首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Science最新文献

英文 中文
The role of translation equivalents in bilingual word learning 翻译对等词在双语词汇学习中的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13476
Alvin W. M. Tan, Virginia A. Marchman, Michael C. Frank

Bilingual environments present an important context for word learning. One feature of bilingual environments is the existence of translation equivalents (TEs)—words in different languages that share similar meanings. Documenting TE learning over development may give us insight into the mechanisms underlying word learning in young bilingual children. Prior studies of TE learning have often been confounded by the fact that increases in overall vocabulary size with age lead to greater opportunities for learning TEs. To address this confound, we employed an item-level analysis, which controls for the age trajectory of each item independently. We used Communicative Development Inventory data from four bilingual datasets (two English–Spanish and two English–French; total N = 419) for modeling. Results indicated that knowing a word's TE increased the likelihood of knowing that word for younger children and for TEs that are more similar phonologically. These effects were consistent across datasets, but varied across lexical categories. Thus, TEs may allow bilingual children to bootstrap their early word learning in one language using their knowledge of the other language.

Research highlights

  • Bilingual children must learn words that share a common meaning across both languages, that is, translation equivalents, like dog in English and perro in Spanish.
  • Item-level models explored how translation equivalents affect word learning, in addition to child-level (e.g., exposure) and item-level (e.g., phonological similarity) factors.
  • Knowing a word increased the probability of knowing its corresponding translation equivalent, particularly for younger children and for more phonologically-similar translation equivalents.
  • These findings suggest that young bilingual children use their word knowledge in one language to bootstrap their learning of words in the other language.
双语环境为单词学习提供了重要的语境。双语环境的一个特点是存在翻译等效词(TEs)--不同语言中具有相似含义的单词。记录双语儿童在成长过程中的翻译对等词学习情况,可以帮助我们深入了解双语儿童词汇学习的内在机制。以往关于翻译等效词学习的研究常常被这样一个事实所混淆,即随着年龄的增长,词汇总量的增加会带来更多学习翻译等效词的机会。为了解决这个问题,我们采用了项目层面的分析方法,即对每个项目的年龄轨迹进行独立控制。我们使用了四个双语数据集(两个英语-西班牙语数据集和两个英语-法语数据集;总人数=419)中的 "交际发展量表 "数据进行建模。结果表明,对于年龄较小的儿童来说,知道一个单词的TE会增加他们知道这个单词的可能性,而对于语音上更相似的TE来说,知道TE也会增加他们知道这个单词的可能性。这些效应在不同的数据集上是一致的,但在不同的词汇类别上则有所不同。因此,TE 可以让双语儿童利用他们对另一种语言的了解来引导他们对一种语言的早期单词学习。研究亮点:双语儿童必须学习在两种语言中具有共同含义的单词,即翻译对等词,如英语中的 dog 和西班牙语中的 perro。除了儿童层面(如接触)和项目层面(如语音相似性)的因素外,项目层面的模型还探讨了翻译对等词如何影响单词学习。知道一个单词会增加知道其相应翻译等同物的概率,尤其是对于年龄较小的儿童和语音相似性较高的翻译等同物。这些研究结果表明,年幼的双语儿童利用他们在一种语言中的单词知识来引导他们学习另一种语言中的单词。
{"title":"The role of translation equivalents in bilingual word learning","authors":"Alvin W. M. Tan,&nbsp;Virginia A. Marchman,&nbsp;Michael C. Frank","doi":"10.1111/desc.13476","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13476","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Bilingual environments present an important context for word learning. One feature of bilingual environments is the existence of translation equivalents (TEs)—words in different languages that share similar meanings. Documenting TE learning over development may give us insight into the mechanisms underlying word learning in young bilingual children. Prior studies of TE learning have often been confounded by the fact that increases in overall vocabulary size with age lead to greater opportunities for learning TEs. To address this confound, we employed an item-level analysis, which controls for the age trajectory of each item independently. We used Communicative Development Inventory data from four bilingual datasets (two English–Spanish and two English–French; total <i>N</i> = 419) for modeling. Results indicated that knowing a word's TE increased the likelihood of knowing that word for younger children and for TEs that are more similar phonologically. These effects were consistent across datasets, but varied across lexical categories. Thus, TEs may allow bilingual children to bootstrap their early word learning in one language using their knowledge of the other language.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Bilingual children must learn words that share a common meaning across both languages, that is, translation equivalents, like <i>dog</i> in English and <i>perro</i> in Spanish.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Item-level models explored how translation equivalents affect word learning, in addition to child-level (e.g., exposure) and item-level (e.g., phonological similarity) factors.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Knowing a word increased the probability of knowing its corresponding translation equivalent, particularly for younger children and for more phonologically-similar translation equivalents.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>These findings suggest that young bilingual children use their word knowledge in one language to bootstrap their learning of words in the other language.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139472850","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of vision in the acquisition of words: Vocabulary development in blind toddlers 视觉在词汇学习中的作用:盲人幼儿的词汇发展。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-16 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13475
Erin Campbell, Robyn Casillas, Elika Bergelson

What is vision's role in driving early word production? To answer this, we assessed parent-report vocabulary questionnaires administered to congenitally blind children (N = 40, Mean age = 24 months [R: 7–57 months]) and compared the size and contents of their productive vocabulary to those of a large normative sample of sighted children (N = 6574). We found that on average, blind children showed a roughly half-year vocabulary delay relative to sighted children, amid considerable variability. However, the content of blind and sighted children's vocabulary was statistically indistinguishable in word length, part of speech, semantic category, concreteness, interactiveness, and perceptual modality. At a finer-grained level, we also found that words’ perceptual properties intersect with children's perceptual abilities. Our findings suggest that while an absence of visual input may initially make vocabulary development more difficult, the content of the early productive vocabulary is largely resilient to differences in perceptual access.

Research Highlights

  • Infants and toddlers born blind (with no other diagnoses) show a 7.5 month productive vocabulary delay on average, with wide variability.
  • Across the studied age range (7–57 months), vocabulary delays widened with age.
  • Blind and sighted children's early vocabularies contain similar distributions of word lengths, parts of speech, semantic categories, and perceptual modalities.
  • Blind children (but not sighted children) were more likely to say visual words which could also be experienced through other senses.
视觉在早期词汇生成中起什么作用?为了回答这个问题,我们对先天性失明儿童(40 名,平均年龄为 24 个月 [R:7-57 个月])的家长报告词汇量问卷进行了评估,并将其生产词汇量的大小和内容与大量正常视力儿童样本(6574 名)进行了比较。我们发现,与视力正常的儿童相比,盲童的词汇量平均延迟了大约半年,但差异很大。然而,盲童和健视儿童的词汇内容在词长、语篇、语义类别、具体性、互动性和感知模式等方面却没有统计学差异。在更细的层面上,我们还发现词汇的感知属性与儿童的感知能力存在交叉。我们的研究结果表明,虽然缺乏视觉输入可能会在初期增加词汇发展的难度,但早期生产性词汇的内容在很大程度上能够抵御感知能力差异的影响。研究亮点:先天失明的婴幼儿(无其他诊断)的生产性词汇平均延迟 7.5 个月,但差异很大。在所研究的年龄范围内(7-57 个月),随着年龄的增长,词汇延迟的范围也在扩大。盲童和健视儿童的早期词汇在词长、语篇、语义类别和感知模式方面的分布相似。盲童(而非视力正常儿童)更有可能说出通过其他感官也能感受到的视觉词汇。
{"title":"The role of vision in the acquisition of words: Vocabulary development in blind toddlers","authors":"Erin Campbell,&nbsp;Robyn Casillas,&nbsp;Elika Bergelson","doi":"10.1111/desc.13475","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13475","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>What is vision's role in driving early word production? To answer this, we assessed parent-report vocabulary questionnaires administered to congenitally blind children (<i>N</i> = 40, Mean age = 24 months [<i>R</i>: 7–57 months]) and compared the size and contents of their productive vocabulary to those of a large normative sample of sighted children (<i>N</i> = 6574). We found that on average, blind children showed a roughly half-year vocabulary delay relative to sighted children, amid considerable variability. However, the content of blind and sighted children's vocabulary was statistically indistinguishable in word length, part of speech, semantic category, concreteness, interactiveness, and perceptual modality. At a finer-grained level, we also found that words’ perceptual properties intersect with children's perceptual abilities. Our findings suggest that while an absence of visual input may initially make vocabulary development more difficult, the content of the early productive vocabulary is largely resilient to differences in perceptual access.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Infants and toddlers born blind (with no other diagnoses) show a 7.5 month productive vocabulary delay on average, with wide variability.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Across the studied age range (7–57 months), vocabulary delays widened with age.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Blind and sighted children's early vocabularies contain similar distributions of word lengths, parts of speech, semantic categories, and perceptual modalities.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Blind children (but not sighted children) were more likely to say visual words which could also be experienced through other senses.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139479520","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The personal epistemology of parents predicts the development of scientific reasoning in children aged 6–10 years 父母的个人认识论可预测 6-10 岁儿童科学推理能力的发展。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-11 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13474
Christopher Osterhaus, Susanne Koerber

The influence of the epistemological beliefs of parents on the development of comprehensive scientific reasoning abilities was investigated in a five-wave longitudinal study from kindergarten to elementary school. The 161 German 5–10-year-olds (89 girls, 72 boys) were assessed yearly on their scientific reasoning abilities using comprehensive measures for experimentation and data-interpretation skills, as well as understanding of the nature of science. The children were also tested on their language abilities and intelligence. Their parents completed a sociodemographics questionnaire and answered ten questions about their epistemological beliefs regarding (1) the interpretive nature of science, (2) the tentative nature of knowledge, and (3) the role of scientific framework theories. The personal epistemology of the parents significantly predicted the scientific reasoning development of their children regardless of the parents’ education level and the children's general cognitive abilities. However, the effect of the epistemology of parents on their children's scientific reasoning was limited to the intercepts, suggesting that the epistemic understanding of parents affects how scientific reasoning develops in their children, but not the development speed. Although parental epistemology exerts substantial effects on scientific reasoning of their children, it did not affect their reading ability, suggesting an involvement of science-specific mechanisms rather than generalized family-based influences. These findings highlight the importance of family as a variable in the development of scientific reasoning, which is an area lacking in research, and it suggests that early interventions targeted at the epistemic understanding of caregivers can provide useful ways for promoting the reasoning of children.

Research Highlights

  • A five-year longitudinal study shows significant development of scientific reasoning from kindergarten to elementary school.
  • Caregivers’ personal epistemology predicted scientific reasoning development—independent of children's general cognitive abilities and caregivers’ level of education.
  • The effect was most-pronounced for caregivers’ understanding that social framework theories determine which aspects of science are accepted and how they are conducted.
  • Caregivers’ personal epistemology did not predict children's reading abilities, suggesting that the effect of the caregivers’ epistemology on children's scientific reasoning is domain-specific.
在一项从幼儿园到小学的五波纵向研究中,调查了父母的认识论信念对综合科学推理能力发展的影响。每年都会对 161 名 5-10 岁的德国儿童(89 名女孩,72 名男孩)的科学推理能力进行评估,评估方法包括实验和数据解释技能的综合测量,以及对科学本质的理解。他们还接受了语言能力和智力测试。他们的父母填写了一份社会人口调查问卷,并回答了十个有关他们认识论信念的问题,其中包括:(1) 科学的解释性质;(2) 知识的暂定性质;(3) 科学框架理论的作用。无论父母的教育水平和孩子的一般认知能力如何,父母的个人认识论都能显著预测孩子的科学推理能力发展。然而,父母的认识论对子女科学推理的影响仅限于截距,这表明父母的认识论理解会影响子女科学推理的发展,但不会影响其发展速度。虽然父母的认识论对其子女的科学推理能力有很大影响,但并不影响他们的阅读能力,这表明其中涉及到科学的特定机制,而不是基于家庭的普遍影响。这些发现强调了家庭作为一个变量在科学推理发展中的重要性,而这正是一个缺乏研究的领域,研究还表明,针对照顾者的认识论理解的早期干预可以为促进儿童推理提供有用的方法。研究亮点:一项为期五年的纵向研究显示,从幼儿园到小学,儿童的科学推理能力有了显著发展。照顾者的个人认识论预示着科学推理的发展,而与儿童的一般认知能力和照顾者的教育水平无关。对于照护者来说,理解社会框架理论决定了科学的哪些方面被接受以及如何进行科学推理的效果最为明显。照顾者的个人认识论并不能预测儿童的阅读能力,这表明照顾者的认识论对儿童科学推理的影响是针对特定领域的。
{"title":"The personal epistemology of parents predicts the development of scientific reasoning in children aged 6–10 years","authors":"Christopher Osterhaus,&nbsp;Susanne Koerber","doi":"10.1111/desc.13474","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13474","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>The influence of the epistemological beliefs of parents on the development of comprehensive scientific reasoning abilities was investigated in a five-wave longitudinal study from kindergarten to elementary school. The 161 German 5–10-year-olds (89 girls, 72 boys) were assessed yearly on their scientific reasoning abilities using comprehensive measures for experimentation and data-interpretation skills, as well as understanding of the nature of science. The children were also tested on their language abilities and intelligence. Their parents completed a sociodemographics questionnaire and answered ten questions about their epistemological beliefs regarding (1) the interpretive nature of science, (2) the tentative nature of knowledge, and (3) the role of scientific framework theories. The personal epistemology of the parents significantly predicted the scientific reasoning development of their children regardless of the parents’ education level and the children's general cognitive abilities. However, the effect of the epistemology of parents on their children's scientific reasoning was limited to the intercepts, suggesting that the epistemic understanding of parents affects how scientific reasoning develops in their children, but not the development speed. Although parental epistemology exerts substantial effects on scientific reasoning of their children, it did not affect their reading ability, suggesting an involvement of science-specific mechanisms rather than generalized family-based influences. These findings highlight the importance of family as a variable in the development of scientific reasoning, which is an area lacking in research, and it suggests that early interventions targeted at the epistemic understanding of caregivers can provide useful ways for promoting the reasoning of children.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A five-year longitudinal study shows significant development of scientific reasoning from kindergarten to elementary school.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Caregivers’ personal epistemology predicted scientific reasoning development—independent of children's general cognitive abilities and caregivers’ level of education.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The effect was most-pronounced for caregivers’ understanding that social framework theories determine which aspects of science are accepted and how they are conducted.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Caregivers’ personal epistemology did not predict children's reading abilities, suggesting that the effect of the caregivers’ epistemology on children's scientific reasoning is domain-specific.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 ","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13474","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139425777","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Act generously when others do so: Majority influence on young children's sharing behavior 当别人这样做时,自己也会慷慨解囊:多数人对幼儿分享行为的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-10 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13472
Qiao Chai, Jun Yin, Mowei Shen, Jie He

Children's sharing behavior is profoundly shaped by social norms within their society, and they can learn these norms by directly observing how most others share in their immediate environment. Here we systematically investigated the impact of majority influence on the sharing behavior of young Chinese children through three studies (N = 336, 168 girls). Four- and 6-year-olds were allowed to choose 10 favorite stickers and had an opportunity to engage in anonymous sharing. Before making the sharing decision, children were assigned to one of two conditions: watching a video in which three peers all shared 8 out of 10 stickers (i.e., the majority sharing condition) or making their decisions without watching the video (i.e., the control condition). Results showed that both the 4- and 6-year-old children shared more stickers in the majority sharing condition than in the control condition (Studies 1 & 2). Moreover, the influence of the majority had a stronger effect compared to the influence of a single role model. Children shared more stickers after observing three peers sharing, compared to watching one peer sharing three times (Study 2). Furthermore, children were less likely to copy the majority's non-sharing behavior when it came to giving away stickers without prosocial outcomes, which was particularly evident among 4-year-olds (Study 3). The results reveal that majority influence uniquely shapes children's sharing behavior and that children selectively follow the majority based on whether the behavior exhibits prosocial attributes. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/8qNNhf9754I?si=7YfpaFpcD_IjlXjJ

Research Highlights

  • Observing a majority of three peers’ unanimous generous sharing promoted sharing behavior in both 4- and 6-year-olds.
  • The influence of three peers on children's sharing was stronger than that of one peer sharing three times.
  • Four-year-olds, but not 6-year-olds, did not copy the non-sharing behavior of the majority as it did not lead to prosocial outcomes.
儿童的分享行为深受社会规范的影响,他们可以通过直接观察周围环境中大多数人的分享方式来学习这些规范。在此,我们通过三项研究系统地探讨了多数影响对中国幼儿分享行为的影响(研究对象为336人,其中168人为女孩)。我们允许 4 岁和 6 岁的儿童选择 10 张最喜欢的贴纸,并让他们有机会进行匿名分享。在做出分享决定之前,孩子们被分配到两个条件中的一个:观看三位同伴都分享了 10 张贴纸中的 8 张的视频(即多数分享条件)或不观看视频而做出决定(即对照条件)。结果显示,在多数分享条件下,4 岁和 6 岁儿童分享的贴纸都比对照条件下多(研究 1 和 2)。此外,与单一榜样的影响相比,多数榜样的影响更大。与观看一个同伴分享三次贴纸相比,儿童在观看三个同伴分享贴纸后分享的贴纸更多(研究 2)。此外,在没有亲社会结果的情况下,儿童不太可能模仿大多数人不分享贴纸的行为,这一点在 4 岁儿童中尤为明显(研究 3)。研究结果表明,多数人的影响独特地塑造了儿童的分享行为,而且儿童会根据行为是否具有亲社会属性而选择性地追随多数人。研究亮点观察三个同伴中大多数人一致慷慨分享的行为会促进 4 岁和 6 岁儿童的分享行为。三个同伴对儿童分享行为的影响比一个同伴分享三次的影响更大。4 岁儿童(而非 6 岁儿童)不会模仿大多数人的不分享行为,因为这种行为不会带来亲社会的结果。
{"title":"Act generously when others do so: Majority influence on young children's sharing behavior","authors":"Qiao Chai,&nbsp;Jun Yin,&nbsp;Mowei Shen,&nbsp;Jie He","doi":"10.1111/desc.13472","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13472","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Children's sharing behavior is profoundly shaped by social norms within their society, and they can learn these norms by directly observing how most others share in their immediate environment. Here we systematically investigated the impact of majority influence on the sharing behavior of young Chinese children through three studies (<i>N</i> = 336, 168 girls). Four- and 6-year-olds were allowed to choose 10 favorite stickers and had an opportunity to engage in anonymous sharing. Before making the sharing decision, children were assigned to one of two conditions: watching a video in which three peers all shared 8 out of 10 stickers (i.e., the majority sharing condition) or making their decisions without watching the video (i.e., the control condition). Results showed that both the 4- and 6-year-old children shared more stickers in the majority sharing condition than in the control condition (Studies 1 &amp; 2). Moreover, the influence of the majority had a stronger effect compared to the influence of a single role model. Children shared more stickers after observing three peers sharing, compared to watching one peer sharing three times (Study 2). Furthermore, children were less likely to copy the majority's non-sharing behavior when it came to giving away stickers without prosocial outcomes, which was particularly evident among 4-year-olds (Study 3). The results reveal that majority influence uniquely shapes children's sharing behavior and that children selectively follow the majority based on whether the behavior exhibits prosocial attributes. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/8qNNhf9754I?si=7YfpaFpcD_IjlXjJ</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Observing a majority of three peers’ unanimous generous sharing promoted sharing behavior in both 4- and 6-year-olds.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The influence of three peers on children's sharing was stronger than that of one peer sharing three times.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Four-year-olds, but not 6-year-olds, did not copy the non-sharing behavior of the majority as it did not lead to prosocial outcomes.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139404789","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Digital rhythm training improves reading fluency in children 数字节奏训练可提高儿童的阅读流畅性。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-09 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13473
Theodore P. Zanto, Anastasia Giannakopoulou, Courtney L. Gallen, Avery E. Ostrand, Jessica W. Younger, Roger Anguera-Singla, Joaquin A. Anguera, Adam Gazzaley

Musical instrument training has been linked to improved academic and cognitive abilities in children, but it remains unclear why this occurs. Moreover, access to instrument training is not always feasible, thereby leaving less fortunate children without opportunity to benefit from such training. Although music-based video games may be more accessible to a broader population, research is lacking regarding their benefits on academic and cognitive performance. To address this gap, we assessed a custom-designed, digital rhythm training game as a proxy for instrument training to evaluate its ability to engender benefits in math and reading abilities. Furthermore, we tested for changes in core cognitive functions related to math and reading to inform how rhythm training may facilitate improved academic abilities. Classrooms of 8–9 year old children were randomized to receive either 6 weeks of rhythm training (N = 32) or classroom instruction as usual (control; N = 21). Compared to the control group, results showed that rhythm training improved reading, but not math, fluency. Assessments of cognition showed that rhythm training also led to improved rhythmic timing and language-based executive function (Stroop task), but not sustained attention, inhibitory control, or working memory. Interestingly, only the improvements in rhythmic timing correlated with improvements in reading ability. Together, these results provide novel evidence that a digital platform may serve as a proxy for musical instrument training to facilitate reading fluency in children, and that such reading improvements are related to enhanced rhythmic timing ability and not other cognitive functions associated with reading performance.

Research Highlights

  • Digital rhythm training in the classroom can improve reading fluency in 8–9 year old children
  • Improvements in reading fluency were positively correlated with enhanced rhythmic timing ability
  • Alterations in reading fluency were not predicted by changes in other executive functions that support reading
  • A digital platform may be a convenient and cost-effective means to provide musical rhythm training, which in turn, can facilitate academic skills
乐器培训与儿童学业和认知能力的提高有关,但目前仍不清楚出现这种情况的原因。此外,接受乐器培训并不总是可行的,因此,较不幸的儿童没有机会从这种培训中受益。虽然音乐类电子游戏可能更容易被更多人接受,但有关其对学业和认知表现的益处的研究还很缺乏。为了填补这一空白,我们评估了一款定制设计的数字节奏训练游戏,将其作为乐器训练的替代品,以评估其对数学和阅读能力的益处。此外,我们还测试了与数学和阅读相关的核心认知功能的变化,以了解节奏训练如何促进学习能力的提高。8-9岁儿童被随机分配到接受为期6周的节奏训练(32人)或照常接受课堂教学(21人)。结果显示,与对照组相比,节奏训练提高了阅读流利度,但数学流利度没有提高。对认知能力的评估显示,节奏训练也提高了节奏计时能力和语言执行功能(Stroop 任务),但没有提高持续注意力、抑制控制能力或工作记忆能力。有趣的是,只有节奏计时能力的提高与阅读能力的提高相关。总之,这些结果提供了新的证据,证明数字平台可以作为乐器训练的替代品,促进儿童阅读的流畅性,而且这种阅读能力的提高与节奏计时能力的增强有关,而与阅读表现相关的其他认知功能无关。研究亮点在课堂上进行数字节奏训练可提高 8-9 岁儿童的阅读流利度 阅读流利度的提高与节奏计时能力的增强呈正相关。
{"title":"Digital rhythm training improves reading fluency in children","authors":"Theodore P. Zanto,&nbsp;Anastasia Giannakopoulou,&nbsp;Courtney L. Gallen,&nbsp;Avery E. Ostrand,&nbsp;Jessica W. Younger,&nbsp;Roger Anguera-Singla,&nbsp;Joaquin A. Anguera,&nbsp;Adam Gazzaley","doi":"10.1111/desc.13473","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13473","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Musical instrument training has been linked to improved academic and cognitive abilities in children, but it remains unclear why this occurs. Moreover, access to instrument training is not always feasible, thereby leaving less fortunate children without opportunity to benefit from such training. Although music-based video games may be more accessible to a broader population, research is lacking regarding their benefits on academic and cognitive performance. To address this gap, we assessed a custom-designed, digital rhythm training game as a proxy for instrument training to evaluate its ability to engender benefits in math and reading abilities. Furthermore, we tested for changes in core cognitive functions related to math and reading to inform how rhythm training may facilitate improved academic abilities. Classrooms of 8–9 year old children were randomized to receive either 6 weeks of rhythm training (<i>N</i> = 32) or classroom instruction as usual (control; <i>N</i> = 21). Compared to the control group, results showed that rhythm training improved reading, but not math, fluency. Assessments of cognition showed that rhythm training also led to improved rhythmic timing and language-based executive function (Stroop task), but not sustained attention, inhibitory control, or working memory. Interestingly, only the improvements in rhythmic timing correlated with improvements in reading ability. Together, these results provide novel evidence that a digital platform may serve as a proxy for musical instrument training to facilitate reading fluency in children, and that such reading improvements are related to enhanced rhythmic timing ability and not other cognitive functions associated with reading performance.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Digital rhythm training in the classroom can improve reading fluency in 8–9 year old children</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Improvements in reading fluency were positively correlated with enhanced rhythmic timing ability</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Alterations in reading fluency were not predicted by changes in other executive functions that support reading</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A digital platform may be a convenient and cost-effective means to provide musical rhythm training, which in turn, can facilitate academic skills</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-09","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13473","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139404790","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Individual differences in processing speed and curiosity explain infant habituation and dishabituation performance 处理速度和好奇心的个体差异解释了婴儿的习惯化和非习惯化表现。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-28 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13460
Francesco Poli, Tommaso Ghilardi, Roseriet Beijers, Carolina de Weerth, Max Hinne, Rogier B. Mars, Sabine Hunnius

Habituation and dishabituation are the most prevalent measures of infant cognitive functioning, and they have reliably been shown to predict later cognitive outcomes. Yet, the exact mechanisms underlying infant habituation and dishabituation are still unclear. To investigate them, we tested 106 8-month-old infants on a classic habituation task and a novel visual learning task. We used a hierarchical Bayesian model to identify individual differences in sustained attention, learning performance, processing speed and curiosity from the visual learning task. These factors were then related to habituation and dishabituation. We found that habituation time was related to individual differences in processing speed, while dishabituation was related to curiosity, but only for infants who did not habituate. These results offer novel insights in the mechanisms underlying habituation and serve as proof of concept for hierarchical models as an effective tool to measure individual differences in infant cognitive functioning.

Research Highlights

  • We used a hierarchical Bayesian model to measure individual differences in infants’ processing speed, learning performance, sustained attention, and curiosity.
  • Faster processing speed was related to shorter habituation time.
  • High curiosity was related to stronger dishabituation responses, but only for infants who did not habituate.
习惯化和非习惯化是婴儿认知功能最普遍的测量方法,它们已被可靠地证明可预测日后的认知结果。然而,婴儿习惯化和非习惯化的确切机制仍不清楚。为了研究这些机制,我们对 106 名 8 个月大的婴儿进行了经典的习惯化任务和新颖的视觉学习任务测试。我们使用分层贝叶斯模型从视觉学习任务中找出了持续注意力、学习成绩、处理速度和好奇心的个体差异。然后将这些因素与习惯化和非习惯化联系起来。我们发现,习惯化时间与处理速度的个体差异有关,而非习惯化与好奇心有关,但只针对没有习惯化的婴儿。这些结果为研究习惯化的内在机制提供了新的视角,并证明了分层模型是测量婴儿认知功能个体差异的有效工具。研究亮点:我们使用分层贝叶斯模型来测量婴儿处理速度、学习成绩、持续注意力和好奇心的个体差异。处理速度快与习惯化时间短有关。高好奇心与较强的去习惯化反应有关,但只针对没有习惯化的婴儿。
{"title":"Individual differences in processing speed and curiosity explain infant habituation and dishabituation performance","authors":"Francesco Poli,&nbsp;Tommaso Ghilardi,&nbsp;Roseriet Beijers,&nbsp;Carolina de Weerth,&nbsp;Max Hinne,&nbsp;Rogier B. Mars,&nbsp;Sabine Hunnius","doi":"10.1111/desc.13460","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13460","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Habituation and dishabituation are the most prevalent measures of infant cognitive functioning, and they have reliably been shown to predict later cognitive outcomes. Yet, the exact mechanisms underlying infant habituation and dishabituation are still unclear. To investigate them, we tested 106 8-month-old infants on a classic habituation task and a novel visual learning task. We used a hierarchical Bayesian model to identify individual differences in sustained attention, learning performance, processing speed and curiosity from the visual learning task. These factors were then related to habituation and dishabituation. We found that habituation time was related to individual differences in processing speed, while dishabituation was related to curiosity, but only for infants who did not habituate. These results offer novel insights in the mechanisms underlying habituation and serve as proof of concept for hierarchical models as an effective tool to measure individual differences in infant cognitive functioning.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We used a hierarchical Bayesian model to measure individual differences in infants’ processing speed, learning performance, sustained attention, and curiosity.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Faster processing speed was related to shorter habituation time.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>High curiosity was related to stronger dishabituation responses, but only for infants who did not habituate.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13460","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139058931","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Postpartum romantic attachment and constructiveness: The protective effects of a conflict communication intervention for parents’ relationship functioning over one year 产后浪漫依恋和建构性:冲突沟通干预在一年内对父母关系功能的保护作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-25 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13470
Samantha A. Murray-Perdue, Amanda L. Nowak, Molly J. O'Neill, Lijuan Wang, E. Mark Cummings, Julia M. Braungart-Rieker

Parent relationship functioning has a well-documented influence on children's early socioemotional development as early as infancy. Postpartum parenting is also a critically vulnerable period for relationships and often results in relationship decline. We investigated the effects of a rigorous, psycho-educational conflict communication intervention for supporting parents’ relationship functioning in terms of self-reported romantic attachment and observed conflict constructiveness. Using latent growth curve models, we evaluated the change in romantic attachment and constructiveness among 202 mother-father couples from 6 to 18 months postpartum. We further tested a comparison of the effects of the Conflict Intervention (CI) versus the control group and the Conflict Intervention paired with an additional parent sensitivity intervention (anyCI) versus the control group. Results indicated romantic attachment and observed constructiveness decreased over the 1-year period; this decline was partially mitigated for fathers participating in the intervention(s), wherein fathers who received the Conflict Intervention showed less decline in observed conflict constructiveness over time. Moreover, compared with those in the control condition, mothers who received the Conflict Intervention reported lower attachment security at 18 months postpartum. These results underscore the importance of including perspectives from both mothers and fathers when investigating intervention effects and considering the impact of combining interventions for parents.

Research Highlights

  • A randomized control trial of a conflict intervention including mothers and fathers demonstrates protective effects for fathers’ constructiveness between 6 and 18 months postpartum but was not protective for mothers.
  • Parenting experience, whether parents were transitioning to parenthood or had older children, did not significantly predict romantic attachment or behavioral constructiveness trajectories.
  • Interparental romantic attachment and constructiveness declined only slightly postpartum.
  • Effects of the intervention were reduced when the intervention was combined with a second intervention.
早在婴儿时期,父母的关系功能就对儿童的早期社会情感发展产生了影响,这是有据可查的。产后养育也是关系极其脆弱的时期,往往会导致关系衰退。我们通过自我报告的浪漫依恋和观察到的冲突建构性,研究了严格的心理教育冲突沟通干预对支持父母关系功能的影响。我们使用潜在的成长曲线模型,评估了 202 对母子在产后 6 至 18 个月期间在浪漫依恋和建设性方面的变化。我们进一步测试了冲突干预(CI)与对照组的效果比较,以及冲突干预配以额外的父母敏感性干预(anyCI)与对照组的效果比较。结果表明,浪漫依恋和观察到的建构性在一年的时间里有所下降;这种下降在参与干预的父亲身上得到了部分缓解,其中接受冲突干预的父亲在观察到的冲突建构性方面的下降幅度较小。此外,与对照组相比,接受冲突干预的母亲在产后 18 个月的依恋安全感较低。这些结果表明,在调查干预效果和考虑对父母进行综合干预的影响时,纳入母亲和父亲的观点非常重要。研究亮点:一项包括母亲和父亲的冲突干预随机对照试验表明,父亲的建构性在产后 6 到 18 个月期间具有保护作用,但对母亲没有保护作用。养育子女的经历,无论父母是初为人父还是已育有年长子女,对浪漫依恋或行为建构力轨迹的影响都不大。父母间的浪漫依恋和建构性在产后仅略有下降。当干预与第二项干预相结合时,干预的效果会减弱。
{"title":"Postpartum romantic attachment and constructiveness: The protective effects of a conflict communication intervention for parents’ relationship functioning over one year","authors":"Samantha A. Murray-Perdue,&nbsp;Amanda L. Nowak,&nbsp;Molly J. O'Neill,&nbsp;Lijuan Wang,&nbsp;E. Mark Cummings,&nbsp;Julia M. Braungart-Rieker","doi":"10.1111/desc.13470","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13470","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Parent relationship functioning has a well-documented influence on children's early socioemotional development as early as infancy. Postpartum parenting is also a critically vulnerable period for relationships and often results in relationship decline. We investigated the effects of a rigorous, psycho-educational conflict communication intervention for supporting parents’ relationship functioning in terms of self-reported romantic attachment and observed conflict constructiveness. Using latent growth curve models, we evaluated the change in romantic attachment and constructiveness among 202 mother-father couples from 6 to 18 months postpartum. We further tested a comparison of the effects of the Conflict Intervention (CI) versus the control group and the Conflict Intervention paired with an additional parent sensitivity intervention (anyCI) versus the control group. Results indicated romantic attachment and observed constructiveness decreased over the 1-year period; this decline was partially mitigated for fathers participating in the intervention(s), wherein fathers who received the Conflict Intervention showed less decline in observed conflict constructiveness over time. Moreover, compared with those in the control condition, mothers who received the Conflict Intervention reported lower attachment security at 18 months postpartum. These results underscore the importance of including perspectives from both mothers and fathers when investigating intervention effects and considering the impact of combining interventions for parents.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A randomized control trial of a conflict intervention including mothers and fathers demonstrates protective effects for fathers’ constructiveness between 6 and 18 months postpartum but was not protective for mothers.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Parenting experience, whether parents were transitioning to parenthood or had older children, did not significantly predict romantic attachment or behavioral constructiveness trajectories.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Interparental romantic attachment and constructiveness declined only slightly postpartum.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Effects of the intervention were reduced when the intervention was combined with a second intervention.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13470","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139038126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Uncomfortable staring? Gaze to other people in social situations is inhibited in both infants and adults 不舒服的凝视?无论是婴儿还是成年人,在社交场合注视他人都会受到抑制。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-22 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13468
Louisa Kulke, Sahura Ertugrul, Emely Reyentanz, Vanessa Thomas

People attract infants’ and adults’ gaze when presented on a computer screen. However, in live social situations, adults inhibit their gaze at strangers to avoid sending inappropriate social signals. Such inhibition of gaze has never been directly investigated in infants. The current preregistered study measured gaze and neural responses (EEG alpha power) to a confederate in a live social situation compared to a video of this confederate. Adults looked less at the live confederate than at the video of the confederate, although their neural responses suggest that they were overall equally attentive in both situations. Infants also looked less at the live confederate than at the video of the confederate, with similar neural response patterns. The gaze difference between live social and video situations increased with age. The study shows that young infants are already sensitive to social context and show decreased gaze to strangers in social situations.

Research Highlights

  • This study shows that infants and adults look more at a video of a stranger than at a stranger that is present live in a social situation.
  • Neural responses suggest that adults are equally attentive in both live and video situations but inhibit their gaze at the stranger in live social situations.
  • Infants show a similar pattern of shorter gaze at a stranger who is present in person than at a video of this stranger.
  • The study shows that gaze in infants and adults may diverge from cognitive processes measured through EEG, highlighting the importance of combining behavioural and neural measures in natural interactions.
人在电脑屏幕上出现时,会吸引婴儿和成人的目光。然而,在现场社交场合,成人会抑制对陌生人的注视,以避免发出不恰当的社交信号。这种目光抑制从未在婴儿身上进行过直接研究。当前的预先注册研究测量了在现场社交情境中对同伴的注视和神经反应(脑电图阿尔法功率),并与该同伴的视频进行了比较。尽管成人的神经反应表明他们在这两种情况下的注意力总体上是一样的,但他们注视现场同伴的次数少于注视同伴视频的次数。婴儿注视现场同桌的次数也少于注视同桌视频的次数,他们的神经反应模式也类似。现场社交情境和视频情境之间的注视差异随着年龄的增长而增大。这项研究表明,幼婴对社交环境已经非常敏感,在社交情境中对陌生人的注视会减少。研究亮点:这项研究表明,在社交情境中,婴儿和成人对陌生人视频的注视多于对现场陌生人的注视。神经反应表明,成人在现场和视频情境中同样专注,但在现场社交情境中会抑制对陌生人的注视。婴儿也表现出类似的模式,即注视陌生人本人的时间比注视该陌生人视频的时间短。研究表明,婴儿和成人的注视可能与通过脑电图测量的认知过程不同,这突出了在自然互动中结合行为和神经测量的重要性。
{"title":"Uncomfortable staring? Gaze to other people in social situations is inhibited in both infants and adults","authors":"Louisa Kulke,&nbsp;Sahura Ertugrul,&nbsp;Emely Reyentanz,&nbsp;Vanessa Thomas","doi":"10.1111/desc.13468","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13468","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>People attract infants’ and adults’ gaze when presented on a computer screen. However, in live social situations, adults inhibit their gaze at strangers to avoid sending inappropriate social signals. Such inhibition of gaze has never been directly investigated in infants. The current preregistered study measured gaze and neural responses (EEG alpha power) to a confederate in a live social situation compared to a video of this confederate. Adults looked less at the live confederate than at the video of the confederate, although their neural responses suggest that they were overall equally attentive in both situations. Infants also looked less at the live confederate than at the video of the confederate, with similar neural response patterns. The gaze difference between live social and video situations increased with age. The study shows that young infants are already sensitive to social context and show decreased gaze to strangers in social situations.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study shows that infants and adults look more at a video of a stranger than at a stranger that is present live in a social situation.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Neural responses suggest that adults are equally attentive in both live and video situations but inhibit their gaze at the stranger in live social situations.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Infants show a similar pattern of shorter gaze at a stranger who is present in person than at a video of this stranger.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The study shows that gaze in infants and adults may diverge from cognitive processes measured through EEG, highlighting the importance of combining behavioural and neural measures in natural interactions.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13468","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138886267","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The impact of sufferers’ wealth status on pain perceptions: Its development and relation to allocation of healthcare resources 患者的财富状况对疼痛认知的影响:其发展及其与医疗资源分配的关系。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13467
Yuhang Shu, Huisi (Jessica) Li, Shaocong Ma, Lin Bian

Wealth-based disparities in health care wherein the poor receive undertreatment in painful conditions are a prominent issue that requires immediate attention. Research with adults suggests that these disparities are partly rooted in stereotypes associating poor individuals with pain insensitivity. However, whether and how children consider a sufferer's wealth status in their pain perceptions remains unknown. The present work addressed this question by testing 4- to 9-year-olds from the US and China. In Study 1 (N = 108, 56 girls, 79% White), US participants saw rich and poor White children experiencing identical injuries and indicated who they thought felt more pain. Although 4- to 6-year-olds responded at chance, children aged seven and above attributed more pain to the poor than to the rich. Study 2 with a new sample of US children (N = 111, 56 girls, 69% White) extended this effect to judgments of White adults’ pain. Pain judgments also informed children's prosocial behaviors, leading them to provide medical resources to the poor. Studies 3 (N = 118, 59 girls, 100% Asian) and 4 (N = 80, 40 girls, 100% Asian) found that, when evaluating White and Asian people's suffering, Chinese children began to attribute more pain to the poor than to the rich earlier than US children. Thus, unlike US adults, US children and Chinese children recognize the poor's pain from early on. These findings add to our knowledge of group-based beliefs about pain sensitivity and have broad implications on ways to promote equitable health care.

Research Highlights

  • Four studies examined whether 4- to 9-year-old children's pain perceptions were influenced by sufferers’ wealth status.
  • US children attributed more pain to White individuals of low wealth status than those of high wealth status by age seven.
  • Chinese children demonstrated an earlier tendency to attribute more pain to the poor (versus the rich) compared to US children.
  • Children's wealth-based pain judgments underlied their tendency to provide healthcare resources to people of low wealth status.
医疗保健中的贫富差距是一个亟待解决的突出问题,穷人在疼痛情况下得不到足够的治疗。对成年人的研究表明,这些差异部分源于将穷人与对疼痛不敏感联系在一起的刻板印象。然而,儿童在感知疼痛时是否以及如何考虑患者的财富状况仍是未知数。本研究通过对来自美国和中国的 4 至 9 岁儿童进行测试来解决这一问题。在研究 1(人数 = 108,56 名女孩,79% 为白人)中,美国参与者看到富有和贫穷的白人儿童经历了相同的伤害,并指出他们认为谁感到更疼痛。虽然 4 至 6 岁的儿童的回答是偶然的,但 7 岁及以上的儿童认为穷人比富人更疼。研究 2 采用了新的美国儿童样本(样本数=111,56 名女孩,69% 为白人),将这一效应延伸到了对白人成年人疼痛的判断上。对疼痛的判断也会影响儿童的亲社会行为,促使他们向穷人提供医疗资源。研究 3(人数=118,59 名女孩,100% 亚洲人)和研究 4(人数=80,40 名女孩,100% 亚洲人)发现,在评价白人和亚洲人的痛苦时,中国儿童比美国儿童更早开始将更多痛苦归因于穷人而非富人。因此,与美国成人不同,美国儿童和中国儿童很早就认识到穷人的痛苦。这些发现增加了我们对基于群体的疼痛敏感性信念的了解,并对如何促进公平的医疗保健产生了广泛的影响。研究亮点:四项研究考察了 4 至 9 岁儿童对疼痛的感知是否受患者财富状况的影响。到 7 岁时,美国儿童对财富地位低的白人比财富地位高的白人更容易产生疼痛感。与美国儿童相比,中国儿童更倾向于将更多疼痛归因于穷人(相对于富人)。儿童基于财富的疼痛判断表明,他们倾向于为财富地位低的人提供医疗资源。
{"title":"The impact of sufferers’ wealth status on pain perceptions: Its development and relation to allocation of healthcare resources","authors":"Yuhang Shu,&nbsp;Huisi (Jessica) Li,&nbsp;Shaocong Ma,&nbsp;Lin Bian","doi":"10.1111/desc.13467","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13467","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Wealth-based disparities in health care wherein the poor receive undertreatment in painful conditions are a prominent issue that requires immediate attention. Research with adults suggests that these disparities are partly rooted in stereotypes associating poor individuals with pain insensitivity. However, whether and how children consider a sufferer's wealth status in their pain perceptions remains unknown. The present work addressed this question by testing 4- to 9-year-olds from the US and China. In Study 1 (<i>N</i> = 108, 56 girls, 79% White), US participants saw rich and poor White children experiencing identical injuries and indicated who they thought felt more pain. Although 4- to 6-year-olds responded at chance, children aged seven and above attributed more pain to the poor than to the rich. Study 2 with a new sample of US children (<i>N</i> = 111, 56 girls, 69% White) extended this effect to judgments of White adults’ pain. Pain judgments also informed children's prosocial behaviors, leading them to provide medical resources to the poor. Studies 3 (<i>N</i> = 118, 59 girls, 100% Asian) and 4 (<i>N</i> = 80, 40 girls, 100% Asian) found that, when evaluating White and Asian people's suffering, Chinese children began to attribute more pain to the poor than to the rich earlier than US children. Thus, unlike US adults, US children and Chinese children recognize the poor's pain from early on. These findings add to our knowledge of group-based beliefs about pain sensitivity and have broad implications on ways to promote equitable health care.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Four studies examined whether 4- to 9-year-old children's pain perceptions were influenced by sufferers’ wealth status.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>US children attributed more pain to White individuals of low wealth status than those of high wealth status by age seven.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Chinese children demonstrated an earlier tendency to attribute more pain to the poor (versus the rich) compared to US children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children's wealth-based pain judgments underlied their tendency to provide healthcare resources to people of low wealth status.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13467","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832370","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Strategic social decision making undergoes significant changes in typically developing and autistic early adolescents 在发育典型的青少年和患有自闭症的早期青少年中,战略性社会决策会发生重大变化。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2023-12-21 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13463
Wenda Liu, Nikita Shah, Ili Ma, Gabriela Rosenblau

Information sampling about others’ trustworthiness prior to cooperation allows humans to minimize the risk of exploitation. Here, we examined whether early adolescence or preadolescence, a stage defined as in between childhood and adolescence, is a significant developmental period for strategic social decisions. We also sought to characterize differences between autistic children and their typically developing (TD) peers. TD (N = 48) and autistic (N = 56) 8- to 12-year-olds played an online information sampling trust game. While both groups adapted their information sampling and cooperation to the various trustworthiness levels of the trustees, groups differed in how age and social skills modulated task behavior. In the TD group social skills were a stronger overall predictor of task behavior. In the autistic group, age was a stronger predictor and interacted with social skills. Computational modeling revealed that both groups used the same heuristic information sampling strategy—albeit older TD children were more efficient as reflected by decreasing decision noise with age. Autistic children had lower prior beliefs about the trustee's trustworthiness compared to TD children. These lower priors indicate that children believed the trustees to be less trustworthy. Lower priors scaled with lower social skills across groups. Notably, groups did not differ in prior uncertainty, meaning that the priors of TD and autistic children were equally strong. Taken together, we found significant development in information sampling and cooperation in early adolescence and nuanced differences between TD and autistic children. Our study highlights the importance of deep phenotyping of children including clinical measures, behavioral experiments and computational modeling.

Research Highlights

  • We specified how early adolescents with and without an autism diagnosis sampled information about their interaction partners and made cooperation decisions in a strategic game.
  • Early adolescence is a significant developmental period for strategic decision making, marked by significant changes in information sampling efficiency and adaptivity to the partner's behavior.
  • Autistic and non-autistic groups differed in how age and social skills modulated task behavior; in non-autistic children behavior was more indicative of overall social skills.
  • Computational modeling revealed differences between autistic and non-autistic groups in their initial beliefs about cooperation partners; autistic children expected their partners to be less trustworthy.
在合作之前对他人的可信度进行信息采样,可以使人类最大限度地降低被利用的风险。在此,我们研究了青春期早期或青春期前期(即介于童年和青春期之间的阶段)是否是策略性社会决策的重要发展时期。我们还试图找出自闭症儿童与发育正常(TD)的同龄人之间的差异。8至12岁的TD儿童(48人)和自闭症儿童(56人)玩了一个在线信息采样信任游戏。虽然两组儿童都根据受托人的不同可信度调整了他们的信息采样和合作方式,但两组儿童在年龄和社交技能如何调节任务行为方面却有所不同。在TD组中,社交技能对任务行为的总体预测作用更大。在自闭症组中,年龄是一个更强的预测因素,并与社交技能相互作用。计算模型显示,两组儿童都使用了相同的启发式信息取样策略--尽管年龄较大的自闭症儿童效率更高,这反映在他们的决策噪音随着年龄的增长而减少。与 TD 儿童相比,自闭症儿童对受托人可信度的先验信念较低。这些较低的先验信念表明,儿童认为受托人的可信度较低。较低的先验信念与各组儿童较低的社交能力成正比。值得注意的是,各组在先验不确定性方面没有差异,这意味着TD和自闭症儿童的先验同样强烈。综上所述,我们发现在青春期早期,信息取样和合作能力有了显著发展,而TD儿童和自闭症儿童之间存在细微差别。我们的研究强调了对儿童进行深度表型分析(包括临床测量、行为实验和计算建模)的重要性。研究亮点:我们具体说明了有自闭症诊断和无自闭症诊断的早期青少年如何在战略游戏中采样互动伙伴的信息并做出合作决策。青春期早期是战略决策的重要发展时期,其特点是信息采样效率和对伙伴行为的适应性发生了显著变化。自闭症群体和非自闭症群体在年龄和社交技能如何调节任务行为方面存在差异;非自闭症儿童的行为更能反映其整体社交技能。计算模型显示,自闭症群体和非自闭症群体在最初对合作伙伴的看法上存在差异;自闭症儿童认为他们的合作伙伴不太值得信任。
{"title":"Strategic social decision making undergoes significant changes in typically developing and autistic early adolescents","authors":"Wenda Liu,&nbsp;Nikita Shah,&nbsp;Ili Ma,&nbsp;Gabriela Rosenblau","doi":"10.1111/desc.13463","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13463","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Information sampling about others’ trustworthiness prior to cooperation allows humans to minimize the risk of exploitation. Here, we examined whether early adolescence or preadolescence, a stage defined as in between childhood and adolescence, is a significant developmental period for strategic social decisions. We also sought to characterize differences between autistic children and their typically developing (TD) peers. TD (<i>N</i> = 48) and autistic (<i>N</i> = 56) 8- to 12-year-olds played an online information sampling trust game. While both groups adapted their information sampling and cooperation to the various trustworthiness levels of the trustees, groups differed in how age and social skills modulated task behavior. In the TD group social skills were a stronger overall predictor of task behavior. In the autistic group, age was a stronger predictor and interacted with social skills. Computational modeling revealed that both groups used the same heuristic information sampling strategy—albeit older TD children were more efficient as reflected by decreasing decision noise with age. Autistic children had lower prior beliefs about the trustee's trustworthiness compared to TD children. These lower priors indicate that children believed the trustees to be less trustworthy. Lower priors scaled with lower social skills across groups. Notably, groups did not differ in prior uncertainty, meaning that the priors of TD and autistic children were equally strong. Taken together, we found significant development in information sampling and cooperation in early adolescence and nuanced differences between TD and autistic children. Our study highlights the importance of deep phenotyping of children including clinical measures, behavioral experiments and computational modeling.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We specified how early adolescents with and without an autism diagnosis sampled information about their interaction partners and made cooperation decisions in a strategic game.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Early adolescence is a significant developmental period for strategic decision making, marked by significant changes in information sampling efficiency and adaptivity to the partner's behavior.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Autistic and non-autistic groups differed in how age and social skills modulated task behavior; in non-autistic children behavior was more indicative of overall social skills.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Computational modeling revealed differences between autistic and non-autistic groups in their initial beliefs about cooperation partners; autistic children expected their partners to be less trustworthy.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 ","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-21","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138832369","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1