首页 > 最新文献

Developmental Science最新文献

英文 中文
A dyadic investigation of shy children's behavioral and affective responses to delivering a speech. 对害羞儿童在发表演讲时的行为和情感反应的双向调查。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13558
Raha Hassan, Louis A Schmidt

Shyness is typically associated with avoidant social behavior and restricted affect in new social situations. However, we know considerably less about how one child's shyness influences another child's behavior and affect in new social situations. Children's shyness was parent-reported when children were age 3 (N = 105, 52 girls, Mage = 3.50 years), and children were tested approximately 1 year later (Mage = 4.76 years) in same-gender dyads where they were asked to give an impromptu speech about their most recent birthday in front of an experimenter and the other member of the dyad. We examined whether children's shyness and speech order influenced their own and their social partner's observed behavior and affect during the speech. Regardless of speech order, children's own shyness was positively associated with their own avoidant social behavior and gaze aversion. Regardless of shyness, children who gave their speech second averted their gaze more than children who gave their speech first. We also found that children who gave their speech second displayed less positive affect if their social partner who they watched give the speech first was shyer. We speculate that some 4-year-old children may be sensitive to the avoidant behaviors of their shy peers and, in turn, respond with less animation when it is their turn to participate in the same activity. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We examined whether preschool children's shyness and speech order influenced their own and their social partner's observed behavior and affect during a dyadic speech task Children's own shyness was positively associated with their own avoidant social behavior and gaze aversion Children who gave their speech second averted their gaze more than children who gave their speech first. Children who gave their speech second displayed less positive affect if their social partner who they watched give a speech first was shyer These findings suggest that preschool-aged children are sensitive to learning about their environment indirectly through social observation.

害羞通常与回避社交行为和在新的社交环境中情感受限有关。然而,我们对一个孩子的害羞如何影响另一个孩子在新社交情境中的行为和情感却知之甚少。在儿童 3 岁时,家长会报告儿童的害羞情况(人数 = 105,52 名女孩,年龄 = 3.50 岁),大约 1 年后(年龄 = 4.76 岁),儿童在同性别的二人组中接受测试,他们被要求在实验者和二人组的另一名成员面前就自己最近的生日发表即兴演讲。我们研究了儿童的害羞心理和演讲顺序是否会影响他们自己及其社会伙伴在演讲过程中的行为和情感。无论说话顺序如何,儿童自己的害羞与他们自己的回避社交行为和目光厌恶呈正相关。无论害羞与否,第二个发言的儿童比第一个发言的儿童更多地将目光转向他人。我们还发现,如果先发言的社交伙伴比较害羞,那么后发言的儿童表现出的积极情绪就会减少。我们推测,一些 4 岁儿童可能对害羞同伴的回避行为很敏感,因此当轮到他们参加同样的活动时,他们的反应就不那么生动了。研究亮点:我们研究了学龄前儿童的害羞心理和说话顺序是否会影响他们自己及其社会伙伴在双人说话任务中的行为和情绪。儿童自己的害羞心理与他们自己的回避社会行为和目光厌恶呈正相关。这些发现表明,学龄前儿童对通过社会观察间接了解周围环境很敏感。
{"title":"A dyadic investigation of shy children's behavioral and affective responses to delivering a speech.","authors":"Raha Hassan, Louis A Schmidt","doi":"10.1111/desc.13558","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13558","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Shyness is typically associated with avoidant social behavior and restricted affect in new social situations. However, we know considerably less about how one child's shyness influences another child's behavior and affect in new social situations. Children's shyness was parent-reported when children were age 3 (N = 105, 52 girls, M<sub>age </sub>= 3.50 years), and children were tested approximately 1 year later (M<sub>age </sub>= 4.76 years) in same-gender dyads where they were asked to give an impromptu speech about their most recent birthday in front of an experimenter and the other member of the dyad. We examined whether children's shyness and speech order influenced their own and their social partner's observed behavior and affect during the speech. Regardless of speech order, children's own shyness was positively associated with their own avoidant social behavior and gaze aversion. Regardless of shyness, children who gave their speech second averted their gaze more than children who gave their speech first. We also found that children who gave their speech second displayed less positive affect if their social partner who they watched give the speech first was shyer. We speculate that some 4-year-old children may be sensitive to the avoidant behaviors of their shy peers and, in turn, respond with less animation when it is their turn to participate in the same activity. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We examined whether preschool children's shyness and speech order influenced their own and their social partner's observed behavior and affect during a dyadic speech task Children's own shyness was positively associated with their own avoidant social behavior and gaze aversion Children who gave their speech second averted their gaze more than children who gave their speech first. Children who gave their speech second displayed less positive affect if their social partner who they watched give a speech first was shyer These findings suggest that preschool-aged children are sensitive to learning about their environment indirectly through social observation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001073","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of formal schooling in the development of children's reading and arithmetic white matter networks. 正规学校教育在儿童阅读和算术白质网络发展中的作用。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13557
Floor Vandecruys, Maaike Vandermosten, Bert De Smedt

Children's white matter development is driven by experience, yet it remains poorly understood how it is shaped by attending formal education. A small number of studies compared children before and after the start of formal schooling to understand this, yet they do not allow to separate maturational effects from schooling-related effects. A clever way to (quasi-)experimentally address this issue is the longitudinal school cut-off design, which compares children who are similar in age but differ in schooling (because they are born right before or after the cut-off date for school entry). We used for the first time such a longitudinal school cut-off design to experimentally investigate the effect of schooling on children's white matter networks. We compared "young" first graders (schooling group, n = 34; Mage = 68 months; 20 girls) and "old" preschoolers (non-schooling group, n = 33; Mage = 66 months; 18 girls) that were similar in age but differed in the amount of formal instruction they received. Our study revealed that changes in fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity in five a priori selected white matter tracts during the transition from preschool to primary school were predominantly driven by age-related maturation. We did not find specific schooling effects on white matter, despite their strong presence for early reading and early arithmetic skills. The present study is the first to disentangle the effects of age-related maturation and schooling on white matter within a longitudinal cohort of 5-year-old preschoolers. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: White matter tracts that have been associated with reading and arithmetic may be susceptible to experience-dependent neuroplasticity when children learn to read and calculate. This longitudinal study used the school cut-off design to isolate schooling-induced from coinciding maturational influences on children's white matter development. White matter changes during the transition from preschool to primary school are predominantly driven by age-related maturation and not by schooling effects. Strong effects of schooling on behavior were shown for early reading and early arithmetic, but not for verbal ability and spatial ability.

儿童的白质发育是由经验驱动的,但人们对正规教育如何影响儿童的白质发育仍然知之甚少。为了解这一问题,少数研究对儿童开始接受正规教育前后进行了比较,但这些研究无法将成熟效应与学校教育相关效应区分开来。纵向学校截止日期设计是(准)实验性地解决这一问题的一种巧妙方法,它可以比较年龄相似但就学情况不同的儿童(因为他们是在入学截止日期之前或之后出生的)。我们首次采用了这种纵向学校截止日期设计来实验研究学校教育对儿童白质网络的影响。我们比较了 "年轻 "的一年级学生(入学组,人数=34;年龄=68个月;20个女孩)和 "年老 "的学龄前儿童(未入学组,人数=33;年龄=66个月;18个女孩)。我们的研究表明,在从学前班到小学的过渡时期,五个先验选定的白质束的分数各向异性和平均扩散率的变化主要是由与年龄相关的成熟度引起的。尽管白质对早期阅读和早期算术能力有很大影响,但我们没有发现学校教育对白质的具体影响。本研究首次在 5 岁学龄前儿童的纵向队列中区分了年龄相关成熟和学校教育对白质的影响。研究亮点:当儿童学习阅读和计算时,与阅读和计算相关的白质束可能会受到经验依赖性神经可塑性的影响。这项纵向研究采用了 "学校截断设计",以区分学校教育和同时成熟对儿童白质发育的影响。从学前班到小学的过渡时期,白质的变化主要由与年龄相关的成熟而非学校教育的影响所驱动。在早期阅读和早期算术方面,学校教育对行为有很强的影响,但在语言能力和空间能力方面则没有。
{"title":"The role of formal schooling in the development of children's reading and arithmetic white matter networks.","authors":"Floor Vandecruys, Maaike Vandermosten, Bert De Smedt","doi":"10.1111/desc.13557","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13557","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Children's white matter development is driven by experience, yet it remains poorly understood how it is shaped by attending formal education. A small number of studies compared children before and after the start of formal schooling to understand this, yet they do not allow to separate maturational effects from schooling-related effects. A clever way to (quasi-)experimentally address this issue is the longitudinal school cut-off design, which compares children who are similar in age but differ in schooling (because they are born right before or after the cut-off date for school entry). We used for the first time such a longitudinal school cut-off design to experimentally investigate the effect of schooling on children's white matter networks. We compared \"young\" first graders (schooling group, n = 34; M<sub>age</sub> = 68 months; 20 girls) and \"old\" preschoolers (non-schooling group, n = 33; M<sub>age</sub> = 66 months; 18 girls) that were similar in age but differed in the amount of formal instruction they received. Our study revealed that changes in fractional anisotropy and mean diffusivity in five a priori selected white matter tracts during the transition from preschool to primary school were predominantly driven by age-related maturation. We did not find specific schooling effects on white matter, despite their strong presence for early reading and early arithmetic skills. The present study is the first to disentangle the effects of age-related maturation and schooling on white matter within a longitudinal cohort of 5-year-old preschoolers. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: White matter tracts that have been associated with reading and arithmetic may be susceptible to experience-dependent neuroplasticity when children learn to read and calculate. This longitudinal study used the school cut-off design to isolate schooling-induced from coinciding maturational influences on children's white matter development. White matter changes during the transition from preschool to primary school are predominantly driven by age-related maturation and not by schooling effects. Strong effects of schooling on behavior were shown for early reading and early arithmetic, but not for verbal ability and spatial ability.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141917861","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Event-related potentials of familiar monosyllabic words with unexpected lexical tones: A picture-word study of Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with and without a history of late talking. 带有意外词调的熟悉单音节词的事件相关电位:对有和没有晚说话史的讲普通话学龄前儿童进行的图画词语研究。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-07 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13553
Hsin-Hui Lu, Hong-Hsiang Liu, Feng-Ming Tsao

This study examined how Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with and without a history of late talking (LT) process familiar monosyllabic words with unexpected lexical tones, focusing on both phonological and semantic violations. This study initially enrolled 64 Mandarin-speaking toddlers: 31 with a history of LT (mean age: 27.67 months) and 33 without a history of LT (non-LT) (mean age: 27.85 months). Event-related potentials were recorded at the age of 4 years during a picture-word mismatch task (LT mean age: 51.36 months; non-LT mean age: 51.20 months); in this task, the participants were presented with auditory words either matching (Tone 3) or mismatching with images in terms of their lexical tones; the mismatches encompassed acoustically dissimilar (Tone 1) and similar (Tone 2) mismatches. A significant difference in the phonological mapping negativity (PMN) responses to Tones 1 and 3 was observed only in the non-LT group. However, differences in the N400 responses to Tones 1 and 3 remained consistent across both groups. In addition, greater differences in the PMN responses between Tones 1 and 3 were associated with higher language proficiency during the preschool period. The PMN response serves as an indicator of neural correlates in lexical tone processing, reflecting challenges encountered by preschoolers with a history of LT when processing the lexical tones of familiar words. Furthermore, the PMN response was correlated with concurrent language abilities. These findings indicate the importance of early tonal perception development for Mandarin speakers with a history of LT. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Preschoolers with a history of late talking (LT), similar to preschoolers without such a history, can establish word expectations and detect the lexical tone violation in real time. However, those with a history of LT require additional time to process acoustic cues and differentiate between word semantics based on lexical tone information. The phonological mapping negativity response serves as an indicator of neural correlates in lexical tone processing, reflecting challenges encountered by preschoolers with a history of LT when processing the lexical tones of familiar words. The present findings indicate the importance of early intervention for Mandarin speakers with a history of LT, with an emphasis on lexical tone processing from toddlerhood.

本研究考察了有和没有晚说话(LT)病史的普通话学前儿童如何处理带有意外词调的熟悉单音节词,重点关注语音和语义方面的违规行为。本研究最初招募了 64 名说普通话的幼儿:其中 31 名幼儿有低龄学步期(平均年龄:27.67 个月),33 名幼儿无低龄学步期(非低龄学步期)(平均年龄:27.85 个月)。在这项任务中,参与者将看到与图像在词调上匹配(音调 3)或不匹配的听觉单词;不匹配包括声学上不同(音调 1)和相似(音调 2)的不匹配。语音映射负性(PMN)对音调 1 和音调 3 的反应仅在非 LT 组观察到明显差异。然而,两组对音调 1 和 3 的 N400 反应差异仍然一致。此外,音调 1 和 3 之间的 PMN 反应差异越大,学龄前的语言水平越高。PMN 反应是词汇音调处理神经相关性的指标,反映了有语言障碍史的学龄前儿童在处理熟悉单词的词汇音调时所遇到的挑战。此外,PMN 反应还与同时具备的语言能力相关。这些研究结果表明,早期音调感知的发展对有语言障碍史的普通话学习者非常重要。研究亮点:有晚期说话(LT)史的学龄前儿童与无晚期说话史的学龄前儿童相似,都能建立词的预期并实时检测词性语调的违反。然而,有晚期说话史的学龄前儿童需要更多的时间来处理声音线索,并根据词调信息来区分单词语义。语音映射负性反应是词调处理过程中神经相关性的一个指标,反映了有词调失常病史的学龄前儿童在处理熟悉单词的词调时所遇到的挑战。本研究结果表明,对有语言障碍史的普通话学习者进行早期干预非常重要,重点是从幼儿期开始进行词调处理。
{"title":"Event-related potentials of familiar monosyllabic words with unexpected lexical tones: A picture-word study of Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with and without a history of late talking.","authors":"Hsin-Hui Lu, Hong-Hsiang Liu, Feng-Ming Tsao","doi":"10.1111/desc.13553","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13553","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined how Mandarin-speaking preschoolers with and without a history of late talking (LT) process familiar monosyllabic words with unexpected lexical tones, focusing on both phonological and semantic violations. This study initially enrolled 64 Mandarin-speaking toddlers: 31 with a history of LT (mean age: 27.67 months) and 33 without a history of LT (non-LT) (mean age: 27.85 months). Event-related potentials were recorded at the age of 4 years during a picture-word mismatch task (LT mean age: 51.36 months; non-LT mean age: 51.20 months); in this task, the participants were presented with auditory words either matching (Tone 3) or mismatching with images in terms of their lexical tones; the mismatches encompassed acoustically dissimilar (Tone 1) and similar (Tone 2) mismatches. A significant difference in the phonological mapping negativity (PMN) responses to Tones 1 and 3 was observed only in the non-LT group. However, differences in the N400 responses to Tones 1 and 3 remained consistent across both groups. In addition, greater differences in the PMN responses between Tones 1 and 3 were associated with higher language proficiency during the preschool period. The PMN response serves as an indicator of neural correlates in lexical tone processing, reflecting challenges encountered by preschoolers with a history of LT when processing the lexical tones of familiar words. Furthermore, the PMN response was correlated with concurrent language abilities. These findings indicate the importance of early tonal perception development for Mandarin speakers with a history of LT. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Preschoolers with a history of late talking (LT), similar to preschoolers without such a history, can establish word expectations and detect the lexical tone violation in real time. However, those with a history of LT require additional time to process acoustic cues and differentiate between word semantics based on lexical tone information. The phonological mapping negativity response serves as an indicator of neural correlates in lexical tone processing, reflecting challenges encountered by preschoolers with a history of LT when processing the lexical tones of familiar words. The present findings indicate the importance of early intervention for Mandarin speakers with a history of LT, with an emphasis on lexical tone processing from toddlerhood.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-07","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141903263","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neural evidence of core foundations and conceptual change in preschool numeracy. 学前算术核心基础和概念变化的神经证据。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-06 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13556
Chi-Chuan Chen, Ilaria Berteletti, Daniel C Hyde

Symbolic numeracy first emerges as children learn the meanings of number words and how to use them to precisely count sets of objects. This development starts before children enter school and forms a foundation for lifelong mathematics achievement. Despite its importance, exactly how children acquire this basic knowledge is unclear. Here we test competing theories of early number learning by measuring event-related brain potentials during a novel number word-quantity comparison task in 3-4-year-old preschool children (N = 128). We find several qualitative differences in neural processing of number by conceptual stage of development. Specifically, we find differences in early attention-related parietal electrophysiology (N1), suggesting that less conceptually advanced children process arrays as individual objects and more advanced children distribute attention over the entire set. Subsequently, we find that only more conceptually advanced children show later-going frontal (N2) sensitivity to the numerical-distance relationship between the number word and visual quantity. The nature of this response suggested that exact rather than approximate numerical meanings were being associated with number words over frontal sites. No evidence of numerical distance effects was observed over posterior scalp sites. Together these results suggest that children may engage parallel individuation of objects to learn the meanings of the first few number words, but, ultimately, create new exact cardinal value representations for number words that cannot be defined in terms of core, nonverbal number systems. More broadly, these results document an interaction between attentional and general cognitive mechanisms in cognitive development. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Conceptual development in numeracy is associated with a shift in attention from objects to sets. Children acquire meanings of the first few number words through associations with parallel attentional individuation of objects. Understanding of cardinality is associated with attentional processing of sets rather than individuals. Brain signatures suggest children attribute exact rather than approximate numerical meanings to the first few number words. Number-quantity relationship processing for the first few number words is evident in frontal but not parietal scalp electrophysiology of young children.

符号运算能力首先出现在儿童学习数词的含义以及如何使用数词精确计算一组物体的过程中。这一发展始于儿童入学之前,并为其终生的数学成就奠定了基础。尽管这很重要,但儿童究竟是如何获得这些基础知识的,目前尚不清楚。在这里,我们通过测量 3-4 岁学龄前儿童(128 人)在新颖的数词-数量比较任务中的事件相关脑电位,来检验早期数字学习的各种理论。我们发现,不同概念发展阶段的儿童对数字的神经处理存在一些质的差异。具体来说,我们发现早期与注意力相关的顶叶电生理学(N1)存在差异,这表明概念发展水平较低的儿童将数组作为单个物体来处理,而概念发展水平较高的儿童则将注意力分散到整个数组上。随后,我们发现只有概念较先进的儿童才会对数字词和视觉量之间的数字距离关系表现出较晚的额叶(N2)敏感性。这种反应的性质表明,在额叶部位,确切而非近似的数字意义与数词相关联。在后部头皮部位没有观察到数字距离效应的证据。这些结果表明,儿童在学习最初几个数词的含义时,可能会对物体进行平行的个体化处理,但最终会为数词创建新的精确心值表征,而这些表征无法用核心的非语言数字系统来定义。更广泛地说,这些结果证明了认知发展过程中注意机制和一般认知机制之间的相互作用。研究亮点计算概念的发展与注意力从物体转向集合有关。儿童通过联想和平行注意物体的个体化来获得最初几个数词的含义。对万有引力的理解与对集合而非个体的注意处理有关。大脑特征表明,儿童对前几个数字词会赋予确切而非近似的数字含义。在幼儿的额叶而非顶叶头皮电生理学中,前几个数字词的数字-数量关系处理非常明显。
{"title":"Neural evidence of core foundations and conceptual change in preschool numeracy.","authors":"Chi-Chuan Chen, Ilaria Berteletti, Daniel C Hyde","doi":"10.1111/desc.13556","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13556","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Symbolic numeracy first emerges as children learn the meanings of number words and how to use them to precisely count sets of objects. This development starts before children enter school and forms a foundation for lifelong mathematics achievement. Despite its importance, exactly how children acquire this basic knowledge is unclear. Here we test competing theories of early number learning by measuring event-related brain potentials during a novel number word-quantity comparison task in 3-4-year-old preschool children (N = 128). We find several qualitative differences in neural processing of number by conceptual stage of development. Specifically, we find differences in early attention-related parietal electrophysiology (N1), suggesting that less conceptually advanced children process arrays as individual objects and more advanced children distribute attention over the entire set. Subsequently, we find that only more conceptually advanced children show later-going frontal (N2) sensitivity to the numerical-distance relationship between the number word and visual quantity. The nature of this response suggested that exact rather than approximate numerical meanings were being associated with number words over frontal sites. No evidence of numerical distance effects was observed over posterior scalp sites. Together these results suggest that children may engage parallel individuation of objects to learn the meanings of the first few number words, but, ultimately, create new exact cardinal value representations for number words that cannot be defined in terms of core, nonverbal number systems. More broadly, these results document an interaction between attentional and general cognitive mechanisms in cognitive development. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Conceptual development in numeracy is associated with a shift in attention from objects to sets. Children acquire meanings of the first few number words through associations with parallel attentional individuation of objects. Understanding of cardinality is associated with attentional processing of sets rather than individuals. Brain signatures suggest children attribute exact rather than approximate numerical meanings to the first few number words. Number-quantity relationship processing for the first few number words is evident in frontal but not parietal scalp electrophysiology of young children.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141894657","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
A vision for a diverse, inclusive, equitable, and representative developmental science. 发展科学多元化、包容性、公平性和代表性的愿景。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13548
Leher Singh
{"title":"A vision for a diverse, inclusive, equitable, and representative developmental science.","authors":"Leher Singh","doi":"10.1111/desc.13548","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13548","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141876404","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Towards a global developmental science 迈向全球发展科学。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-29 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13555
Leher Singh, Heather Bortfeld
{"title":"Towards a global developmental science","authors":"Leher Singh,&nbsp;Heather Bortfeld","doi":"10.1111/desc.13555","DOIUrl":"10.1111/desc.13555","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-29","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141793811","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Food insecurity and adolescent impulsivity: The mediating role of functional connectivity in the context of family flexibility. 粮食不安全与青少年冲动:家庭灵活性背景下功能连通性的中介作用。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13554
Ava Reck, Lawrence H Sweet, Charles Geier, Steven M Kogan, Zehua Cui, Assaf Oshri

Adolescent food insecurity is a salient adversity hypothesized to affect neural systems associated with increased impulsive behavior. Family environments shape how adverse experiences influence development. In this study, hypotheses were tested regarding the conjoint effects of food insecurity and family flexibility on impulsivity via alterations in connectivity between regions within the salience and central executive networks. Such alterations are reflected in resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) metrics between the anterior insula (AI) and the middle frontal gyrus (MFG). Hypotheses were tested in a longitudinal moderated mediation model with two waves of data from 142 adolescents (Time 1 [T1] Mage = 12.89, SD = 0.85; Time 2 [T2] Mage = 15.01, SD = 1.07). Data on past-year household food insecurity, family flexibility, and rsFC were obtained at T1. Impulsivity was self-reported by the adolescent at T1 and T2. Findings revealed that high T1 left-to-left rsFC between the AI and MFG was associated with increased impulsivity at T2. The interaction of family flexibility and food insecurity was associated with AI and MFG rsFC. In the context of low family flexibility, food insecurity was linked to high levels of AI and MFG rsFC. Conversely, in the context of optimal family flexibility, food insecurity was associated with low levels of AI and MFG rsFC. Conditional indirect analysis suggests that the links among food insecurity, rsFC, and impulsive behavior depend on family flexibility. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Adolescent food insecurity was associated with anterior insula and middle frontal gyrus connectivity only at certain levels of family flexibility. High family flexibility attenuated the link between food insecurity and neural connectivity, while low levels of family flexibility increased this risk. High left anterior insula and left middle frontal gyrus connectivity was associated with increased impulsivity 1 year later.

青少年的食物不安全是一种突出的逆境,据推测会影响与冲动行为增加有关的神经系统。家庭环境决定了逆境经历如何影响成长。在这项研究中,我们通过改变显著性网络和中央执行网络中各区域之间的连通性,检验了有关食物不安全和家庭灵活性对冲动性的共同影响的假设。这种改变反映在前脑岛(AI)和额叶中回(MFG)之间的静息态功能连接(rsFC)指标上。研究人员利用 142 名青少年的两波数据(时间 1 [T1] Mage = 12.89,SD = 0.85;时间 2 [T2] Mage = 15.01,SD = 1.07),在纵向调节中介模型中对假设进行了检验。在时间 1 获得了有关过去一年家庭粮食不安全、家庭灵活性和 rsFC 的数据。冲动性由青少年在 T1 和 T2 进行自我报告。研究结果表明,T1时AI和MFG之间从左到右的高rsFC与T2时冲动性的增加有关。家庭灵活性和食物不安全的交互作用与 AI 和 MFG rsFC 相关。在家庭灵活性低的情况下,食物不安全与高水平的人工智能和 MFG rsFC 有关。相反,在家庭灵活性最佳的情况下,粮食不安全与低水平的 AI 和 MFG rsFC 相关。条件间接分析表明,食物不安全、rsFC 和冲动行为之间的联系取决于家庭灵活性。研究亮点:只有在特定的家庭灵活性水平下,青少年的食物不安全才与前脑岛和额叶中回的连通性有关。家庭灵活性高会减弱食物不安全与神经连接之间的联系,而家庭灵活性低则会增加这种风险。左侧前脑岛和左侧额中回的高连接性与一年后冲动性的增加有关。
{"title":"Food insecurity and adolescent impulsivity: The mediating role of functional connectivity in the context of family flexibility.","authors":"Ava Reck, Lawrence H Sweet, Charles Geier, Steven M Kogan, Zehua Cui, Assaf Oshri","doi":"10.1111/desc.13554","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13554","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Adolescent food insecurity is a salient adversity hypothesized to affect neural systems associated with increased impulsive behavior. Family environments shape how adverse experiences influence development. In this study, hypotheses were tested regarding the conjoint effects of food insecurity and family flexibility on impulsivity via alterations in connectivity between regions within the salience and central executive networks. Such alterations are reflected in resting-state functional connectivity (rsFC) metrics between the anterior insula (AI) and the middle frontal gyrus (MFG). Hypotheses were tested in a longitudinal moderated mediation model with two waves of data from 142 adolescents (Time 1 [T1] M<sub>age</sub> = 12.89, SD = 0.85; Time 2 [T2] M<sub>age</sub> = 15.01, SD = 1.07). Data on past-year household food insecurity, family flexibility, and rsFC were obtained at T1. Impulsivity was self-reported by the adolescent at T1 and T2. Findings revealed that high T1 left-to-left rsFC between the AI and MFG was associated with increased impulsivity at T2. The interaction of family flexibility and food insecurity was associated with AI and MFG rsFC. In the context of low family flexibility, food insecurity was linked to high levels of AI and MFG rsFC. Conversely, in the context of optimal family flexibility, food insecurity was associated with low levels of AI and MFG rsFC. Conditional indirect analysis suggests that the links among food insecurity, rsFC, and impulsive behavior depend on family flexibility. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Adolescent food insecurity was associated with anterior insula and middle frontal gyrus connectivity only at certain levels of family flexibility. High family flexibility attenuated the link between food insecurity and neural connectivity, while low levels of family flexibility increased this risk. High left anterior insula and left middle frontal gyrus connectivity was associated with increased impulsivity 1 year later.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761778","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Limited evidence of test-retest reliability in infant-directed speech preference in a large preregistered infant experiment. 在一项大型预先登记的婴儿实验中,婴儿引导言语偏好的重复测试可靠性证据有限。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13551
Melanie S Schreiner, Martin Zettersten, Christina Bergmann, Michael C Frank, Tom Fritzsche, Nayeli Gonzalez-Gomez, Kiley Hamlin, Natalia Kartushina, Danielle J Kellier, Nivedita Mani, Julien Mayor, Jenny Saffran, Mohinish Shukla, Priya Silverstein, Melanie Soderstrom, Matthias Lippold

Test-retest reliability-establishing that measurements remain consistent across multiple testing sessions-is critical to measuring, understanding, and predicting individual differences in infant language development. However, previous attempts to establish measurement reliability in infant speech perception tasks are limited, and reliability of frequently used infant measures is largely unknown. The current study investigated the test-retest reliability of infants' preference for infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech in a large sample (N = 158) in the context of the ManyBabies1 collaborative research project. Labs were asked to bring in participating infants for a second appointment retesting infants on their preference for infant-directed speech. This approach allowed us to estimate test-retest reliability across three different methods used to investigate preferential listening in infancy: the head-turn preference procedure, central fixation, and eye-tracking. Overall, we found no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants' speech preference (overall r = 0.09, 95% CI [-0.06,0.25]). While increasing the number of trials that infants needed to contribute for inclusion in the analysis revealed a numeric growth in test-retest reliability, it also considerably reduced the study's effective sample size. Therefore, future research on infant development should take into account that not all experimental measures may be appropriate for assessing individual differences between infants. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We assessed test-retest reliability of infants' preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech in a large pre-registered sample (N = 158). There was no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants' speech preference. Applying stricter criteria for the inclusion of participants may lead to higher test-retest reliability, but at the cost of substantial decreases in sample size. Developmental research relying on stable individual differences should consider the underlying reliability of its measures.

测试重复可靠性--确定测量结果在多次测试中保持一致--对于测量、理解和预测婴儿语言发展的个体差异至关重要。然而,以往在婴儿言语感知任务中建立测量可靠性的尝试非常有限,而常用婴儿测量方法的可靠性在很大程度上也是未知的。本研究在 "ManyBabies "1 合作研究项目的背景下,通过大样本(样本数 = 158)调查了婴儿对婴儿引导的语言而非成人引导的语言的偏好的重测可靠性。我们要求实验室对参与研究的婴儿进行第二次预约,重新测试婴儿对婴儿引导语言的偏好。通过这种方法,我们可以估算出用于调查婴儿期倾听偏好的三种不同方法的重测可靠性:转头偏好程序、中心固定和眼动跟踪。总体而言,我们没有发现一致的证据表明婴儿语言偏好的测量结果具有重测可靠性(总体 r = 0.09,95% CI [-0.06,0.25])。虽然增加婴儿参与分析所需的试验次数可以从数字上提高重测可靠性,但这也大大减少了研究的有效样本量。因此,未来的婴儿发展研究应考虑到,并非所有的实验措施都适合评估婴儿的个体差异。研究亮点:我们评估了大量预先登记样本(N = 158)中婴儿对婴儿引导语言的偏好相对于成人引导语言的重测可靠性。没有一致的证据表明婴儿语言偏好的测量结果具有重测可靠性。采用更严格的标准纳入参与者可能会提高重测可靠性,但代价是样本量的大幅减少。依赖于稳定的个体差异的发展研究应考虑其测量的基本可靠性。
{"title":"Limited evidence of test-retest reliability in infant-directed speech preference in a large preregistered infant experiment.","authors":"Melanie S Schreiner, Martin Zettersten, Christina Bergmann, Michael C Frank, Tom Fritzsche, Nayeli Gonzalez-Gomez, Kiley Hamlin, Natalia Kartushina, Danielle J Kellier, Nivedita Mani, Julien Mayor, Jenny Saffran, Mohinish Shukla, Priya Silverstein, Melanie Soderstrom, Matthias Lippold","doi":"10.1111/desc.13551","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13551","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Test-retest reliability-establishing that measurements remain consistent across multiple testing sessions-is critical to measuring, understanding, and predicting individual differences in infant language development. However, previous attempts to establish measurement reliability in infant speech perception tasks are limited, and reliability of frequently used infant measures is largely unknown. The current study investigated the test-retest reliability of infants' preference for infant-directed speech over adult-directed speech in a large sample (N = 158) in the context of the ManyBabies1 collaborative research project. Labs were asked to bring in participating infants for a second appointment retesting infants on their preference for infant-directed speech. This approach allowed us to estimate test-retest reliability across three different methods used to investigate preferential listening in infancy: the head-turn preference procedure, central fixation, and eye-tracking. Overall, we found no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants' speech preference (overall r = 0.09, 95% CI [-0.06,0.25]). While increasing the number of trials that infants needed to contribute for inclusion in the analysis revealed a numeric growth in test-retest reliability, it also considerably reduced the study's effective sample size. Therefore, future research on infant development should take into account that not all experimental measures may be appropriate for assessing individual differences between infants. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: We assessed test-retest reliability of infants' preference for infant-directed over adult-directed speech in a large pre-registered sample (N = 158). There was no consistent evidence of test-retest reliability in measures of infants' speech preference. Applying stricter criteria for the inclusion of participants may lead to higher test-retest reliability, but at the cost of substantial decreases in sample size. Developmental research relying on stable individual differences should consider the underlying reliability of its measures.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141735360","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Parental expressions of love in Chinese American immigrant families: Implications for children's attachment security. 美国华人移民家庭中父母表达爱的方式:对儿童依恋安全的影响。
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13549
Aya Inamori Williams, Chang Liu, Qing Zhou, Jinli Wu, Lionel Meng, Xue Fang Deng, Stephen H Chen

Harlow's seminal work on the nature of attachment focused on the importance of warm, responsive, and loving relationships in children's healthy development. While the need for love and care is arguably universal, the ways in which these emotions are expressed can vary across cultural contexts. We examined how Chinese American parents' expressions of love were associated with children's attachment security. A total of 110 Chinese American immigrant parent-child dyads (children 7-11 years old, 49% girls) participated in 3-min conversations in which parents were instructed to communicate love and care. Proposition-level analyses in speech (total 8825 propositions) identified three types of affection: training (guan and chiao shun); relational affection (qin); and validation (acceptance and encouragement of child's own expression of emotion, thoughts, and behaviors). Higher training was observed in parents with lower American orientation and lower income. Higher relational affection was observed in parents with lower income. Higher validation was observed in parents with higher income. Using path analyses, training and validation were found to be positively associated with children's attachment security beyond parenting styles. Effects of parents' relational affection were moderated by children's American orientation. Results demonstrate how immigrant parents draw on multiple cultural scripts to express love and care. These findings expand traditional concepts of parental love in immigrant families and illustrate how bicultural expressions of love can shape attachment security in middle childhood. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/HqUfIDxkFsE RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Analyses of parent-child discussions identified three parental affection styles (i.e., training, relational affection, and validation) as expressions of love and care in Chinese American immigrant families. Training and validation were positively associated with children's attachment security. Relational affection was associated with lower attachment security for children with higher American cultural orientation, suggesting the effects of parent-child expressions of love are shaped by acculturation.

哈洛(Harlow)关于依恋本质的开创性研究侧重于温暖、回应和爱的关系对儿童健康成长的重要性。虽然对爱和关怀的需求可以说是普遍存在的,但在不同的文化背景下,这些情感的表达方式可能会有所不同。我们研究了华裔美国父母表达爱的方式与儿童依恋安全感之间的关系。共有 110 个华裔美国移民亲子家庭(孩子 7-11 岁,49% 为女孩)参加了 3 分钟的对话,在对话中父母被要求传达爱和关怀。通过对言语中的命题(共 8825 个命题)进行分析,发现了三种类型的亲情:训练型亲情("管 "和 "顺")、关系型亲情("琴")和验证型亲情(接受并鼓励孩子表达自己的情感、想法和行为)。在美国倾向较低和收入较低的父母中,训练程度较高。在收入较低的家长中,关系亲和力较高。在收入较高的父母中,验证度较高。通过路径分析发现,除了养育方式之外,训练和验证与儿童的依恋安全感呈正相关。儿童的美国取向调节了父母亲情的影响。研究结果表明,移民父母是如何利用多种文化脚本来表达爱和关怀的。这些研究结果拓展了移民家庭中父母之爱的传统概念,并说明了双文化的爱的表达方式是如何影响儿童中期的依恋安全感的。本文的视频摘要可在 https://youtu.be/HqUfIDxkFsE RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS 上观看:通过分析亲子间的讨论,发现在美国华人移民家庭中,父母的三种爱的表达方式(即训练式、亲情式和验证式)。训练式和验证式与儿童的依恋安全感呈正相关。对于美国文化取向较高的儿童来说,亲情与较低的依恋安全感相关,这表明亲子间爱的表达方式的效果受文化适应的影响。
{"title":"Parental expressions of love in Chinese American immigrant families: Implications for children's attachment security.","authors":"Aya Inamori Williams, Chang Liu, Qing Zhou, Jinli Wu, Lionel Meng, Xue Fang Deng, Stephen H Chen","doi":"10.1111/desc.13549","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13549","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Harlow's seminal work on the nature of attachment focused on the importance of warm, responsive, and loving relationships in children's healthy development. While the need for love and care is arguably universal, the ways in which these emotions are expressed can vary across cultural contexts. We examined how Chinese American parents' expressions of love were associated with children's attachment security. A total of 110 Chinese American immigrant parent-child dyads (children 7-11 years old, 49% girls) participated in 3-min conversations in which parents were instructed to communicate love and care. Proposition-level analyses in speech (total 8825 propositions) identified three types of affection: training (guan and chiao shun); relational affection (qin); and validation (acceptance and encouragement of child's own expression of emotion, thoughts, and behaviors). Higher training was observed in parents with lower American orientation and lower income. Higher relational affection was observed in parents with lower income. Higher validation was observed in parents with higher income. Using path analyses, training and validation were found to be positively associated with children's attachment security beyond parenting styles. Effects of parents' relational affection were moderated by children's American orientation. Results demonstrate how immigrant parents draw on multiple cultural scripts to express love and care. These findings expand traditional concepts of parental love in immigrant families and illustrate how bicultural expressions of love can shape attachment security in middle childhood. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at https://youtu.be/HqUfIDxkFsE RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Analyses of parent-child discussions identified three parental affection styles (i.e., training, relational affection, and validation) as expressions of love and care in Chinese American immigrant families. Training and validation were positively associated with children's attachment security. Relational affection was associated with lower attachment security for children with higher American cultural orientation, suggesting the effects of parent-child expressions of love are shaped by acculturation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635024","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Encoding colors and tones into working memory concurrently: A developmental investigation. 将颜色和音调同时编码到工作记忆中:发展调查
IF 3.1 1区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, DEVELOPMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.1111/desc.13552
Nelson Cowan, Dominic Guitard

Working memory serves as a means to accumulate information and reorganize it. Researchers have long assumed that the natural organization of information is one stream at a time. This logic leads to the expectation that, when two different series of stimuli are to be remembered, performance should be superior if the series are presented one before the other in succession, rather than concurrently. Moreover, different accounts of attentional limits lead to different expectations for the change in the ability to encode two sets across age groups in childhood. Testing children from first grade (6-7 years) to adulthood, we presented sequences of colored objects and tones in succession or concurrently (with one color accompanying an unrelated tone) and found that performance was equally good no matter which presentation method was used. The results for both presentation methods closely matched the intricate pattern of development observed by Cowan et al. (2018), who used successive presentation only. We found marked developmental improvement in the ability to retain materials in each modality without an increasing cost of attention-sharing between modalities. Humans at least from the elementary school years through young adulthood thus display an ability to accommodate and organize two concurrent streams of information. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Memory for stimuli from multiple modalities is relevant to school performance and learning; here we investigate how attention is shared between remembering colors and tones. Participants received four colors and/or four tones for subsequent recognition on a trial, with dual modalities presented successively (0.5 s per stimulus) or concurrently (0.5 s per pair). Successive versus concurrent presentation had little effect on recognition, and the marked increase in memory performance with age did not come from dividing attention during encoding or maintenance. Children as young as first grade thus can encode and organize for later recognition colors and concurrently-presented, but unrelated, tones.

工作记忆是积累信息和重组信息的一种手段。长期以来,研究人员一直认为,信息的自然组织方式是一次一个信息流。这种逻辑导致了这样一种预期,即当需要记忆两个不同系列的刺激时,如果这两个系列的刺激一个接一个地出现,而不是同时出现,那么记忆效果应该会更好。此外,对注意极限的不同解释导致了对儿童期不同年龄组对两组刺激进行编码的能力变化的不同预期。我们对从一年级(6-7 岁)到成年的儿童进行了测试,将彩色物体和音调的序列依次或同时呈现(一种颜色伴随一种不相关的音调),结果发现,无论采用哪种呈现方法,儿童的表现都同样出色。两种呈现方法的结果与 Cowan 等人(2018 年)观察到的错综复杂的发展模式非常吻合,他们只采用了连续呈现的方法。我们发现,在保留每种模态材料的能力方面都有明显的发展进步,而模态之间的注意力共享成本却没有增加。由此可见,人类至少从小学阶段到青年时期都具有容纳和组织两种并发信息流的能力。研究亮点对多种模式刺激的记忆与学习成绩和学习相关;在此,我们研究了记忆颜色和音调时如何共享注意力。在一次试验中,受试者会收到四种颜色和/或四种音调的后续识别信息,双模式信息会先后(每个刺激 0.5 秒)或同时(每对刺激 0.5 秒)呈现。连续呈现与同时呈现对识别影响不大,而且随着年龄的增长,记忆能力的显著提高并不是因为在编码或维持过程中注意力被分散了。因此,年仅一年级的儿童就能对颜色和同时出现但不相关的音调进行编码和组织,以便日后识别。
{"title":"Encoding colors and tones into working memory concurrently: A developmental investigation.","authors":"Nelson Cowan, Dominic Guitard","doi":"10.1111/desc.13552","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13552","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory serves as a means to accumulate information and reorganize it. Researchers have long assumed that the natural organization of information is one stream at a time. This logic leads to the expectation that, when two different series of stimuli are to be remembered, performance should be superior if the series are presented one before the other in succession, rather than concurrently. Moreover, different accounts of attentional limits lead to different expectations for the change in the ability to encode two sets across age groups in childhood. Testing children from first grade (6-7 years) to adulthood, we presented sequences of colored objects and tones in succession or concurrently (with one color accompanying an unrelated tone) and found that performance was equally good no matter which presentation method was used. The results for both presentation methods closely matched the intricate pattern of development observed by Cowan et al. (2018), who used successive presentation only. We found marked developmental improvement in the ability to retain materials in each modality without an increasing cost of attention-sharing between modalities. Humans at least from the elementary school years through young adulthood thus display an ability to accommodate and organize two concurrent streams of information. RESEARCH HIGHLIGHTS: Memory for stimuli from multiple modalities is relevant to school performance and learning; here we investigate how attention is shared between remembering colors and tones. Participants received four colors and/or four tones for subsequent recognition on a trial, with dual modalities presented successively (0.5 s per stimulus) or concurrently (0.5 s per pair). Successive versus concurrent presentation had little effect on recognition, and the marked increase in memory performance with age did not come from dividing attention during encoding or maintenance. Children as young as first grade thus can encode and organize for later recognition colors and concurrently-presented, but unrelated, tones.</p>","PeriodicalId":48392,"journal":{"name":"Developmental Science","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.1,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635023","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Developmental Science
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1