首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Science最新文献

英文 中文
Mother of all bonds: Influences on spatial association across the lifespan in capuchins. 纽带之母卷尾猴一生中空间联系的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-27 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13486
Irene Godoy, Peter Korsten, Susan E Perry

In humans, being more socially integrated is associated with better physical and mental health and/or with lower mortality. This link between sociality and health may have ancient roots: sociality also predicts survival or reproduction in other mammals, such as rats, dolphins, and non-human primates. A key question, therefore, is which factors influence the degree of sociality over the life course. Longitudinal data can provide valuable insight into how environmental variability drives individual differences in sociality and associated outcomes. The first year of life-when long-lived mammals are the most reliant on others for nourishment and protection-is likely to play an important role in how individuals learn to integrate into groups. Using behavioral, demographic, and pedigree information on 376 wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus imitator) across 20 years, we address how changes in group composition influence spatial association. We further try to determine the extent to which early maternal social environments have downstream effects on sociality across the juvenile and (sub)adult stages. We find a positive effect of early maternal spatial association, where female infants whose mothers spent more time around others also later spent more time around others as juveniles and subadults. Our results also highlight the importance of kin availability and other aspects of group composition (e.g., group size) in dynamically influencing spatial association across developmental stages. We bring attention to the importance of-and difficulty in-determining the social versus genetic influences that parents have on offspring phenotypes. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Having more maternal kin (mother and siblings) is associated with spending more time near others across developmental stages in both male and female capuchins. Having more offspring as a subadult or adult female is additionally associated with spending more time near others. A mother's average sociality (time near others) is predictive of how social her daughters (but not sons) become as juveniles and subadults (a between-mother effect). Additional variation within sibling sets in this same maternal phenotype is not predictive of how social they become later relative to each other (no within-mother effect).

在人类中,社会融合度越高,身心健康越好和/或死亡率越低。社交性与健康之间的这种联系可能有其古老的根源:社交性也能预测其他哺乳动物的生存或繁殖,如大鼠、海豚和非人灵长类动物。因此,一个关键问题是,哪些因素会影响生命过程中的社会性程度。纵向数据可以提供有价值的洞察力,让我们了解环境变化是如何驱动个体在社会性和相关结果方面的差异的。生命的第一年--长寿哺乳动物最依赖他人提供营养和保护的时候--很可能在个体如何学会融入群体方面扮演重要角色。我们利用 20 年间 376 只野生卷尾猴(Cebus imitator)的行为、人口和血统信息,探讨了群体组成的变化如何影响空间关联。我们进一步尝试确定早期母性社会环境对幼年和(亚)成年阶段社会性的下游影响程度。我们发现早期母性空间联结的积极影响,即母亲花更多时间与他人在一起的女婴,后来在少年和亚成体阶段也花更多时间与他人在一起。我们的研究结果还强调了亲缘关系和群体组成的其他方面(如群体大小)在动态影响不同发育阶段的空间关联方面的重要性。我们提请人们注意确定父母对后代表型的社会影响和遗传影响的重要性和难度。研究亮点:在雄性卷尾猴和雌性卷尾猴的各个发育阶段,拥有更多的母系亲属(母亲和兄弟姐妹)与花更多时间接近他人有关。亚成体或成年雌性卷尾猴拥有更多后代也与花更多时间接近他人有关。母亲的平均社会性(接近他人的时间)可以预测其女儿(而不是儿子)在幼年和亚成体阶段的社会性(母亲间效应)。同母异父的兄弟姐妹在这一表型上的额外差异并不能预测他们以后的社会性程度(无母内效应)。
{"title":"Mother of all bonds: Influences on spatial association across the lifespan in capuchins.","authors":"Irene Godoy, Peter Korsten, Susan E Perry","doi":"10.1111/desc.13486","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13486","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In humans, being more socially integrated is associated with better physical and mental health and/or with lower mortality. This link between sociality and health may have ancient roots: sociality also predicts survival or reproduction in other mammals, such as rats, dolphins, and non-human primates. A key question, therefore, is which factors influence the degree of sociality over the life course. Longitudinal data can provide valuable insight into how environmental variability drives individual differences in sociality and associated outcomes. The first year of life-when long-lived mammals are the most reliant on others for nourishment and protection-is likely to play an important role in how individuals learn to integrate into groups. Using behavioral, demographic, and pedigree information on 376 wild capuchin monkeys (Cebus imitator) across 20 years, we address how changes in group composition influence spatial association. We further try to determine the extent to which early maternal social environments have downstream effects on sociality across the juvenile and (sub)adult stages. We find a positive effect of early maternal spatial association, where female infants whose mothers spent more time around others also later spent more time around others as juveniles and subadults. Our results also highlight the importance of kin availability and other aspects of group composition (e.g., group size) in dynamically influencing spatial association across developmental stages. We bring attention to the importance of-and difficulty in-determining the social versus genetic influences that parents have on offspring phenotypes. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Having more maternal kin (mother and siblings) is associated with spending more time near others across developmental stages in both male and female capuchins. Having more offspring as a subadult or adult female is additionally associated with spending more time near others. A mother's average sociality (time near others) is predictive of how social her daughters (but not sons) become as juveniles and subadults (a between-mother effect). Additional variation within sibling sets in this same maternal phenotype is not predictive of how social they become later relative to each other (no within-mother effect).</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-27","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139984155","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Hebbian learning can explain rhythmic neural entrainment to statistical regularities 希比学习可以解释神经节律对统计规律性的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-19 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13487
Ansgar D. Endress

In many domains, learners extract recurring units from continuous sequences. For example, in unknown languages, fluent speech is perceived as a continuous signal. Learners need to extract the underlying words from this continuous signal and then memorize them. One prominent candidate mechanism is statistical learning, whereby learners track how predictive syllables (or other items) are of one another. Syllables within the same word predict each other better than syllables straddling word boundaries. But does statistical learning lead to memories of the underlying words—or just to pairwise associations among syllables? Electrophysiological results provide the strongest evidence for the memory view. Electrophysiological responses can be time-locked to statistical word boundaries (e.g., N400s) and show rhythmic activity with a periodicity of word durations. Here, I reproduce such results with a simple Hebbian network. When exposed to statistically structured syllable sequences (and when the underlying words are not excessively long), the network activation is rhythmic with the periodicity of a word duration and activation maxima on word-final syllables. This is because word-final syllables receive more excitation from earlier syllables with which they are associated than less predictable syllables that occur earlier in words. The network is also sensitive to information whose electrophysiological correlates were used to support the encoding of ordinal positions within words. Hebbian learning can thus explain rhythmic neural activity in statistical learning tasks without any memory representations of words. Learners might thus need to rely on cues beyond statistical associations to learn the words of their native language.

Research Highlights

  • Statistical learning may be utilized to identify recurring units in continuous sequences (e.g., words in fluent speech) but may not generate explicit memory for words.
  • Exposure to statistically structured sequences leads to rhythmic activity with a period of the duration of the underlying units (e.g., words).
  • I show that a memory-less Hebbian network model can reproduce this rhythmic neural activity as well as putative encodings of ordinal positions observed in earlier research.
  • Direct tests are needed to establish whether statistical learning leads to declarative memories for words.
在许多领域,学习者从连续序列中提取重复出现的单元。例如,在未知语言中,流利的语音被视为一个连续的信号。学习者需要从这一连续信号中提取基本单词,然后记忆它们。一个突出的候选机制是统计学习,学习者通过统计学习跟踪音节(或其他项目)之间的预测性。同一单词中的音节比跨越单词边界的音节更能预测彼此。但是,统计学习是否会导致对基本单词的记忆,还是仅仅导致音节之间的成对关联?电生理学结果为记忆观点提供了最有力的证据。电生理反应可以与统计单词边界(如 N400s)时间锁定,并显示出与单词持续时间周期性相关的节律活动。在这里,我用一个简单的希比网络重现了这样的结果。当暴露于统计结构化的音节序列时(当基础单词不是过长时),网络激活会随着单词持续时间的周期性而有节奏地进行,并且在单词最后的音节上激活达到最大值。这是因为词尾音节比词中较早出现的可预测性较低的音节从与之相关的较早音节中获得了更多的兴奋。该网络对其电生理相关信息也很敏感,这些信息被用来支持词内顺序位置的编码。因此,希比学习可以解释统计学习任务中的韵律神经活动,而不需要任何单词记忆表征。因此,学习者可能需要依靠统计联想以外的线索来学习母语单词。研究亮点:统计学习可用于识别连续序列中重复出现的单元(如流利语音中的单词),但可能不会产生明确的单词记忆。接触统计结构序列会导致有节奏的活动,其周期与基础单元(如单词)的持续时间相同。我的研究表明,无记忆海比网络模型可以再现这种有节奏的神经活动,以及早期研究中观察到的顺序位置的推定编码。要确定统计学习是否会导致对单词的陈述性记忆,还需要进行直接测试。
{"title":"Hebbian learning can explain rhythmic neural entrainment to statistical regularities","authors":"Ansgar D. Endress","doi":"10.1111/desc.13487","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13487","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>In many domains, learners extract recurring units from continuous sequences. For example, in unknown languages, fluent speech is perceived as a continuous signal. Learners need to extract the underlying words from this continuous signal and then memorize them. One prominent candidate mechanism is statistical learning, whereby learners track how predictive syllables (or other items) are of one another. Syllables within the same word predict each other better than syllables straddling word boundaries. But does statistical learning lead to memories of the underlying words—or just to pairwise associations among syllables? Electrophysiological results provide the strongest evidence for the memory view. Electrophysiological responses can be time-locked to statistical word boundaries (e.g., N400s) and show rhythmic activity with a periodicity of word durations. Here, I reproduce such results with a simple Hebbian network. When exposed to statistically structured syllable sequences (and when the underlying words are not excessively long), the network activation is rhythmic with the periodicity of a word duration and activation maxima on word-final syllables. This is because word-final syllables receive more excitation from earlier syllables with which they are associated than less predictable syllables that occur earlier in words. The network is also sensitive to information whose electrophysiological correlates were used to support the encoding of ordinal positions within words. Hebbian learning can thus explain rhythmic neural activity in statistical learning tasks without any memory representations of words. Learners might thus need to rely on cues beyond statistical associations to learn the words of their native language.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Statistical learning may be utilized to identify recurring units in continuous sequences (e.g., words in fluent speech) but may not generate explicit memory for words.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Exposure to statistically structured sequences leads to rhythmic activity with a period of the duration of the underlying units (e.g., words).</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>I show that a memory-less Hebbian network model can reproduce this rhythmic neural activity as well as putative encodings of ordinal positions observed in earlier research.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Direct tests are needed to establish whether statistical learning leads to declarative memories for words.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13487","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139900657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
To risk or not: The impact of socioeconomic status on preschoolers’ risky decision-making for gains and losses 冒险与否:社会经济地位对学龄前儿童为收益和损失进行风险决策的影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-13 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13485
Delhii Hoid, Ziyan Guo, Zhibin He, Junhui Wu, Zhen Wu

Disparities in socioeconomic status (SES) may affect individuals’ risk preferences, which have important developmental consequences across the lifespan. Yet, previous research has shown inconsistent associations between SES and risky decision-making, and little is known about how this link develops from a young age. The current research is among the first to examine how SES influences preschoolers’ risky decisions in both gain and loss frames. Across two studies, children aged 5 to 6 years (total N = 309, 154 boys) were asked to choose between certain and risky options. The risky option was more advantageous, equal to, or less advantageous than the certain option. Study 1 revealed that in the loss frame, high-SES children (n = 84, 44 boys) chose more risky options and were more sensitive to the expected value compared to low-SES children (n = 78, 42 boys), especially when the risk was more advantageous. However, this SES difference was not significant in the gain frame. Supporting the potential causal link between SES and risky decision-making, Study 2 further found that experimentally increasing low-SES children's (n = 68, 30 boys) status by providing additional resources increased their risk-seeking behavior in the loss frame. Overall, our findings suggest an interaction between environmental cues (gain vs. loss) and early life circumstances (SES) in shaping children's risk preferences.

Research Highlights

  • This research is among the first to examine how school backgrounds and experimentally manipulated SES influence preschoolers’ risk preferences in gain and loss frames.

  • Children were more risk-seeking for losses than for gains; this framing effect was stronger in higher-SES than lower-SES children.

  • Lower-SES children exhibited fewer risk-seeking behaviors and decreased sensitivity to the expected value of options for losses, but not for gains.

  • A temporary boost in SES increased children's risk-seeking behavior, but not sensitivity to expected values.

社会经济地位(SES)的差异可能会影响个人的风险偏好,而风险偏好会对人一生的发展产生重要影响。然而,以往的研究表明,社会经济地位与风险决策之间的联系并不一致,而且人们对这种联系是如何从幼年发展起来的知之甚少。目前的研究首次考察了社会经济地位如何影响学龄前儿童在收益和损失框架下的风险决策。在两项研究中,5 至 6 岁的儿童(总人数 = 309,其中男孩 154 人)被要求在确定选项和风险选项之间做出选择。风险选项比确定选项更有利、同等有利或更不利。研究 1 显示,在损失框架中,高社会经济地位儿童(n = 84,44 名男孩)与低社会经济地位儿童(n = 78,42 名男孩)相比,选择了更多的风险选项,对预期值更敏感,尤其是当风险更有利时。然而,这种社会经济地位差异在收益框架中并不显著。研究 2 进一步发现,通过提供额外资源来提高低社会经济地位儿童(n = 68,30 名男孩)的地位,会增加他们在损失框架下的风险寻求行为,这支持了社会经济地位与风险决策之间的潜在因果关系。总之,我们的研究结果表明,环境线索(收益与损失)和早期生活环境(社会经济地位)在塑造儿童的风险偏好方面存在相互作用。研究亮点:这项研究首次考察了学校背景和通过实验操纵的社会经济地位如何影响学龄前儿童在收益和损失框架下的风险偏好。儿童对损失的风险偏好高于对收益的风险偏好;这种框架效应在社会经济地位较高的儿童中比在社会经济地位较低的儿童中更为明显。社会经济地位较低的儿童则表现出较少的风险偏好行为,对损失选项预期值的敏感度也有所下降,但对收益选项的敏感度却没有下降。社会经济地位的暂时提高会增加儿童寻求风险的行为,但不会增加他们对预期价值的敏感性。
{"title":"To risk or not: The impact of socioeconomic status on preschoolers’ risky decision-making for gains and losses","authors":"Delhii Hoid,&nbsp;Ziyan Guo,&nbsp;Zhibin He,&nbsp;Junhui Wu,&nbsp;Zhen Wu","doi":"10.1111/desc.13485","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13485","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Disparities in socioeconomic status (SES) may affect individuals’ risk preferences, which have important developmental consequences across the lifespan. Yet, previous research has shown inconsistent associations between SES and risky decision-making, and little is known about how this link develops from a young age. The current research is among the first to examine how SES influences preschoolers’ risky decisions in both gain and loss frames. Across two studies, children aged 5 to 6 years (total <i>N</i> = 309, 154 boys) were asked to choose between certain and risky options. The risky option was more advantageous, equal to, or less advantageous than the certain option. Study 1 revealed that in the loss frame, high-SES children (<i>n</i> = 84, 44 boys) chose more risky options and were more sensitive to the expected value compared to low-SES children (<i>n</i> = 78, 42 boys), especially when the risk was more advantageous. However, this SES difference was not significant in the gain frame. Supporting the potential causal link between SES and risky decision-making, Study 2 further found that experimentally increasing low-SES children's (<i>n</i> = 68, 30 boys) status by providing additional resources increased their risk-seeking behavior in the loss frame. Overall, our findings suggest an interaction between environmental cues (gain vs. loss) and early life circumstances (SES) in shaping children's risk preferences.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>This research is among the first to examine how school backgrounds and experimentally manipulated SES influence preschoolers’ risk preferences in gain and loss frames.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Children were more risk-seeking for losses than for gains; this framing effect was stronger in higher-SES than lower-SES children.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Lower-SES children exhibited fewer risk-seeking behaviors and decreased sensitivity to the expected value of options for losses, but not for gains.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>A temporary boost in SES increased children's risk-seeking behavior, but not sensitivity to expected values.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-13","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139730773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The development of visual cognition: The emergence of spatial congruency bias 视觉认知的发展:空间一致性偏差的出现
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-08 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13482
Mengcun Gao, Maurryce D. Starks, Julie D. Golomb, Vladimir M. Sloutsky

In adults, spatial location plays a special role in visual object processing. People are more likely to judge two sequentially presented objects as being identical when they appear in the same location compared to in different locations (a phenomenon referred to as Spatial Congruency Bias [SCB]). However, no comparable Identity Congruency Bias (ICB) is found, suggesting an asymmetric location-identity relationship in object binding. What gives rise to this asymmetric congruency bias? This paper considered two possible hypotheses. Hypothesis 1 suggests that the asymmetric congruency bias results from an inherently special role of location in the visual system. In contrast, Hypothesis 2 suggests that the asymmetric congruency bias is a product of development, reflecting people's experience with the world. To distinguish the two hypotheses, we tested both adults’ and 5-year-old children's SCB and ICB by Identity Judgment Experiments and Spatial Judgment Experiments, respectively. The study found that adults only exhibited a SCB, but no ICB. However, young children exhibited both SCB and ICB, suggesting a symmetric congruency bias and reciprocal influences between location and identity in early development. The results indicate that the asymmetric location-identity relationship develops as object identity's influence on location gets pruned away, while location's influence on identity is preserved, possibly due to people's gained experiences with regularities of the world.

Research Highlights

  • Adults exhibit Spatial Congruency Bias—an asymmetric location-identity relationship with location biasing their judgment of object identities, but not vice versa.

  • Asymmetric congruency bias may result from an inherently special role of location in visual system (Hypothesis 1) or accumulated experiences with the world (Hypothesis 2).

  • To distinguish the two hypotheses, the study investigated the Spatial Congruency Bias and Identity Congruency Bias in both adults and 5-year-old children.

  • Unlike adults who exhibited only Spatial Congruency Bias, 5-year-old children exhibited both Spatial Congruency Bias and Identity Congruency Bias.

在成年人的视觉对象处理过程中,空间位置起着特殊的作用。当两个依次出现的物体出现在相同位置时,人们更容易判断它们是相同的,而出现在不同位置时则不同(这种现象被称为空间一致性偏差(Spatial Congruency Bias [SCB]))。然而,却没有发现类似的身份一致性偏差(ICB),这表明在物体绑定中存在着非对称的位置-身份关系。是什么导致了这种不对称的一致性偏差?本文考虑了两种可能的假设。假设 1 认为,非对称一致性偏差是由于位置在视觉系统中固有的特殊作用造成的。相反,假设 2 则认为非对称一致性偏差是发展的产物,反映了人们对世界的体验。为了区分这两个假设,我们通过身份判断实验和空间判断实验分别测试了成人和 5 岁儿童的 SCB 和 ICB。研究发现,成人只表现出 SCB,而没有 ICB。然而,幼儿同时表现出 SCB 和 ICB,这表明在早期发育过程中,位置和身份之间存在对称一致性偏差和相互影响。研究结果表明,随着客体身份对位置的影响逐渐减弱,而位置对身份的影响得以保留,位置与身份之间的非对称关系便逐渐形成,这可能是由于人们获得了世界规律性的经验。研究亮点成人表现出空间一致性偏差--一种非对称的位置-身份关系,位置会使他们对物体身份的判断产生偏差,反之亦然。不对称的一致性偏差可能源于位置在视觉系统中的固有特殊作用(假设 1),也可能源于积累的世界经验(假设 2)。为了区分这两个假设,研究调查了成人和 5 岁儿童的空间一致性偏差和身份一致性偏差。与只表现出空间一致性偏差的成人不同,5 岁儿童同时表现出空间一致性偏差和身份一致性偏差。
{"title":"The development of visual cognition: The emergence of spatial congruency bias","authors":"Mengcun Gao,&nbsp;Maurryce D. Starks,&nbsp;Julie D. Golomb,&nbsp;Vladimir M. Sloutsky","doi":"10.1111/desc.13482","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13482","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>In adults, spatial location plays a special role in visual object processing. People are more likely to judge two sequentially presented objects as being identical when they appear in the same location compared to in different locations (a phenomenon referred to as Spatial Congruency Bias [SCB]). However, no comparable Identity Congruency Bias (ICB) is found, suggesting an asymmetric location-identity relationship in object binding. What gives rise to this asymmetric congruency bias? This paper considered two possible hypotheses. Hypothesis 1 suggests that the asymmetric congruency bias results from an inherently special role of location in the visual system. In contrast, Hypothesis 2 suggests that the asymmetric congruency bias is a product of development, reflecting people's experience with the world. To distinguish the two hypotheses, we tested both adults’ and 5-year-old children's SCB and ICB by Identity Judgment Experiments and Spatial Judgment Experiments, respectively. The study found that adults only exhibited a SCB, but no ICB. However, young children exhibited both SCB and ICB, suggesting a symmetric congruency bias and reciprocal influences between location and identity in early development. The results indicate that the asymmetric location-identity relationship develops as object identity's influence on location gets pruned away, while location's influence on identity is preserved, possibly due to people's gained experiences with regularities of the world.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Adults exhibit Spatial Congruency Bias—an asymmetric location-identity relationship with location biasing their judgment of object identities, but not vice versa.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Asymmetric congruency bias may result from an inherently special role of location in visual system (Hypothesis 1) or accumulated experiences with the world (Hypothesis 2).</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>To distinguish the two hypotheses, the study investigated the Spatial Congruency Bias and Identity Congruency Bias in both adults and 5-year-old children.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Unlike adults who exhibited only Spatial Congruency Bias, 5-year-old children exhibited both Spatial Congruency Bias and Identity Congruency Bias.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-08","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13482","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139708229","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Toddlers do not preferentially transmit generalizable information to others 幼儿不会优先向他人传递可概括的信息。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13479
Didar Karadağ, Marina Bazhydai, Gert Westermann

Children actively and selectively transmit information to others based on the type of information and the context during learning. Four- to 7-year-old children preferentially transmit generalizable information in teaching-like contexts. Although 2-year-old children are able to distinguish between generalizable and non-generalizable information, it is not known whether they likewise transmit generalizable information selectively. We designed a behavioral study to address this question. Two-year-old children were presented with three novel boxes, identical except for their color. In each box, one of two equally salient actions led to a generalizable outcome (e.g., playing a [different] tune in each box), whereas the other led to a non-generalizable outcome (e.g., turning on a light, vibrating the box, or making a noise). In the discovery phase, children had a chance to discover the functions of each box presented one-by-one. Then, in the exploration phase, they were given the opportunity to independently explore all three boxes presented together. Finally, in the transmission phase, an ignorant recipient entered the room and asked the child to show them how these toys work. We measured whether children preferentially transmitted either generalizable or non-generalizable information when they were asked to demonstrate the function of the toys to a naïve adult. We found that children did not display any preference for transmitting generalizable information. These findings are discussed with respect to toddlers’ selectivity in transmitting information but also the development of sensitivity to information generalizability.

Research Highlight

  • Young children transmit information to others and do so with some degree of selectivity to a variety of factors.
  • Generalizability is an important factor affecting information transmission, and older children tend to associate generalizable information with teaching-like interactions.
  • We tested whether toddlers selectively transmitted it to others over non-generalizable information.
  • We found that toddlers do not show a preference to transmit generalizable over non-generalizable information.
在学习过程中,儿童会根据信息的类型和情境主动地、有选择性地向他人传递信息。在类似教学的情境中,4 到 7 岁的儿童更倾向于传递可概括的信息。虽然两岁的儿童能够区分可概括的信息和不可概括的信息,但他们是否同样有选择地传递可概括的信息却不得而知。为了解决这个问题,我们设计了一项行为研究。我们向两岁儿童展示了三个除颜色外完全相同的新奇盒子。在每个盒子中,两个同样突出的动作中的一个会导致可概括的结果(例如,在每个盒子中播放[不同的]曲调),而另一个会导致不可概括的结果(例如,打开灯、振动盒子或发出声音)。在发现阶段,儿童有机会逐一发现每个盒子的功能。然后,在探索阶段,他们有机会独立探索一起展示的所有三个盒子。最后,在传递阶段,一名无知的接受者进入房间,要求儿童向他们展示这些玩具是如何工作的。我们测量了当儿童被要求向天真无邪的成人演示玩具的功能时,他们是否会优先传递可概括的或不可概括的信息。我们发现,儿童并没有表现出偏好传递可概括的信息。我们将就幼儿在传递信息时的选择性以及对信息可概括性的敏感性的发展对这些发现进行讨论。研究亮点:幼儿向他人传递信息时对各种因素有一定程度的选择性。可概括性是影响信息传递的一个重要因素,年龄较大的儿童倾向于将可概括的信息与类似教学的互动联系起来。我们测试了幼儿是否会选择性地向他人传递可概括的信息,而不是不可概括的信息。我们发现,学步儿童并没有表现出传递可概括信息而非不可概括信息的偏好。
{"title":"Toddlers do not preferentially transmit generalizable information to others","authors":"Didar Karadağ,&nbsp;Marina Bazhydai,&nbsp;Gert Westermann","doi":"10.1111/desc.13479","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13479","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Children actively and selectively transmit information to others based on the type of information and the context during learning. Four- to 7-year-old children preferentially transmit generalizable information in teaching-like contexts. Although 2-year-old children are able to distinguish between generalizable and non-generalizable information, it is not known whether they likewise transmit generalizable information selectively. We designed a behavioral study to address this question. Two-year-old children were presented with three novel boxes, identical except for their color. In each box, one of two equally salient actions led to a generalizable outcome (e.g., playing a [different] tune in each box), whereas the other led to a non-generalizable outcome (e.g., turning on a light, vibrating the box, or making a noise). In the discovery phase, children had a chance to discover the functions of each box presented one-by-one. Then, in the exploration phase, they were given the opportunity to independently explore all three boxes presented together. Finally, in the transmission phase, an ignorant recipient entered the room and asked the child to show them how these toys work. We measured whether children preferentially transmitted either generalizable or non-generalizable information when they were asked to demonstrate the function of the toys to a naïve adult. We found that children did not display any preference for transmitting generalizable information. These findings are discussed with respect to toddlers’ selectivity in transmitting information but also the development of sensitivity to information generalizability.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlight</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Young children transmit information to others and do so with some degree of selectivity to a variety of factors.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Generalizability is an important factor affecting information transmission, and older children tend to associate generalizable information with teaching-like interactions.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We tested whether toddlers selectively transmitted it to others over non-generalizable information.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We found that toddlers do not show a preference to transmit generalizable over non-generalizable information.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13479","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139703735","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exposure to sign language prior and after cochlear implantation increases language and cognitive skills in deaf children 耳蜗植入前后接触手语可提高聋儿的语言和认知能力。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-07 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13481
A. Delcenserie, F. Genesee, F. Champoux

Recent evidence suggests that deaf children with CIs exposed to nonnative sign language from hearing parents can attain age-appropriate vocabularies in both sign and spoken language. It remains to be explored whether deaf children with CIs who are exposed to early nonnative sign language, but only up to implantation, also benefit from this input and whether these benefits also extend to memory abilities, which are strongly linked to language development. The present study examined the impact of deaf children's early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on their spoken language and their phonological memory abilities. Deaf children who had been exposed to nonnative sign input before and after cochlear implantation were compared to deaf children who never had any exposure to sign input as well as to children with typical hearing. The children were between 5;1 and 7;1 years of age at the time of testing and were matched on age, sex, and socioeconomic status. The results suggest that even short-term exposure to nonnative sign input has positive effects on general language and phonological memory abilities as well as on nonverbal working memory—with total length of exposure to sign input being the best predictor of deaf children's performance on these measures. The present data suggest that even access to early short-term nonnative visual language input is beneficial for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with cochlear implants, suggesting also that parents should not be discouraged from learning and exposing their child to sign language.

Research Highlights

  • This is the first study to examine the effects of early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on French-speaking children with cochlear implants’ spoken language and memory abilities.
  • Early short-term nonnative exposure to sign input can have positive consequences for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with CIs.
  • Extended exposure to sign input has some additional and important benefits, allowing children to perform on par with children with typical hearing.
最近的证据表明,植入人工耳蜗的聋儿从听力正常的父母那里接触非母语手语后,手语和口语词汇量都能达到与年龄相适应的水平。至于早期接触非母语手语的 CI 失聪儿童是否也能从这种输入中获益,以及这些获益是否也能扩展到与语言发展密切相关的记忆能力,还有待探讨。本研究探讨了聋儿早期短期接触非母语手语对其口语和语音记忆能力的影响。在人工耳蜗植入前后接触过非母语手语输入的聋儿与从未接触过手语输入的聋儿以及具有典型听力的儿童进行了比较。接受测试时,这些儿童的年龄在 5;1 到 7;1 岁之间,他们的年龄、性别和社会经济状况均匹配。结果表明,即使短期接触非母语手语输入,也会对一般语言和语音记忆能力以及非语言工作记忆产生积极影响--接触手语输入的总时间是预测聋儿在这些方面表现的最佳指标。本研究数据表明,即使是早期的短期非母语视觉语言输入也有益于植入人工耳蜗的聋儿的语言和语音记忆能力,这也表明家长不应该阻止孩子学习和接触手语。研究亮点:这是首次研究早期短期接触非母语手语输入对法语人工耳蜗植入儿童口语和记忆能力的影响。早期短期接触非母语手语输入会对植入人工耳蜗的聋儿的语言和语音记忆能力产生积极影响。长时间接触手语输入还能带来一些额外的重要益处,使儿童的表现与具有典型听力的儿童相当。
{"title":"Exposure to sign language prior and after cochlear implantation increases language and cognitive skills in deaf children","authors":"A. Delcenserie,&nbsp;F. Genesee,&nbsp;F. Champoux","doi":"10.1111/desc.13481","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13481","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Recent evidence suggests that deaf children with CIs exposed to nonnative sign language from hearing parents can attain age-appropriate vocabularies in both sign and spoken language. It remains to be explored whether deaf children with CIs who are exposed to early nonnative sign language, but only up to implantation, also benefit from this input and whether these benefits also extend to memory abilities, which are strongly linked to language development. The present study examined the impact of deaf children's early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on their spoken language and their phonological memory abilities. Deaf children who had been exposed to nonnative sign input before and after cochlear implantation were compared to deaf children who never had any exposure to sign input as well as to children with typical hearing. The children were between 5;1 and 7;1 years of age at the time of testing and were matched on age, sex, and socioeconomic status. The results suggest that even short-term exposure to nonnative sign input has positive effects on general language and phonological memory abilities as well as on nonverbal working memory—with total length of exposure to sign input being the best predictor of deaf children's performance on these measures. The present data suggest that even access to early short-term nonnative visual language input is beneficial for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with cochlear implants, suggesting also that parents should not be discouraged from learning and exposing their child to sign language.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This is the first study to examine the effects of early short-term exposure to nonnative sign input on French-speaking children with cochlear implants’ spoken language and memory abilities.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Early short-term nonnative exposure to sign input can have positive consequences for the language and phonological memory abilities of deaf children with CIs.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Extended exposure to sign input has some additional and important benefits, allowing children to perform on par with children with typical hearing.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139703734","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Direct and indirect effects of mother's spatial ability on child's spatial ability: What role does the home environment play? 母亲的空间能力对孩子空间能力的直接和间接影响:家庭环境起什么作用?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13480
Nelcida L. Garcia-Sanchez, Anthony Steven Dick, Timothy Hayes, Shannon M. Pruden

Individual differences in spatial thinking are predictive of children's math and science achievement and later entry into Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) disciplines. Little is known about whether parent characteristics predict individual differences in children's spatial thinking. This study aims to understand whether, and to what extent, mother's intrinsic (i.e., mental rotation) and extrinsic (i.e., spatial scaling) spatial ability directly and indirectly, via the variation in home spatial environment, predicts children's intrinsic and extrinsic spatial ability. A total of 165 mothers and their 4–6-year-old children were recruited to participate in a remote video session with an experimenter. Mothers were administered a forced-choice Intrinsic Spatial Toy Preference Task gauging their preference for highly spatial versus less spatial toys and asked questions with the Home Intrinsic Spatial Environmental Questionnaire about the frequency with which they engage their child in spatial activities at home. Mothers completed a Mental Rotations Test and a Spatial Scaling Task adapted for adults. Children were administered the Picture Rotation Task, the Spatial Scaling Task, and the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. Structural equation modeling was used to examine direct and indirect, via home spatial environment and toy choices, influences of mother spatial ability on child spatial ability. Contrary to our predictions, we did not find direct, nor indirect, relations between mother and child spatial ability. These findings suggest that researchers should consider alternative conceptualizations of the early home spatial environment beyond the frequency of spatial play in the home.

Research Highlights

  • The identification of factors that predict individual differences in children's spatial ability is important in order to maximize STEM learning outcomes.
  • Data collection was conducted remotely rather than in traditional preschool or laboratory settings.
  • Contrary to our pre-registered hypotheses, no significant relations between mother spatial ability, the early home spatial environment, and children's development of spatial skills were found.
  • Future research should consider examining the amount of spatial language used in the home or the quality of parent–child interactions during spatial play as potential explanations for individual differences in children's spatial ability.
空间思维的个体差异可预测儿童的数学和科学成绩,以及日后进入科学、技术、工程和数学(STEM)学科的情况。对于父母的特征是否能预测儿童空间思维的个体差异,人们知之甚少。本研究旨在了解母亲的内在空间能力(即心智旋转)和外在空间能力(即空间缩放)是否以及在多大程度上通过家庭空间环境的变化直接或间接地预测儿童的内在和外在空间能力。实验共招募了 165 位母亲和她们 4-6 岁的孩子,与实验人员进行远程视频会话。实验人员对母亲们进行了强迫选择内在空间玩具偏好任务,以衡量她们对空间感强与空间感弱玩具的偏好,并通过家庭内在空间环境问卷询问了她们在家中让孩子参与空间活动的频率。母亲们完成了智力旋转测试和为成人改编的空间缩放任务。儿童则接受了图片旋转任务、空间缩放任务和皮博迪图片词汇测试。我们使用结构方程模型来研究母亲的空间能力通过家庭空间环境和玩具选择对儿童空间能力的直接和间接影响。与我们的预测相反,我们没有发现母亲和孩子的空间能力之间存在直接或间接的关系。这些研究结果表明,研究人员应考虑早期家庭空间环境的其他概念,而不仅仅是家庭空间游戏的频率。研究亮点:确定预测儿童空间能力个体差异的因素对于最大限度地提高 STEM 学习成果非常重要。数据收集是在远程而非传统的学前教育或实验室环境中进行的。与我们预先登记的假设相反,在母亲的空间能力、早期家庭空间环境和儿童空间技能发展之间没有发现明显的关系。未来的研究应考虑将家庭中使用的空间语言的数量或空间游戏中亲子互动的质量作为儿童空间能力个体差异的潜在解释因素。
{"title":"Direct and indirect effects of mother's spatial ability on child's spatial ability: What role does the home environment play?","authors":"Nelcida L. Garcia-Sanchez,&nbsp;Anthony Steven Dick,&nbsp;Timothy Hayes,&nbsp;Shannon M. Pruden","doi":"10.1111/desc.13480","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13480","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Individual differences in spatial thinking are predictive of children's math and science achievement and later entry into Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics (STEM) disciplines. Little is known about whether parent characteristics predict individual differences in children's spatial thinking. This study aims to understand whether, and to what extent, mother's intrinsic (i.e., mental rotation) and extrinsic (i.e., spatial scaling) spatial ability directly and indirectly, via the variation in home spatial environment, predicts children's intrinsic and extrinsic spatial ability. A total of 165 mothers and their 4–6-year-old children were recruited to participate in a remote video session with an experimenter. Mothers were administered a forced-choice <i>Intrinsic Spatial Toy Preference Task</i> gauging their preference for highly spatial versus less spatial toys and asked questions with the <i>Home Intrinsic Spatial Environmental Questionnaire</i> about the frequency with which they engage their child in spatial activities at home. Mothers completed a <i>Mental Rotations Test</i> and a <i>Spatial Scaling Task</i> adapted for adults. Children were administered the <i>Picture Rotation Task</i>, the <i>Spatial Scaling Task</i>, and the <i>Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test</i>. Structural equation modeling was used to examine direct and indirect, via home spatial environment and toy choices, influences of mother spatial ability on child spatial ability. Contrary to our predictions, we did not find direct, nor indirect, relations between mother and child spatial ability. These findings suggest that researchers should consider alternative conceptualizations of the early home spatial environment beyond the frequency of spatial play in the home.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The identification of factors that predict individual differences in children's spatial ability is important in order to maximize STEM learning outcomes.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Data collection was conducted remotely rather than in traditional preschool or laboratory settings.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Contrary to our pre-registered hypotheses, no significant relations between mother spatial ability, the early home spatial environment, and children's development of spatial skills were found.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Future research should consider examining the amount of spatial language used in the home or the quality of parent–child interactions during spatial play as potential explanations for individual differences in children's spatial ability.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139698726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Cognitive deficits and enhancements in youth from adverse conditions: An integrative assessment using Drift Diffusion Modeling in the ABCD study 来自不利条件的青少年的认知缺陷和增强:在 ABCD 研究中使用漂移扩散模型进行综合评估。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-02-06 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13478
Stefan Vermeent, Ethan S. Young, Meriah L. DeJoseph, Anna-Lena Schubert, Willem E. Frankenhuis

Childhood adversity can lead to cognitive deficits or enhancements, depending on many factors. Though progress has been made, two challenges prevent us from integrating and better understanding these patterns. First, studies commonly use and interpret raw performance differences, such as response times, which conflate different stages of cognitive processing. Second, most studies either isolate or aggregate abilities, obscuring the degree to which individual differences reflect task-general (shared) or task-specific (unique) processes. We addressed these challenges using Drift Diffusion Modeling (DDM) and structural equation modeling (SEM). Leveraging a large, representative sample of 9–10 year-olds from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, we examined how two forms of adversity—material deprivation and household threat—were associated with performance on tasks measuring processing speed, inhibition, attention shifting, and mental rotation. Using DDM, we decomposed performance on each task into three distinct stages of processing: speed of information uptake, response caution, and stimulus encoding/response execution. Using SEM, we isolated task-general and task-specific variances in each processing stage and estimated their associations with the two forms of adversity. Youth with more exposure to household threat (but not material deprivation) showed slower task-general processing speed, but showed intact task-specific abilities. In addition, youth with more exposure to household threat tended to respond more cautiously in general. These findings suggest that traditional assessments might overestimate the extent to which childhood adversity reduces specific abilities. By combining DDM and SEM approaches, we can develop a more nuanced understanding of how adversity affects different aspects of youth's cognitive performance.

Research Highlight

  • To understand how childhood adversity shapes cognitive abilities, the field needs analytical approaches that can jointly document and explain patterns of lowered and enhanced performance.

  • Using Drift Diffusion Modeling and Structural Equation Modeling, we analyzed associations between adversity and processing speed, inhibition, attention shifting, and mental rotation.

  • Household threat, but not material deprivation, was mostly associated with slower task-general processing speed and more response caution. In contrast, task-specific abilities were largel

童年逆境会导致认知缺陷或认知能力增强,这取决于许多因素。尽管我们已经取得了一些进展,但仍有两个挑战阻碍我们整合并更好地理解这些模式。首先,研究通常使用和解释原始表现差异,如反应时间,这混淆了认知处理的不同阶段。其次,大多数研究要么将能力孤立起来,要么将能力综合起来,从而掩盖了个体差异在多大程度上反映了任务的一般过程(共享)或任务的特定过程(独特)。我们利用漂移扩散模型(DDM)和结构方程模型(SEM)解决了这些难题。我们利用青少年大脑认知发展(ABCD)研究中具有代表性的大量 9-10 岁儿童样本,研究了物质匮乏和家庭威胁这两种形式的逆境与处理速度、抑制、注意力转移和心智旋转等任务的表现之间的关系。我们使用DDM将每项任务的成绩分解为三个不同的处理阶段:信息吸收速度、反应谨慎和刺激编码/反应执行。利用 SEM,我们分离出了每个处理阶段的任务一般方差和任务特定方差,并估计了它们与两种逆境形式的关联。受到家庭威胁(而非物质匮乏)较多的青少年的任务一般处理速度较慢,但其任务特定能力却完好无损。此外,受到家庭威胁较多的青少年在一般情况下往往反应更为谨慎。这些发现表明,传统的评估可能会高估童年逆境对特定能力的削弱程度。通过将DDM和SEM方法相结合,我们可以更细致地了解逆境如何影响青少年认知表现的不同方面。研究亮点:为了了解童年逆境是如何塑造认知能力的,该领域需要能够共同记录和解释能力降低和提高模式的分析方法。利用漂移扩散模型和结构方程模型,我们分析了逆境与处理速度、抑制、注意力转移和心智旋转之间的关联。家庭威胁(而非物质匮乏)主要与较慢的任务一般处理速度和更谨慎的反应有关。相比之下,特定任务的能力则基本保持不变。研究人员可能会高估童年逆境对特定能力的影响,而低估传统测量方法对一般处理速度和反应谨慎的影响。
{"title":"Cognitive deficits and enhancements in youth from adverse conditions: An integrative assessment using Drift Diffusion Modeling in the ABCD study","authors":"Stefan Vermeent,&nbsp;Ethan S. Young,&nbsp;Meriah L. DeJoseph,&nbsp;Anna-Lena Schubert,&nbsp;Willem E. Frankenhuis","doi":"10.1111/desc.13478","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13478","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Childhood adversity can lead to cognitive deficits or enhancements, depending on many factors. Though progress has been made, two challenges prevent us from integrating and better understanding these patterns. First, studies commonly use and interpret raw performance differences, such as response times, which conflate different stages of cognitive processing. Second, most studies either isolate or aggregate abilities, obscuring the degree to which individual differences reflect task-general (shared) or task-specific (unique) processes. We addressed these challenges using Drift Diffusion Modeling (DDM) and structural equation modeling (SEM). Leveraging a large, representative sample of 9–10 year-olds from the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study, we examined how two forms of adversity—material deprivation and household threat—were associated with performance on tasks measuring processing speed, inhibition, attention shifting, and mental rotation. Using DDM, we decomposed performance on each task into three distinct stages of processing: speed of information uptake, response caution, and stimulus encoding/response execution. Using SEM, we isolated task-general and task-specific variances in each processing stage and estimated their associations with the two forms of adversity. Youth with more exposure to household threat (but not material deprivation) showed slower task-general processing speed, but showed intact task-specific abilities. In addition, youth with more exposure to household threat tended to respond more cautiously in general. These findings suggest that traditional assessments might overestimate the extent to which childhood adversity reduces specific abilities. By combining DDM and SEM approaches, we can develop a more nuanced understanding of how adversity affects different aspects of youth's cognitive performance.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlight</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>To understand how childhood adversity shapes cognitive abilities, the field needs analytical approaches that can jointly document and explain patterns of lowered and enhanced performance.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Using Drift Diffusion Modeling and Structural Equation Modeling, we analyzed associations between adversity and processing speed, inhibition, attention shifting, and mental rotation.</p>\u0000 </li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>\u0000 <p>Household threat, but not material deprivation, was mostly associated with slower task-general processing speed and more response caution. In contrast, task-specific abilities were largel","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13478","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139698725","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
How pervasive is joint attention? Mother-child dyads from a Wichi community reveal a different form of “togetherness” 共同关注有多普遍?来自威奇社区的母子二人组揭示了一种不同形式的 "团结"。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-01-28 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13471
Andrea Taverna, Migdalia Padilla, Sandra Waxman

Theories of early development have emphasized the power of caregivers as active agents in infant socialization and learning. However, there is variability, across communities, in the tendency of caregivers to engage with their infants directly. This raises the possibility that infants and children in some communities spend more time engaged in solitary activities than in dyadic or triadic interactions. Here, we focus on one such community (indigenous Wichi living in Argentina's Chaco Forest) to test this possibility. We examine naturally occurring attentional activity involving the mother and child among the Wichi and among Eurodescendant Spanish-speaking families living in Argentina. We engaged 16 families—8 Wichi and 8 Eurodescendant—in an observational study of interactions between caregivers and their 1- to 2-year-olds. A mixed-analytic approach revealed no differences between communities in the proportion of time infants spent alone, or in mother-child interaction. What does differ, however, is how mothers engage in these interactions: Wichi mothers spend a greater proportion of their time observing their infants than do Eurodescendant mothers. Moreover, when infants in both groups are alone, they focus their ‘solitary’ activities differently: Wichi infants engaged primarily in observation alone, whereas Eurodescendant infants were more focused on the object. Finally, all mother-child pairs engaged in dyadic and triadic (object-infant-caregiver) patterns of attention, but the triadic patterns differed considerably between cultures: Among Wichi, mothers actively “watched” infants as they engaged with objects, whereas Eurodescendant mothers actively engaged with their infants in joint attentional episodes. This work illustrates how attention and socialization, key mechanisms of early development, are culturally organized. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=SwsOCLXubKQ.

Research Highlights

  • Longitudinal, observational investigation of mother-infant interaction in two distinct Argentine cultural groups (Wichi and Eurodescendants) reveals both commonalities and clear community-based differences in interactions between mothers and their 1- to 2-year-olds.
  • Wichi mother-infant dyads engaged primarily in visual observation of one another, but their Eurodescendant counterparts tended to engage in more verbal or physical interaction.
  • We identify a new form of triadic interaction—lateral joint attention—among the Wichi dyads.
  • This work underscores that attention and socialization, key mechanisms of early development, are
早期发展理论强调,看护者在婴儿社会化和学习过程中发挥着积极的作用。然而,在不同的社区,照料者与婴儿直接接触的倾向存在差异。这就提出了一种可能性,即某些社区的婴儿和儿童有更多的时间从事单独活动,而不是进行二人或三人互动。在此,我们将重点放在一个这样的社区(生活在阿根廷查科森林的原住民维奇人)来验证这种可能性。我们研究了维奇人和生活在阿根廷的讲西班牙语的欧洲后裔家庭中自然发生的母子注意力活动。我们对 16 个家庭进行了观察研究,其中 8 个是维奇人家庭,8 个是欧洲后裔家庭。混合分析法显示,在婴儿独处的时间比例或母子互动方面,不同社区之间没有差异。然而,不同的是母亲参与这些互动的方式:与欧洲后裔母亲相比,维奇母亲花在观察婴儿上的时间更多。此外,当两组婴儿单独相处时,他们 "独处 "活动的重点也不同:日裔婴儿主要单独进行观察,而欧裔婴儿则更多地关注物体。最后,所有的母子都参与了二元和三元(客体-婴儿-照料者)的注意模式,但三元模式在不同文化之间有很大差异:在维奇人中,当婴儿与物体接触时,母亲会主动 "观察 "婴儿,而欧洲后裔的母亲则会主动与婴儿一起参与共同的注意力活动。这项研究说明了早期发展的关键机制--注意力和社会化是如何通过文化组织起来的。研究重点通过对两个不同阿根廷文化群体(维奇人和欧洲后裔)的母婴互动进行纵向观察调查,发现母亲与 1 到 2 岁婴儿之间的互动既有共性,也有明显的社区差异。维奇族母婴二人组主要通过视觉观察彼此,而欧洲裔母婴二人组则倾向于更多的语言或肢体互动。我们发现了一种新的三元互动形式--维奇母婴互动中的侧向联合注意。这项研究强调,注意力和社会化作为早期发展的关键机制,是由文化组织的。
{"title":"How pervasive is joint attention? Mother-child dyads from a Wichi community reveal a different form of “togetherness”","authors":"Andrea Taverna,&nbsp;Migdalia Padilla,&nbsp;Sandra Waxman","doi":"10.1111/desc.13471","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13471","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Theories of early development have emphasized the power of caregivers as active agents in infant socialization and learning. However, there is variability, across communities, in the tendency of caregivers to engage with their infants directly. This raises the possibility that infants and children in some communities spend more time engaged in solitary activities than in dyadic or triadic interactions. Here, we focus on one such community (indigenous Wichi living in Argentina's Chaco Forest) to test this possibility. We examine naturally occurring attentional activity involving the mother and child among the Wichi and among Eurodescendant Spanish-speaking families living in Argentina. We engaged 16 families—8 Wichi and 8 Eurodescendant—in an observational study of interactions between caregivers and their 1- to 2-year-olds. A mixed-analytic approach revealed no differences between communities in the proportion of time infants spent alone, or in mother-child interaction. What does differ, however, is how mothers engage in these interactions: Wichi mothers spend a greater proportion of their time observing their infants than do Eurodescendant mothers. Moreover, when infants in both groups are alone, they focus their ‘solitary’ activities differently: Wichi infants engaged primarily in observation alone, whereas Eurodescendant infants were more focused on the object. Finally, all mother-child pairs engaged in dyadic and triadic (object-infant-caregiver) patterns of attention, but the triadic patterns differed considerably between cultures: Among Wichi, mothers actively “watched” infants as they engaged with objects, whereas Eurodescendant mothers actively engaged with their infants in joint attentional episodes. This work illustrates how attention and socialization, key mechanisms of early development, are culturally organized. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=SwsOCLXubKQ.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Longitudinal, observational investigation of mother-infant interaction in two distinct Argentine cultural groups (Wichi and Eurodescendants) reveals both commonalities and clear community-based differences in interactions between mothers and their 1- to 2-year-olds.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Wichi mother-infant dyads engaged primarily in visual observation of one another, but their Eurodescendant counterparts tended to engage in more verbal or physical interaction.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We identify a new form of triadic interaction—lateral joint attention—among the Wichi dyads.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This work underscores that attention and socialization, key mechanisms of early development, are ","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-01-28","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139571804","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Sustained pacifier use is associated with smaller vocabulary sizes at 1 and 2 years of age: A cross-sectional study 持续使用安抚奶嘴与 1 岁和 2 岁儿童词汇量较小有关:一项横断面研究。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13477
Luis E. Muñoz, Natalia Kartushina, Julien Mayor

Pacifier use during childhood has been hypothesized to interfere with language processing, but, to date, there is limited evidence revealing detrimental effects of prolonged pacifier use on infant vocabulary learning. In the present study, parents of 12- and 24-month-old infants were recruited in Oslo (Norway). The sample included 1187 monolingual full-term born (without visual, auditory, or cognitive impairments) infants: 452 (230 girls; 222 boys) 12-month-olds and 735 (345 girls; 390 boys) 24-month-olds. Parents filled out an online Norwegian Communicative Development Inventory (CDI), which assesses the vocabulary in comprehension and production for 12-month-old infants and in production only for 24-month-old infants. CDI scores were transformed into age- and sex-adjusted percentiles using Norwegian norms. Additionally, parents retrospectively reported their child's daytime pacifier use, in hours, at 2-month intervals, from birth to the assessment date. Maternal education was used to control, in the analyses, for the socio-economic status. We found that greater pacifier use in an infant's lifespan was associated with lower vocabulary size. Pacifier use later in life was more negatively associated with vocabulary size than precocious use, and increased the odds of being a low language scorer. In sum, our study moves beyond the findings of momentary effects of experimentally induced “impairment” in articulators’ movement on speech perception and suggests that, from 12 months of age, constraints on the infant's speech articulators (pacifier use) may be negatively associated with word comprehension and production.

Research Highlight

  • We examined the relationship between pacifier use and vocabulary sizes in production at 24 months of age and comprehension and production at 12 months of age.
  • Lifespan Pacifier Use (LPU) was negatively correlated with vocabulary sizes in comprehension and production among 12-month-old infants and negatively correlated with production for 24-month-olds.
  • Later pacifier use was found to be more negatively correlated with vocabulary size in infants, as compared to more precocious use.
  • The amount of pacifier use in the 2 months prior to a child's second birthday was predictive of a higher prevalence of low vocabulary scores in 24-month-olds.
儿童时期使用安抚奶嘴被认为会干扰语言处理过程,但迄今为止,只有有限的证据表明长期使用安抚奶嘴会对婴儿的词汇学习产生不利影响。本研究在奥斯陆(挪威)招募了 12 个月和 24 个月大婴儿的父母。样本包括 1187 名单语足月产婴儿(无视觉、听觉或认知障碍):12个月大的婴儿有452名(女婴230名;男婴222名),24个月大的婴儿有735名(女婴345名;男婴390名)。家长们在线填写了挪威沟通能力发展量表(CDI),该量表对 12 个月大婴儿的理解和表达词汇量进行评估,对 24 个月大婴儿仅对表达词汇量进行评估。CDI得分根据挪威标准转换成按年龄和性别调整的百分位数。此外,父母还回顾性地报告了孩子从出生到评估日期每两个月使用安抚奶嘴的情况(以小时为单位)。在分析中,母亲的教育程度被用来控制社会经济地位。我们发现,婴儿在一生中使用安抚奶嘴的次数越多,其词汇量就越少。与早熟使用安抚奶嘴相比,晚期使用安抚奶嘴与词汇量的负相关更大,而且会增加语言得分低的几率。总之,我们的研究超越了实验诱导的发音器运动 "障碍 "对言语感知的瞬间影响,表明从婴儿 12 个月大开始,对其言语发音器的限制(使用安抚奶嘴)可能与词汇理解和生成呈负相关。研究亮点:1.我们研究了安抚奶嘴的使用与 24 个月大时的词汇量大小以及 12 个月大时的理解力和造词能力之间的关系。2.在 12 个月大的婴儿中,安抚奶嘴的使用(LPU)与理解和表达词汇量呈负相关,而在 24 个月大的婴儿中,安抚奶嘴的使用与表达词汇量呈负相关。3.与较早使用安抚奶嘴的婴儿相比,较晚使用安抚奶嘴与婴儿词汇量的负相关程度更高。4.婴儿两周岁前两个月使用安抚奶嘴的次数可预测 24 个月大婴儿低词汇量的发生率。
{"title":"Sustained pacifier use is associated with smaller vocabulary sizes at 1 and 2 years of age: A cross-sectional study","authors":"Luis E. Muñoz,&nbsp;Natalia Kartushina,&nbsp;Julien Mayor","doi":"10.1111/desc.13477","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13477","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Pacifier use during childhood has been hypothesized to interfere with language processing, but, to date, there is limited evidence revealing detrimental effects of prolonged pacifier use on infant vocabulary learning. In the present study, parents of 12- and 24-month-old infants were recruited in Oslo (Norway). The sample included 1187 monolingual full-term born (without visual, auditory, or cognitive impairments) infants: 452 (230 girls; 222 boys) 12-month-olds and 735 (345 girls; 390 boys) 24-month-olds. Parents filled out an online Norwegian Communicative Development Inventory (CDI), which assesses the vocabulary in comprehension and production for 12-month-old infants and in production only for 24-month-old infants. CDI scores were transformed into age- and sex-adjusted percentiles using Norwegian norms. Additionally, parents retrospectively reported their child's daytime pacifier use, in hours, at 2-month intervals, from birth to the assessment date. Maternal education was used to control, in the analyses, for the socio-economic status. We found that greater pacifier use in an infant's lifespan was associated with lower vocabulary size. Pacifier use later in life was more negatively associated with vocabulary size than precocious use, and increased the odds of being a low language scorer. In sum, our study moves beyond the findings of momentary effects of experimentally induced “impairment” in articulators’ movement on speech perception and suggests that, from 12 months of age, constraints on the infant's speech articulators (pacifier use) may be negatively associated with word comprehension and production.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlight</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We examined the relationship between pacifier use and vocabulary sizes in production at 24 months of age and comprehension and production at 12 months of age.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Lifespan Pacifier Use (LPU) was negatively correlated with vocabulary sizes in comprehension and production among 12-month-old infants and negatively correlated with production for 24-month-olds.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Later pacifier use was found to be more negatively correlated with vocabulary size in infants, as compared to more precocious use.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The amount of pacifier use in the 2 months prior to a child's second birthday was predictive of a higher prevalence of low vocabulary scores in 24-month-olds.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13477","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139547552","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1