首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Science最新文献

英文 中文
Greater attention to socioeconomic status in developmental research can improve the external validity, generalizability, and replicability of developmental science 在发展研究中更多地关注社会经济地位,可以提高发展科学的外部有效性、普遍性和可复制性。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-25 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13521
Leher Singh, Sarah J. Rajendra

Psychological researchers have been criticized for making broad presumptions about human behavior based on limited sampling. In part, presumptive generalizability is reflected in the limited representation of sociodemographic variation in research reports. In this analysis, we examine time-trends in reporting of a key sociodemographic construct relevant to many aspects of child development—socioeconomic status (SES)—across six mainstream developmental journals (Infancy, Child Development, Developmental Science, Developmental Psychology, Infant and Child Development, and Infant Behavior & Development) between 2016 and 2022. Findings point to limited reporting of SES across developmental journals and across time. Reporting rates varied significantly by region and by topic of development. In terms of specific indicators of SES, there was consistent use of income and caregiver education as SES indicators. The epistemic costs of the lack of integration of socio-economic factors in developmental research are addressed. Pathways to greater integration of SES are proposed.

Research Highlights

  • We analyzed reporting and representation of socioeconomic status in published studies on early child development.
  • A large proportion of published studies did not report any socio-economic information.
  • Suggestions for greater attention to socioeconomic status are proposed.
心理学研究人员因根据有限的抽样对人类行为做出广泛的推断而受到批评。在一定程度上,推定的普遍性反映在研究报告中对社会人口变异的有限表述上。在本分析中,我们考察了 2016 年至 2022 年间六种主流发展期刊(《婴儿学》、《儿童发展》、《发展科学》、《发展心理学》、《婴幼儿发展》和《婴幼儿行为与发展》)对与儿童发展诸多方面相关的关键社会人口结构--社会经济地位(SES)--的报道的时间趋势。研究结果表明,不同发展期刊和不同时期对 SES 的报道都很有限。不同地区和不同发展主题的报告率差异很大。在SES的具体指标方面,收入和照顾者的教育程度被一致用作SES指标。本文探讨了发展研究中缺乏社会经济因素整合所带来的认识代价。提出了进一步整合社会经济因素的途径。研究亮点:我们分析了已发表的儿童早期发展研究中社会经济地位的报告和代表性。很大一部分已发表的研究没有报告任何社会经济信息。我们提出了更多关注社会经济地位的建议。
{"title":"Greater attention to socioeconomic status in developmental research can improve the external validity, generalizability, and replicability of developmental science","authors":"Leher Singh,&nbsp;Sarah J. Rajendra","doi":"10.1111/desc.13521","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13521","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Psychological researchers have been criticized for making broad presumptions about human behavior based on limited sampling. In part, presumptive generalizability is reflected in the limited representation of sociodemographic variation in research reports. In this analysis, we examine time-trends in reporting of a key sociodemographic construct relevant to many aspects of child development—socioeconomic status (SES)—across six mainstream developmental journals (<i>Infancy, Child Development, Developmental Science, Developmental Psychology, Infant and Child Development</i>, and <i>Infant Behavior &amp; Development</i>) between 2016 and 2022. Findings point to limited reporting of SES across developmental journals and across time. Reporting rates varied significantly by region and by topic of development. In terms of specific indicators of SES, there was consistent use of income and caregiver education as SES indicators. The epistemic costs of the lack of integration of socio-economic factors in developmental research are addressed. Pathways to greater integration of SES are proposed.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We analyzed reporting and representation of socioeconomic status in published studies on early child development.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>A large proportion of published studies did not report any socio-economic information.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Suggestions for greater attention to socioeconomic status are proposed.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13521","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140658913","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The relationship between attachment representations and minimal intergroup bias in preschool-aged children. 学龄前儿童的依恋表征与最小群体间偏见之间的关系。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13514
Antonia Misch, Andrea Kramer, Markus Paulus
Attachment theory proposes that young children's experiences with their caregivers has a tremendous influence on how children navigate their social relationships. By the end of early childhood, intergroup contexts play an important role in their social life and children build strong ties to their ingroups. Although both domains relate to the same psychological processes-children's affective ties to others-surprisingly very little research has addressed how children's attachment relates to their intergroup attitudes and behavior. In this study, we investigate the link. For that purpose, 5-year-old children (n = 100) first underwent the German Attachment Story Completion Task (GEV-B). Then we allocated children into minimal groups based on T-Shirt color and assessed their intergroup attitudes and intergroup behavior. Results showed that while most children showed a strong and robust ingroup bias in their attitudes and behavior, children with an insecure-ambivalent attachment representation treated in- and outgroup similarly. Overall, this study provides novel perspectives on the relationship between children's attachment representation and their interactions in the social world. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: This study provides novel insights into the relationship between children's attachment security and the development of intergroup bias in a minimal group context Children with secure, insecure-avoidant and disorganized attachment representation showed a strong intergroup bias in explicit attitudes and behavior Children with insecure-ambivalent attachment representation displayed no intergroup bias Insecure-ambivalent attachment representations might be detrimental to the formation of ingroup attachment.
依恋理论认为,幼儿与照顾者相处的经历对他们如何处理社会关系有着巨大的影响。到幼儿期结束时,群体间的环境在他们的社会生活中扮演了重要角色,儿童与他们的内群体建立了牢固的联系。虽然这两个领域都与儿童与他人的情感联系这一心理过程有关,但令人惊讶的是,很少有研究涉及儿童的依恋与他们的群体间态度和行为之间的关系。在本研究中,我们对这种联系进行了调查。为此,5 岁儿童(n = 100)首先接受了德国依恋故事完成任务(GEV-B)。然后,我们根据 T-Shirt 的颜色将儿童分成最小组,并对他们的群体间态度和群体间行为进行评估。结果表明,虽然大多数儿童在态度和行为上表现出强烈的内群体偏向,但具有不安全-矛盾依恋表征的儿童对待内群体和外群体的态度是相似的。总之,本研究为儿童的依恋表征与他们在社会世界中的互动关系提供了新的视角。研究亮点:本研究对儿童的依恋安全感与最小群体背景下群体间偏见的发展之间的关系提供了新的见解 安全型、不安全型-回避型和无组织型依恋表征的儿童在明确的态度和行为中表现出强烈的群体间偏见 不安全型-矛盾型依恋表征的儿童没有表现出群体间偏见 不安全型-矛盾型依恋表征可能不利于内群体依恋的形成。
{"title":"The relationship between attachment representations and minimal intergroup bias in preschool-aged children.","authors":"Antonia Misch, Andrea Kramer, Markus Paulus","doi":"10.1111/desc.13514","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13514","url":null,"abstract":"Attachment theory proposes that young children's experiences with their caregivers has a tremendous influence on how children navigate their social relationships. By the end of early childhood, intergroup contexts play an important role in their social life and children build strong ties to their ingroups. Although both domains relate to the same psychological processes-children's affective ties to others-surprisingly very little research has addressed how children's attachment relates to their intergroup attitudes and behavior. In this study, we investigate the link. For that purpose, 5-year-old children (n = 100) first underwent the German Attachment Story Completion Task (GEV-B). Then we allocated children into minimal groups based on T-Shirt color and assessed their intergroup attitudes and intergroup behavior. Results showed that while most children showed a strong and robust ingroup bias in their attitudes and behavior, children with an insecure-ambivalent attachment representation treated in- and outgroup similarly. Overall, this study provides novel perspectives on the relationship between children's attachment representation and their interactions in the social world. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: This study provides novel insights into the relationship between children's attachment security and the development of intergroup bias in a minimal group context Children with secure, insecure-avoidant and disorganized attachment representation showed a strong intergroup bias in explicit attitudes and behavior Children with insecure-ambivalent attachment representation displayed no intergroup bias Insecure-ambivalent attachment representations might be detrimental to the formation of ingroup attachment.","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140668838","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Linking caregiving quality during infancy to brain activity in early childhood and later executive function. 将婴儿期的照料质量与幼儿期的大脑活动和日后的执行功能联系起来。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-23 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13517
Mark Wade, Victoria Parker, Alva Tang, Nathan A Fox, C. Zeanah, Charles A. Nelson
There is no relationship more vital than the one a child shares with their primary caregivers early in development. Yet many children worldwide are raised in settings that lack the warmth, connection, and stimulation provided by a responsive primary caregiver. In this study, we used data from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project (BEIP), a longitudinal study of institutionally-reared and family-reared children, to test how caregiving quality during infancy is associated with average EEG power over the first 3.5 years of life in alpha, beta, and theta frequency bands, and associations with later executive function (EF) at age 8 years. The sample comprised 189 children (129 institutionally-reared; 60 family-reared) who contributed data on observed caregiving quality during infancy (baseline; average age of 22 months), resting EEG power at baseline, 30, and 42 months, and performance-based data on a series of EF tasks at 8 years. Using Bayesian estimation, observed caregiving quality at baseline was marginally linked with higher average alpha and beta power, and lower theta power, from baseline to 42 months. In turn, higher average beta power and lower average theta power were marginally associated with higher EF at 8 years. In indirect effects models, higher caregiving quality at baseline was associated with higher EF at 8 years, with a marginal indirect effect through average theta power from baseline to 42 months. Variation in the quality of the early caregiving environment may be associated with later executive function, which is partially underpinned by individual differences in brain activity during early childhood. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Examined associations between caregiving quality during infancy, brain activity during early childhood, and executive function during mid-childhood in sample of never-institutionalized and institutionally-reared children. Significant associations between higher quality caregiving during infancy and higher executive function during middle childhood. Marginal associations between caregiving quality during infancy and brain activity during early childhood. Marginal associations between brain activity during early childhood and executive function during mid-childhood.
没有什么关系比儿童在成长早期与主要照顾者之间的关系更重要了。然而,世界上许多儿童都是在缺乏温暖、联系和刺激的环境中长大的。在这项研究中,我们利用布加勒斯特早期干预项目(Bucharest Early Intervention Project,BEIP)的数据(这是一项针对机构抚养儿童和家庭抚养儿童的纵向研究),测试了婴儿期的照料质量与出生后前 3.5 年阿尔法、贝塔和θ频段的平均脑电图功率之间的关系,以及与 8 岁时的执行功能(EF)之间的关系。样本包括 189 名儿童(129 名由机构抚养;60 名由家庭抚养),他们提供了婴儿期(基线;平均年龄为 22 个月)观察到的照顾质量数据、基线、30 个月和 42 个月时的静息脑电图功率以及 8 岁时一系列执行功能任务的基于表现的数据。通过贝叶斯估计法,观察到的基线照顾质量与基线至 42 个月期间较高的α和β平均功率以及较低的θ功率略有关联。反过来,较高的β平均功率和较低的θ平均功率也与8年后较高的EF略有关联。在间接效应模型中,基线时较高的护理质量与 8 岁时较高的 EF 值相关,而从基线到 42 个月期间的平均 Theta 功率则具有边际间接效应。早期照料环境质量的差异可能与日后的执行功能有关,而幼儿期大脑活动的个体差异是其部分基础。研究亮点以从未在机构生活和在机构生活的儿童为样本,研究婴儿期照料质量、幼儿期大脑活动和幼儿中期执行功能之间的关系。婴儿期护理质量越高,童年中期的执行功能越强,两者之间存在明显联系。婴儿期护理质量与幼儿期大脑活动之间存在边际关联。幼儿期的大脑活动与儿童中期的执行功能之间存在边际关联。
{"title":"Linking caregiving quality during infancy to brain activity in early childhood and later executive function.","authors":"Mark Wade, Victoria Parker, Alva Tang, Nathan A Fox, C. Zeanah, Charles A. Nelson","doi":"10.1111/desc.13517","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13517","url":null,"abstract":"There is no relationship more vital than the one a child shares with their primary caregivers early in development. Yet many children worldwide are raised in settings that lack the warmth, connection, and stimulation provided by a responsive primary caregiver. In this study, we used data from the Bucharest Early Intervention Project (BEIP), a longitudinal study of institutionally-reared and family-reared children, to test how caregiving quality during infancy is associated with average EEG power over the first 3.5 years of life in alpha, beta, and theta frequency bands, and associations with later executive function (EF) at age 8 years. The sample comprised 189 children (129 institutionally-reared; 60 family-reared) who contributed data on observed caregiving quality during infancy (baseline; average age of 22 months), resting EEG power at baseline, 30, and 42 months, and performance-based data on a series of EF tasks at 8 years. Using Bayesian estimation, observed caregiving quality at baseline was marginally linked with higher average alpha and beta power, and lower theta power, from baseline to 42 months. In turn, higher average beta power and lower average theta power were marginally associated with higher EF at 8 years. In indirect effects models, higher caregiving quality at baseline was associated with higher EF at 8 years, with a marginal indirect effect through average theta power from baseline to 42 months. Variation in the quality of the early caregiving environment may be associated with later executive function, which is partially underpinned by individual differences in brain activity during early childhood. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Examined associations between caregiving quality during infancy, brain activity during early childhood, and executive function during mid-childhood in sample of never-institutionalized and institutionally-reared children. Significant associations between higher quality caregiving during infancy and higher executive function during middle childhood. Marginal associations between caregiving quality during infancy and brain activity during early childhood. Marginal associations between brain activity during early childhood and executive function during mid-childhood.","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-23","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140668544","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Is vision necessary for the timely acquisition of language-specific patterns in co-speech gesture and their lack in silent gesture? 视觉对于及时掌握共语手势中的语言特定模式以及无声手势中的语言特定模式是否必要?
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-17 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13507
Şeyda Özçalışkan, Ché Lucero, Susan Goldin-Meadow

Blind adults display language-specificity in their packaging and ordering of events in speech. These differences affect the representation of events in co-speech gesture–gesturing with speech–but not in silent gesture–gesturing without speech. Here we examine when in development blind children begin to show adult-like patterns in co-speech and silent gesture. We studied speech and gestures produced by 30 blind and 30 sighted children learning Turkish, equally divided into 3 age groups: 5–6, 7–8, 9–10 years. The children were asked to describe three-dimensional spatial event scenes (e.g., running out of a house) first with speech, and then without speech using only their hands. We focused on physical motion events, which, in blind adults, elicit cross-linguistic differences in speech and co-speech gesture, but cross-linguistic similarities in silent gesture. Our results showed an effect of language on gesture when it was accompanied by speech (co-speech gesture), but not when it was used without speech (silent gesture) across both blind and sighted learners. The language-specific co-speech gesture pattern for both packaging and ordering semantic elements was present at the earliest ages we tested the blind and sighted children. The silent gesture pattern appeared later for blind children than sighted children for both packaging and ordering. Our findings highlight gesture as a robust and integral aspect of the language acquisition process at the early ages and provide insight into when language does and does not have an effect on gesture, even in blind children who lack visual access to gesture.

Research Highlights

  • Gestures, when produced with speech (i.e., co-speech gesture), follow language-specific patterns in event representation in both blind and sighted children.
  • Gestures, when produced without speech (i.e., silent gesture), do not follow the language-specific patterns in event representation in both blind and sighted children.
  • Language-specific patterns in speech and co-speech gestures are observable at the same time in blind and sighted children.
  • The cross-linguistic similarities in silent gestures begin slightly later in blind children than in sighted children.
成年盲人在言语事件的包装和排序方面表现出语言特异性。这些差异会影响事件在共同言语手势中的表征--有言语的手势--但不会影响无声手势--无言语的手势。在此,我们研究了盲童在发育过程中何时开始在共同言语和无声手势中表现出类似成人的模式。我们研究了 30 名学习土耳其语的盲童和 30 名视力正常儿童的语言和手势,他们被平均分成三个年龄组:5-6 岁、7-8 岁、9-10 岁。我们要求孩子们先用语言描述三维空间事件场景(如跑出房子),然后只用手不说话地描述。我们的研究重点是物理运动事件,这些事件在盲人成人中会引起说话和共同说话手势的跨语言差异,但在无声手势中会引起跨语言相似性。我们的研究结果表明,在盲人和视力正常的学习者中,当手势伴有语言(共语手势)时,语言会对手势产生影响,而当手势不伴有语言(无声手势)时,语言则不会对手势产生影响。在我们对盲童和视力正常儿童进行测试的最早年龄段,包装和排序语义元素的特定语言共语手势模式就已出现。与视力正常的儿童相比,盲童在包装和排序方面的无声手势模式出现得更晚。我们的研究结果表明,手势是儿童早期语言习得过程中不可或缺的重要环节,并有助于我们了解语言何时会对手势产生影响,何时不会对手势产生影响,即使是对缺乏手势视觉接触的盲童来说也是如此。在不说话的情况下做出的手势(即无声手势),在盲童和健视儿童的事件表征中都不遵循语言的特定模式。在盲童和健视儿童身上,可以同时观察到语言特定模式的言语和共同言语手势。盲童无声手势的跨语言相似性开始的时间略晚于健视儿童。
{"title":"Is vision necessary for the timely acquisition of language-specific patterns in co-speech gesture and their lack in silent gesture?","authors":"Şeyda Özçalışkan,&nbsp;Ché Lucero,&nbsp;Susan Goldin-Meadow","doi":"10.1111/desc.13507","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13507","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Blind adults display language-specificity in their packaging and ordering of events in speech. These differences affect the representation of events in <i>co-speech gesture</i>–gesturing with speech–but not in <i>silent gesture–</i>gesturing without speech. Here we examine when in development blind children begin to show adult-like patterns in co-speech and silent gesture. We studied speech and gestures produced by 30 blind and 30 sighted children learning Turkish, equally divided into 3 age groups: 5–6, 7–8, 9–10 years. The children were asked to describe three-dimensional spatial event scenes (e.g., running out of a house) first with speech, and then without speech using only their hands. We focused on physical motion events, which, in blind adults, elicit cross-linguistic differences in speech and co-speech gesture, but cross-linguistic similarities in silent gesture. Our results showed an effect of language on gesture when it was accompanied by speech (co-speech gesture), but <i>not</i> when it was used without speech (silent gesture) across both blind and sighted learners. The language-specific co-speech gesture pattern for both packaging and ordering semantic elements was present at the earliest ages we tested the blind and sighted children. The silent gesture pattern appeared later for blind children than sighted children for both packaging and ordering. Our findings highlight gesture as a robust and integral aspect of the language acquisition process at the early ages and provide insight into when language does and does <i>not</i> have an effect on gesture, even in blind children who lack visual access to gesture.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Gestures, when produced with speech (i.e., co-speech gesture), follow language-specific patterns in event representation in both blind and sighted children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Gestures, when produced without speech (i.e., silent gesture), do not follow the language-specific patterns in event representation in both blind and sighted children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Language-specific patterns in speech and co-speech gestures are observable at the same time in blind and sighted children.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The cross-linguistic similarities in silent gestures begin slightly later in blind children than in sighted children.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140617847","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reciprocal self-disclosure makes children feel more loved by their parents in the moment: A proof-of-concept experiment 互惠式自我披露能让孩子在当下感受到父母更多的爱:概念验证实验
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-16 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13516
Eddie Brummelman, Peter A. Bos, Eva de Boer, Barbara Nevicka, Constantine Sedikides
Feeling loved by one's parents is critical for children's health and well-being. How can such feelings be fostered? A vital feature of loving interactions is reciprocal self-disclosure, where individuals disclose intimate information about themselves. In a proof-of-concept experiment, we examined whether encouraging reciprocal self-disclosure in parent-child dyads would make children feel more loved during the conversation. Participants were 218 children (ages 8–13, 50% girls, 94% Dutch) and one of their parents (ages 28–56, 62% women, 90% Dutch). Parent-child dyads received a list of 14 questions and took turns asking them each other for 9 min. Dyads were assigned randomly to engage in self-disclosure (questions invoking escalated intimacy) or small talk (questions invoking minimal intimacy). Before and after, children reported how loved they felt by their parent during the conversation. Self-disclosure made children feel more loved during the conversation than did small talk. Compared to small talk, self-disclosure did not instigate conversations that were lengthier or more positive; rather, it instigated conversations that were more emotionally charged (reflecting anger, anxiety, and sadness), social (discussing family and friends), reflective (creating insight), and meaningful (addressing deeply personal topics, including the passing of loved ones). The dyad's gender composition did not significantly moderate these effects. Our research suggests that reciprocal self-disclosure can make children feel more loved in the moment, uncovers linguistic signatures of reciprocal self-disclosure, and offers developmental scientists a tool to examine causal effects of reciprocal self-disclosure in parent-child dyads. Future work should examine long-term effects in everyday parent-child interactions.
感受到父母的爱对儿童的健康和幸福至关重要。如何培养这种感觉呢?爱的互动的一个重要特征是互惠的自我披露,即个人披露自己的亲密信息。在一项概念验证实验中,我们研究了在亲子关系中鼓励互惠的自我披露是否会让孩子在对话中感受到更多的爱。参与者包括 218 名儿童(8-13 岁,50% 为女孩,94% 为荷兰人)和他们的父母之一(28-56 岁,62% 为女性,90% 为荷兰人)。亲子二人组收到一份包含 14 个问题的清单,并轮流向对方提出这些问题,时间为 9 分钟。亲子二人组被随机分配到进行自我披露(引发亲密关系升级的问题)或闲聊(引发最小亲密关系的问题)。在谈话前后,孩子们报告了他们在谈话中感受到的父母对他们的爱。与闲聊相比,自我披露让孩子们在谈话中感受到更多的爱。与闲聊相比,自我披露并没有促使谈话更长或更积极;相反,它促使谈话更有感情色彩(反映出愤怒、焦虑和悲伤)、社交性(讨论家人和朋友)、反思性(产生洞察力)和意义性(涉及深层次的个人话题,包括亲人的去世)。二人组的性别构成对这些影响没有明显的调节作用。我们的研究表明,互惠式自我披露能让儿童在当下感受到更多的爱,揭示了互惠式自我披露的语言特征,并为发展科学家提供了一种工具,用于研究亲子关系中互惠式自我披露的因果效应。未来的工作应该研究日常亲子互动中的长期影响。
{"title":"Reciprocal self-disclosure makes children feel more loved by their parents in the moment: A proof-of-concept experiment","authors":"Eddie Brummelman, Peter A. Bos, Eva de Boer, Barbara Nevicka, Constantine Sedikides","doi":"10.1111/desc.13516","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13516","url":null,"abstract":"Feeling loved by one's parents is critical for children's health and well-being. How can such feelings be fostered? A vital feature of loving interactions is reciprocal self-disclosure, where individuals disclose intimate information about themselves. In a proof-of-concept experiment, we examined whether encouraging reciprocal self-disclosure in parent-child dyads would make children feel more loved during the conversation. Participants were 218 children (ages 8–13, 50% girls, 94% Dutch) and one of their parents (ages 28–56, 62% women, 90% Dutch). Parent-child dyads received a list of 14 questions and took turns asking them each other for 9 min. Dyads were assigned randomly to engage in self-disclosure (questions invoking escalated intimacy) or small talk (questions invoking minimal intimacy). Before and after, children reported how loved they felt by their parent during the conversation. Self-disclosure made children feel more loved during the conversation than did small talk. Compared to small talk, self-disclosure did not instigate conversations that were lengthier or more positive; rather, it instigated conversations that were more emotionally charged (reflecting anger, anxiety, and sadness), social (discussing family and friends), reflective (creating insight), and meaningful (addressing deeply personal topics, including the passing of loved ones). The dyad's gender composition did not significantly moderate these effects. Our research suggests that reciprocal self-disclosure can make children feel more loved in the moment, uncovers linguistic signatures of reciprocal self-disclosure, and offers developmental scientists a tool to examine causal effects of reciprocal self-disclosure in parent-child dyads. Future work should examine long-term effects in everyday parent-child interactions.","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574430","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Feeling loved in mixed‐gender adolescent romantic relationships in the face of daily stress and support: A dyadic diary study 面对日常压力和支持,在男女混合的青少年恋爱关系中感受爱:二元日记研究
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13511
Thao Ha, Masumi Iida, Selena I. Quiroz, Olivia Maras, Andrea Savord
A profound developmental experience is the emergence of adolescent romantic relationships and first feelings of love. However, the daily nature of feeling loved in adolescents’ everyday lives is poorly understood. We investigated how daily stress severity was associated with adolescents’ levels of feeling loved and whether romantic partner support moderated these associations. Furthermore, we investigated this for non‐interpersonal and interpersonal stressors. N = 97 mixed‐gender adolescent romantic couples (age M = 16.38, SD = 1.02) from an ethnically diverse sample (42.2% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 44.7% White) participated in dyadic diary assessments twice a week for 12 consecutive weeks. Both partners independently completed open‐ended questions about a daily stressor, indicating stress severity, perceived partner support, and feeling loved. Daily stressors were coded for non‐interpersonal and various types of interpersonal stressors. Results from the dyadic multilevel model showed that days with lower than average support from a romantic partner amplify the adverse effects of stress severity on feeling loved, especially when the partner is involved in the stressor. We discuss the spillover of stress in romantic relationships and the stress‐buffering functions of adolescent romantic partner support. Adolescent romantic partners are potentially essential regulators of stress, increasing adolescents’ feelings of love.Research Highlights Romantic love is central in adolescents’ lives; we showed that adolescents generally feel loved by romantic partners. Feeling loved fluctuates daily as adolescents feel less loved on stressful days. However, when adolescents perceive their partner as supportive, there is no association between stress and feeling loved. Partner support is protective for feeling loved. The current study provides essential insights into when adolescents and why adolescents feel loved.
青少年恋爱关系的出现和对爱的最初感受是一种深刻的成长经历。然而,人们对青少年在日常生活中感受到爱的日常性质却知之甚少。我们研究了日常压力的严重程度与青少年感受到爱的程度之间的关系,以及恋爱伴侣的支持是否会调节这种关系。此外,我们还针对非人际和人际压力源进行了调查。N = 97 对混合性别的青少年恋爱情侣(年龄中位数 = 16.38,标准差 = 1.02)参加了连续 12 周每周两次的双人日记评估,他们来自不同种族(42.2% 西班牙/拉美/a/x,44.7% 白人)。伴侣双方独立完成有关日常压力源、压力严重程度、伴侣支持感和被爱感觉的开放式问题。日常压力源分为非人际压力源和各种人际压力源。来自伴侣多层次模型的结果表明,来自恋爱伴侣的支持低于平均水平的日子会放大压力严重程度对被爱感觉的不利影响,尤其是当伴侣与压力源有关时。我们讨论了压力在恋爱关系中的溢出效应以及青少年恋爱伙伴支持的压力缓冲功能。研究亮点 浪漫之爱是青少年生活的核心;我们的研究表明,青少年普遍感受到浪漫伴侣的爱。爱的感觉每天都在波动,因为青少年在压力大的日子里感受到的爱更少。然而,当青少年认为其伴侣支持他们时,压力与被爱的感觉之间没有关联。伴侣的支持对被爱的感觉具有保护作用。本研究为了解青少年何时以及为何感受到被爱提供了重要见解。
{"title":"Feeling loved in mixed‐gender adolescent romantic relationships in the face of daily stress and support: A dyadic diary study","authors":"Thao Ha, Masumi Iida, Selena I. Quiroz, Olivia Maras, Andrea Savord","doi":"10.1111/desc.13511","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13511","url":null,"abstract":"<jats:label />A profound developmental experience is the emergence of adolescent romantic relationships and first feelings of love. However, the daily nature of feeling loved in adolescents’ everyday lives is poorly understood. We investigated how daily stress severity was associated with adolescents’ levels of feeling loved and whether romantic partner support moderated these associations. Furthermore, we investigated this for non‐interpersonal and interpersonal stressors. <jats:italic>N</jats:italic> = 97 mixed‐gender adolescent romantic couples (age <jats:italic>M </jats:italic>= 16.38, <jats:italic>SD </jats:italic>= 1.02) from an ethnically diverse sample (42.2% Hispanic/Latino/a/x, 44.7% White) participated in dyadic diary assessments twice a week for 12 consecutive weeks. Both partners independently completed open‐ended questions about a daily stressor, indicating stress severity, perceived partner support, and feeling loved. Daily stressors were coded for non‐interpersonal and various types of interpersonal stressors. Results from the dyadic multilevel model showed that days with lower than average support from a romantic partner amplify the adverse effects of stress severity on feeling loved, especially when the partner is involved in the stressor. We discuss the spillover of stress in romantic relationships and the stress‐buffering functions of adolescent romantic partner support. Adolescent romantic partners are potentially essential regulators of stress, increasing adolescents’ feelings of love.Research Highlights<jats:list list-type=\"bullet\"> <jats:list-item>Romantic love is central in adolescents’ lives; we showed that adolescents generally feel loved by romantic partners.</jats:list-item> <jats:list-item>Feeling loved fluctuates daily as adolescents feel less loved on stressful days.</jats:list-item> <jats:list-item>However, when adolescents perceive their partner as supportive, there is no association between stress and feeling loved. Partner support is protective for feeling loved.</jats:list-item> <jats:list-item>The current study provides essential insights into when adolescents and why adolescents feel loved.</jats:list-item> </jats:list>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574807","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Infant-directed communication: Examining the many dimensions of everyday caregiver-infant interactions 婴儿主导的交流:研究照料者与婴儿日常互动的多个层面
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-15 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13515
Jessica E. Kosie, Casey Lew-Williams

Everyday caregiver-infant interactions are dynamic and multidimensional. However, existing research underestimates the dimensionality of infants’ experiences, often focusing on one or two communicative signals (e.g., speech alone, or speech and gesture together). Here, we introduce “infant-directed communication” (IDC): the suite of communicative signals from caregivers to infants including speech, action, gesture, emotion, and touch. We recorded 10 min of at-home play between 44 caregivers and their 18- to 24-month-old infants from predominantly white, middle-class, English-speaking families in the United States. Interactions were coded for five dimensions of IDC as well as infants’ gestures and vocalizations. Most caregivers used all five dimensions of IDC throughout the interaction, and these dimensions frequently overlapped. For example, over 60% of the speech that infants heard was accompanied by one or more non-verbal communicative cues. However, we saw marked variation across caregivers in their use of IDC, likely reflecting tailored communication to the behaviors and abilities of their infant. Moreover, caregivers systematically increased the dimensionality of IDC, using more overlapping cues in response to infant gestures and vocalizations, and more IDC with infants who had smaller vocabularies. Understanding how and when caregivers use all five signals—together and separately—in interactions with infants has the potential to redefine how developmental scientists conceive of infants’ communicative environments, and enhance our understanding of the relations between caregiver input and early learning.

Research Highlights

  • Infants’ everyday interactions with caregivers are dynamic and multimodal, but existing research has underestimated the multidimensionality (i.e., the diversity of simultaneously occurring communicative cues) inherent in infant-directed communication.
  • Over 60% of the speech that infants encounter during at-home, free play interactions overlap with one or more of a variety of non-speech communicative cues.
  • The multidimensionality of caregivers’ communicative cues increases in response to infants’ gestures and vocalizations, providing new information about how infants’ own behaviors shape their input.
  • These findings emphasize the importance of understanding how caregivers use a diverse set of communicative behaviors—both separately and together—during everyday interactions with infants.
照顾者与婴儿之间的日常互动是动态和多维的。然而,现有的研究低估了婴儿体验的维度,通常只关注一两个交流信号(如单独的语言,或语言与手势的结合)。在此,我们将介绍 "婴儿引导的交流"(IDC):从照料者到婴儿的一整套交流信号,包括语言、动作、手势、情感和触摸。我们录制了 44 位照顾者与他们 18 到 24 个月大的婴儿在家中玩耍的 10 分钟视频,这些婴儿主要来自美国的白人、中产阶级、讲英语的家庭。对互动的五个 IDC 维度以及婴儿的手势和发声进行了编码。大多数照顾者在整个互动过程中使用了 IDC 的所有五个维度,而且这些维度经常重叠。例如,在婴儿听到的言语中,超过 60% 都伴有一个或多个非语言交流提示。然而,我们发现不同的照顾者在使用 IDC 时存在明显差异,这可能反映出他们是根据婴儿的行为和能力来进行交流的。此外,照护者系统性地增加了 IDC 的维度,针对婴儿的手势和发声使用了更多的重叠提示,并对词汇量较小的婴儿使用了更多的 IDC。研究亮点 婴儿与照料者的日常互动是动态的、多模态的,但现有研究低估了婴儿定向交流中固有的多维性(即同时出现的交流线索的多样性)。婴儿在家中自由玩耍时,60% 以上的语言会与一种或多种非语言交流线索重叠。照顾者的多维度交流暗示会随着婴儿的手势和发声而增加,从而提供了有关婴儿自身行为如何影响其输入的新信息。这些发现强调了了解照顾者如何在与婴儿的日常互动中单独或共同使用各种交流行为的重要性。
{"title":"Infant-directed communication: Examining the many dimensions of everyday caregiver-infant interactions","authors":"Jessica E. Kosie,&nbsp;Casey Lew-Williams","doi":"10.1111/desc.13515","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13515","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Everyday caregiver-infant interactions are dynamic and multidimensional. However, existing research underestimates the dimensionality of infants’ experiences, often focusing on one or two communicative signals (e.g., speech alone, or speech and gesture together). Here, we introduce “infant-directed communication” (IDC): the suite of communicative signals from caregivers to infants including speech, action, gesture, emotion, and touch. We recorded 10 min of at-home play between 44 caregivers and their 18- to 24-month-old infants from predominantly white, middle-class, English-speaking families in the United States. Interactions were coded for five dimensions of IDC as well as infants’ gestures and vocalizations. Most caregivers used all five dimensions of IDC throughout the interaction, and these dimensions frequently overlapped. For example, over 60% of the speech that infants heard was accompanied by one or more non-verbal communicative cues. However, we saw marked variation across caregivers in their use of IDC, likely reflecting tailored communication to the behaviors and abilities of their infant. Moreover, caregivers systematically increased the dimensionality of IDC, using more overlapping cues in response to infant gestures and vocalizations, and more IDC with infants who had smaller vocabularies. Understanding how and when caregivers use all five signals—together and separately—in interactions with infants has the potential to redefine how developmental scientists conceive of infants’ communicative environments, and enhance our understanding of the relations between caregiver input and early learning.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Infants’ everyday interactions with caregivers are dynamic and multimodal, but existing research has underestimated the multidimensionality (i.e., the diversity of simultaneously occurring communicative cues) inherent in infant-directed communication.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Over 60% of the speech that infants encounter during at-home, free play interactions overlap with one or more of a variety of non-speech communicative cues.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The multidimensionality of caregivers’ communicative cues increases in response to infants’ gestures and vocalizations, providing new information about how infants’ own behaviors shape their input.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>These findings emphasize the importance of understanding how caregivers use a diverse set of communicative behaviors—both separately and together—during everyday interactions with infants.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 ","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-15","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13515","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574805","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Preschoolers benefit from sentential context in familiar- and unfamiliar-accented speech 学龄前儿童从熟悉和不熟悉口音语音的句子语境中受益
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-14 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13508
Naz Deniz Atik, Alexander LaTourrette, Sandra R. Waxman

To learn the meaning of a new word, or to recognize the meaning of a known one, both children and adults benefit from surrounding words, or the sentential context. Most of the evidence from children is based on their accuracy and efficiency when listening to speech in their familiar native accent: they successfully use the words they know to identify other words’ referents. Here, we assess how accurately and efficiently 4-year-old children use sentential context to identify referents of known and novel nouns in unfamiliar-accented speech, as compared to familiar-accented speech. In a looking-while-listening task, children showed considerable success in processing unfamiliar-accented speech. Children robustly mapped known nouns produced in an unfamiliar accent to their target referents rather than novel competitors, and they used informative surrounding verbs (e.g., “You can eat the dax”) to identify the referents of both known and novel nouns—although there was a processing cost for unfamiliar-accented speech in some cases. This demonstrates that 4-year-olds successfully and rapidly process unfamiliar-accented speech by recruiting the same strategies available to them in familiar-accented speech, revealing impressive flexibility in word recognition and word learning across diverse linguistic environments.

Research Highlights

  • We examined 4-year-old children's accuracy and processing efficiency in comprehending known and novel nouns embedded in sentences produced in familiar-accented or unfamiliar-accented speech.
  • Children showed limited processing costs for unfamiliar-accented speech and mapped known words to their referents even when these were produced in unfamiliar-accented speech.
  • Children used known verbs to predict the referents of upcoming nouns in both familiar- and unfamiliar-accented speech, but processing costs were evident for unfamiliar-accented speech.
  • Thus, the strategies that support children's word comprehension and word learning in familiar-accented speech are available to them in unfamiliar accents as well.
要学习一个新词的意思,或识别一个已知词的意思,儿童和成人都会从周围的词或句子上下文中受益。来自儿童的大部分证据都是基于他们在听自己熟悉的母语口音时的准确性和效率:他们成功地使用自己熟悉的单词来识别其他单词的所指。在此,我们将评估与熟悉口音的语音相比,4 岁儿童在不熟悉口音的语音中使用句子上下文识别已知和新名词的指代时的准确性和效率。在边听边看的任务中,儿童在处理陌生口音语音时表现得相当成功。他们使用信息丰富的周围动词(如 "你可以吃达克斯")来识别已知和新名词的指代对象--尽管在某些情况下,陌生口音语音会带来处理成本。研究亮点 我们研究了 4 岁儿童在理解以熟悉语调或陌生语调发出的句子中包含的已知和新名词时的准确性和处理效率。儿童对陌生口音语音的处理成本有限,即使在陌生口音语音中,他们也能将已知单词映射到其参照物上。在熟悉和不熟悉语调的语音中,儿童都会使用已知的动词来预测即将出现的名词的所指,但在不熟悉语调的语音中,儿童的处理成本很明显。因此,在熟悉口音的语音中支持儿童词语理解和词语学习的策略,在陌生口音的语音中也同样适用。
{"title":"Preschoolers benefit from sentential context in familiar- and unfamiliar-accented speech","authors":"Naz Deniz Atik,&nbsp;Alexander LaTourrette,&nbsp;Sandra R. Waxman","doi":"10.1111/desc.13508","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13508","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>To learn the meaning of a new word, or to recognize the meaning of a known one, both children and adults benefit from surrounding words, or the sentential context. Most of the evidence from children is based on their accuracy and efficiency when listening to speech in their familiar native accent: they successfully use the words they know to identify other words’ referents. Here, we assess how accurately and efficiently 4-year-old children use sentential context to identify referents of known and novel nouns in unfamiliar-accented speech, as compared to familiar-accented speech. In a looking-while-listening task, children showed considerable success in processing unfamiliar-accented speech. Children robustly mapped known nouns produced in an unfamiliar accent to their target referents rather than novel competitors, and they used informative surrounding verbs (e.g., “You can <i>eat</i> the dax”) to identify the referents of both known and novel nouns—although there was a processing cost for unfamiliar-accented speech in some cases. This demonstrates that 4-year-olds successfully and rapidly process unfamiliar-accented speech by recruiting the same strategies available to them in familiar-accented speech, revealing impressive flexibility in word recognition and word learning across diverse linguistic environments.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>We examined 4-year-old children's accuracy and processing efficiency in comprehending known and novel nouns embedded in sentences produced in familiar-accented or unfamiliar-accented speech.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children showed limited processing costs for unfamiliar-accented speech and mapped known words to their referents even when these were produced in unfamiliar-accented speech.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Children used known verbs to predict the referents of upcoming nouns in both familiar- and unfamiliar-accented speech, but processing costs were evident for unfamiliar-accented speech.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Thus, the strategies that support children's word comprehension and word learning in familiar-accented speech are available to them in unfamiliar accents as well.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-14","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13508","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574905","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
When is a word in good company for learning? 什么时候才是学习的好伙伴?
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-04-10 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13510
Layla Unger, Tyler Chang, Olivera Savic, Benjamin K. Bergen, Vladimir M. Sloutsky

Although identifying the referents of single words is often cited as a key challenge for getting word learning off the ground, it overlooks the fact that young learners consistently encounter words in the context of other words. How does this company help or hinder word learning? Prior investigations into early word learning from children's real-world language input have yielded conflicting results, with some influential findings suggesting an advantage for words that keep a diverse company of other words, and others suggesting the opposite. Here, we sought to triangulate the source of this conflict, comparing different measures of diversity and approaches to controlling for correlated effects of word frequency across multiple languages. The results were striking: while different diversity measures on their own yielded conflicting results, once nonlinear relationships with word frequency were controlled, we found convergent evidence that contextual consistency supports early word learning.

Research Highlights

  • The words children learn occur in a sea of other words. The company words keep ranges from highly variable to highly consistent and circumscribed.
  • Prior findings conflict over whether variability versus consistency helps early word learning.
  • Accounting for correlated effects of word frequency resolved the conflict across multiple languages.
  • Results reveal convergent evidence that consistency helps early word learning.
虽然识别单个单词的指代对象常常被认为是单词学习起步阶段的关键挑战,但这忽略了一个事实,即年轻的学习者总是在其他单词的语境中遇到单词。这种情况是如何帮助或阻碍单词学习的呢?之前对儿童从真实世界的语言输入中进行早期单词学习的研究得出了相互矛盾的结果,一些有影响力的研究结果表明,与其他单词保持多样化同伴关系的单词具有优势,而另一些研究结果则相反。在此,我们试图对这一冲突的根源进行三角分析,比较不同的多样性测量方法和控制多语言词频相关影响的方法。结果令人震惊:虽然不同的多样性测量方法本身会产生相互矛盾的结果,但一旦控制了与词频的非线性关系,我们就发现了一致的证据,即上下文的一致性支持早期的单词学习。儿童所学的单词是在其他单词的海洋中出现的。单词的陪伴者既有高度多变的,也有高度一致和限定的。先前的研究结果在多变性与一致性是否有助于早期单词学习的问题上存在冲突。考虑到词频的相关影响,解决了多种语言中的冲突。研究结果表明,一致性有助于早期单词学习。
{"title":"When is a word in good company for learning?","authors":"Layla Unger,&nbsp;Tyler Chang,&nbsp;Olivera Savic,&nbsp;Benjamin K. Bergen,&nbsp;Vladimir M. Sloutsky","doi":"10.1111/desc.13510","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13510","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Although identifying the referents of single words is often cited as a key challenge for getting word learning off the ground, it overlooks the fact that young learners consistently encounter words in the context of other words. How does this company help or hinder word learning? Prior investigations into early word learning from children's real-world language input have yielded conflicting results, with some influential findings suggesting an advantage for words that keep a diverse company of other words, and others suggesting the opposite. Here, we sought to triangulate the source of this conflict, comparing different measures of diversity and approaches to controlling for correlated effects of word frequency across multiple languages. The results were striking: while different diversity measures on their own yielded conflicting results, once nonlinear relationships with word frequency were controlled, we found convergent evidence that contextual consistency supports early word learning.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>The words children learn occur in a sea of other words. The company words keep ranges from highly variable to highly consistent and circumscribed.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Prior findings conflict over whether variability versus consistency helps early word learning.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Accounting for correlated effects of word frequency resolved the conflict across multiple languages.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Results reveal convergent evidence that consistency helps early word learning.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-04-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13510","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574739","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of self-referential and social processing in the relationship between pubertal status and difficulties in mental health and emotion regulation in adolescent girls in the UK 自我参照和社会处理在青春期状况与英国少女心理健康和情绪调节困难之间的关系中的作用
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 Psychology Pub Date : 2024-04-04 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13503
Saz P. Ahmed, Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer, Madeleine E. Moses-Payne, Anne-Lise Goddings, Lydia G. Speyer, Willem Kuyken, Tim Dalgleish, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore

Adolescence is marked by the onset of puberty, which is associated with an increase in mental health difficulties, particularly in girls. Social and self-referential processes also develop during this period: adolescents become more aware of others’ perspectives, and judgements about themselves become less favourable. In the current study, data from 119 girls (from London, UK) aged 9–16 years were collected at two-time points (between 2019 and 2021) to investigate the relationship between puberty and difficulties in mental health and emotion regulation, as well as the role of self-referential and social processing in this relationship. Structural equation modelling showed that advanced pubertal status predicted greater mental health and emotion regulation difficulties, including depression and anxiety, rumination and overall difficulties in emotion regulation, and in mental health and behaviour. Advanced pubertal status also predicted greater perspective-taking abilities and negative self-schemas. Exploratory analyses showed that negative self-schemas mediated the relationships between puberty and rumination, overall emotion regulation difficulties, and depression (although these effects were small and would not survive correction for multiple comparisons). The results suggest that advanced pubertal status is associated with higher mental health and emotion regulation problems during adolescence and that negative self-schemas may play a role in this association.

Research Highlights

  • This study investigates the relationship between puberty, mental health, emotion regulation difficulties, and social and self-referential processing in girls aged 9–16 years.
  • Advanced pubertal status was associated with worse mental health and greater emotion regulation difficulties, better perspective-taking abilities and negative self-schemas.
  • Negative self-schemas may play a role in the relationships between advanced pubertal status and depression, and advanced pubertal status and emotion regulation difficulties, including rumination.
青春期的特点是开始进入青春期,这与心理健康问题的增加有关,尤其是女孩。在这一时期,社会和自我参照过程也在发展:青少年变得更加了解他人的观点,对自己的判断也变得不那么有利。本研究在两个时间点(2019 年和 2021 年)收集了 119 名 9-16 岁女孩(来自英国伦敦)的数据,以调查青春期与心理健康和情绪调节困难之间的关系,以及自我参照和社会处理在这一关系中的作用。结构方程模型显示,青春期提前预示着更大的心理健康和情绪调节困难,包括抑郁和焦虑、反刍和情绪调节方面的总体困难,以及心理健康和行为方面的困难。青春期提前还预示着更强的透视能力和消极的自我暗示。探索性分析表明,消极的自我暗示在青春期与反刍、整体情绪调节困难和抑郁之间起到了中介作用(尽管这些作用很小,而且经不起多重比较校正)。研究亮点 本研究调查了 9-16 岁女孩的青春期、心理健康、情绪调节障碍以及社会和自我参照处理之间的关系。青春期提前与较差的心理健康状况、较严重的情绪调节障碍、较好的透视能力和消极的自我暗示有关。消极的自我暗示可能在青春期提前与抑郁、青春期提前与情绪调节障碍(包括反刍)之间的关系中起作用。
{"title":"The role of self-referential and social processing in the relationship between pubertal status and difficulties in mental health and emotion regulation in adolescent girls in the UK","authors":"Saz P. Ahmed,&nbsp;Blanca Piera Pi-Sunyer,&nbsp;Madeleine E. Moses-Payne,&nbsp;Anne-Lise Goddings,&nbsp;Lydia G. Speyer,&nbsp;Willem Kuyken,&nbsp;Tim Dalgleish,&nbsp;Sarah-Jayne Blakemore","doi":"10.1111/desc.13503","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13503","url":null,"abstract":"<div>\u0000 \u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <p>Adolescence is marked by the onset of puberty, which is associated with an increase in mental health difficulties, particularly in girls. Social and self-referential processes also develop during this period: adolescents become more aware of others’ perspectives, and judgements about themselves become less favourable. In the current study, data from 119 girls (from London, UK) aged 9–16 years were collected at two-time points (between 2019 and 2021) to investigate the relationship between puberty and difficulties in mental health and emotion regulation, as well as the role of self-referential and social processing in this relationship. Structural equation modelling showed that advanced pubertal status predicted greater mental health and emotion regulation difficulties, including depression and anxiety, rumination and overall difficulties in emotion regulation, and in mental health and behaviour. Advanced pubertal status also predicted greater perspective-taking abilities and negative self-schemas. Exploratory analyses showed that negative self-schemas mediated the relationships between puberty and rumination, overall emotion regulation difficulties, and depression (although these effects were small and would not survive correction for multiple comparisons). The results suggest that advanced pubertal status is associated with higher mental health and emotion regulation problems during adolescence and that negative self-schemas may play a role in this association.</p>\u0000 </section>\u0000 \u0000 <section>\u0000 \u0000 <h3> Research Highlights</h3>\u0000 \u0000 <div>\u0000 <ul>\u0000 \u0000 <li>This study investigates the relationship between puberty, mental health, emotion regulation difficulties, and social and self-referential processing in girls aged 9–16 years.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Advanced pubertal status was associated with worse mental health and greater emotion regulation difficulties, better perspective-taking abilities and negative self-schemas.</li>\u0000 \u0000 <li>Negative self-schemas may play a role in the relationships between advanced pubertal status and depression, and advanced pubertal status and emotion regulation difficulties, including rumination.</li>\u0000 </ul>\u0000 </div>\u0000 </section>\u0000 </div>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-04-04","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/epdf/10.1111/desc.13503","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140574983","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1