhibitory control. The present study examined within-family bidirectional relations between parents' (primarily mothers') harsh discipline, inductive discipline, and children's inhibitory control across the transition to middle childhood. Participants were 118 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and their primary caregivers. At two time points, separated by 12 months, parents completed a questionnaire about their use of harsh and inductive discipline, and children completed a battery of inhibitory control tasks. A latent change score model was used to examine within-family bidirectional relations. More inductive discipline at Time 1 was related to a decline in harsh discipline over the year for parents of 5- and 6-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds. Harsh discipline at Time 1 was not related to change in inductive discipline. Parental discipline was not related to the change in children's inhibitory control, nor was inhibitory control related to the change in parental discipline. Relations between inductive discipline and change in harsh discipline provide within-person support for expectations of normative change in parents' discipline strategies across the transition to middle childhood. Further research examining when parents of 4-year-olds make this transition and predictors of change in parental discipline will inform research of normative trajectories of parental discipline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

据推测,在从幼儿期向中幼儿期(5-7 岁)过渡的过程中,父母使用严厉管教和归纳管教的方式会发生规范性变化,严厉管教的方式会减少,归纳管教的方式会增加。重要的是,这些变化缺乏人际支持。此外,这些变化被认为是由儿童认知和社交能力(包括抑制控制能力)的提高所驱动的。本研究考察了父母(主要是母亲)的严厉管教、归纳管教和儿童的抑制控制能力之间的双向关系。研究对象是 118 名 4、5 和 6 岁儿童及其主要照顾者。在相隔 12 个月的两个时间点,父母填写了一份关于他们使用严厉管教和诱导管教的问卷,儿童则完成了一系列抑制控制任务。我们采用了一个潜在变化评分模型来研究家庭内部的双向关系。对于 5 岁和 6 岁儿童的家长来说,在第一时间进行更多的归纳管教与一年中严厉管教的减少有关,但与 4 岁儿童的情况无关。第一阶段的严厉管教与归纳管教的变化无关。父母的管教方式与儿童抑制控制能力的变化无关,抑制控制能力与父母管教方式的变化也无关。归纳式管教与严厉管教的变化之间的关系为父母在向中童年过渡期间管教策略的规范性变化提供了人际支持。进一步研究 4 岁儿童的父母何时实现这一转变,以及父母管教方式变化的预测因素,将为父母管教方式的规范性轨迹研究提供参考。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Bidirectional relations between parents' discipline strategies and children's inhibitory control from early to middle childhood.","authors":"Daphne M Vrantsidis, Naaila Ali, Mahsa Khoei, Sandra A Wiebe","doi":"10.1037/fam0001261","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0001261","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Parents' use of harsh discipline and inductive discipline are theorized to change normatively across the transition from early to middle childhood (ages 5-7 years), with harsh discipline decreasing and inductive discipline increasing. Importantly, within-person support for these changes is lacking. Additionally, these changes are argued to be driven by improvements in children's cognitive and social abilities, including inhibitory control. The present study examined within-family bidirectional relations between parents' (primarily mothers') harsh discipline, inductive discipline, and children's inhibitory control across the transition to middle childhood. Participants were 118 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and their primary caregivers. At two time points, separated by 12 months, parents completed a questionnaire about their use of harsh and inductive discipline, and children completed a battery of inhibitory control tasks. A latent change score model was used to examine within-family bidirectional relations. More inductive discipline at Time 1 was related to a decline in harsh discipline over the year for parents of 5- and 6-year-olds, but not 4-year-olds. Harsh discipline at Time 1 was not related to change in inductive discipline. Parental discipline was not related to the change in children's inhibitory control, nor was inhibitory control related to the change in parental discipline. Relations between inductive discipline and change in harsh discipline provide within-person support for expectations of normative change in parents' discipline strategies across the transition to middle childhood. Further research examining when parents of 4-year-olds make this transition and predictors of change in parental discipline will inform research of normative trajectories of parental discipline. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Bidirectional Relations Between Parents’ Discipline Strategies and Children’s Inhibitory Control From Early to Middle Childhood 儿童早期至中期父母管教策略与儿童抑制控制之间的双向关系》补充材料
IF 2.3 2区 心理学 Q2 FAMILY STUDIES Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1037/fam0001261.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Bidirectional Relations Between Parents’ Discipline Strategies and Children’s Inhibitory Control From Early to Middle Childhood","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/fam0001261.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/fam0001261.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48381,"journal":{"name":"Journal of Family Psychology","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.3,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141805925","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":2,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Journal of Family Psychology
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1