首页 > 最新文献

Memory & Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Sequential encoding aids working memory for meaningful objects' identities but not for their colors. 顺序编码有助于记忆有意义的对象的身份,而不是它们的颜色。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-10 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01486-4
Yong Hoon Chung, Timothy F Brady, Viola S Störmer

Previous studies have found that real-world objects' identities are better remembered than simple features like colored circles, and this effect is particularly pronounced when these stimuli are encoded one by one in a serial, item-based way. Recent work has also demonstrated that memory for simple features like color is improved if these colors are part of real-world objects, suggesting that meaningful objects can serve as a robust memory scaffold for their associated low-level features. However, it is unclear whether the improved color memory that arises from the colors appearing on real-world objects is affected by encoding format, in particular whether items are encoded sequentially or simultaneously. We test this using randomly colored silhouettes of recognizable versus unrecognizable scrambled objects that offer a uniquely controlled set of stimuli to test color working memory of meaningful versus non-meaningful objects. Participants were presented with four stimuli (silhouettes of objects or scrambled shapes) simultaneously or sequentially. After a short delay, they reported either which colors or which shapes they saw in a two-alternative forced-choice task. We replicated previous findings that meaningful stimuli boost working memory performance for colors (Exp. 1). We found that when participants remembered the colors (Exp. 2) there was no difference in performance across the two encoding formats. However, when participants remembered the shapes and thus identity of the objects (Exp. 3), sequential presentation resulted in better performance than simultaneous presentation. Overall, these results show that different encoding formats can flexibly impact visual working memory depending on what the memory-relevant feature is.

先前的研究发现,现实世界中物体的身份比彩色圆圈等简单特征更容易被记住,当这些刺激以连续的、基于项目的方式逐一编码时,这种影响尤其明显。最近的工作还表明,如果颜色等简单特征是真实世界对象的一部分,则这些特征的记忆会得到改善,这表明有意义的对象可以作为其相关低级特征的强大记忆支架。然而,尚不清楚真实世界对象上出现的颜色所产生的改进的颜色记忆是否受到编码格式的影响,特别是项目是按顺序编码还是同时编码。我们使用可识别和不可识别的混乱物体的随机彩色剪影来测试这一点,这些剪影提供了一组独特控制的刺激,以测试有意义和无意义物体的颜色工作记忆。参与者被同时或依次呈现四种刺激(物体的轮廓或混乱的形状)。在短暂的延迟后,他们报告了他们在两种可选的强迫选择任务中看到的颜色或形状。我们重复了先前的发现,即有意义的刺激可以提高颜色的工作记忆表现(实验1)。我们发现,当参与者记住颜色(实验2)时,两种编码格式的性能没有差异。然而,当参与者记住物体的形状,从而记住物体的身份时(实验3),顺序呈现比同时呈现的表现更好。总之,这些结果表明,不同的编码格式可以灵活地影响视觉工作记忆,这取决于与记忆相关的特征是什么。
{"title":"Sequential encoding aids working memory for meaningful objects' identities but not for their colors.","authors":"Yong Hoon Chung, Timothy F Brady, Viola S Störmer","doi":"10.3758/s13421-023-01486-4","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-023-01486-4","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Previous studies have found that real-world objects' identities are better remembered than simple features like colored circles, and this effect is particularly pronounced when these stimuli are encoded one by one in a serial, item-based way. Recent work has also demonstrated that memory for simple features like color is improved if these colors are part of real-world objects, suggesting that meaningful objects can serve as a robust memory scaffold for their associated low-level features. However, it is unclear whether the improved color memory that arises from the colors appearing on real-world objects is affected by encoding format, in particular whether items are encoded sequentially or simultaneously. We test this using randomly colored silhouettes of recognizable versus unrecognizable scrambled objects that offer a uniquely controlled set of stimuli to test color working memory of meaningful versus non-meaningful objects. Participants were presented with four stimuli (silhouettes of objects or scrambled shapes) simultaneously or sequentially. After a short delay, they reported either which colors or which shapes they saw in a two-alternative forced-choice task. We replicated previous findings that meaningful stimuli boost working memory performance for colors (Exp. 1). We found that when participants remembered the colors (Exp. 2) there was no difference in performance across the two encoding formats. However, when participants remembered the shapes and thus identity of the objects (Exp. 3), sequential presentation resulted in better performance than simultaneous presentation. Overall, these results show that different encoding formats can flexibly impact visual working memory depending on what the memory-relevant feature is.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"2119-2131"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"72015739","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does value-based prioritization at working memory enhance long-term memory? 工作记忆中基于价值的优先排序是否能增强长时记忆?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01532-9
A L Atkinson, A H Waterman, R J Allen

Research has demonstrated that individuals can direct their attention to valuable information in both working memory and long-term memory tasks with observable effects on performance. However, it is currently unclear whether prioritising an item for a working memory task automatically translates into a boost at long-term memory. This was examined in two experiments using relatively short (250 ms per item; Experiment 1) and longer (500 ms per item; Experiment 2) encoding times. Participants first completed a visual working memory task, in which they were presented with series of photographs of everyday objects. Following a brief delay (1,000 ms), they completed a four-alternative forced-choice test. Prior to encoding, participants were informed of the point values associated with each item. In some trials, the first item in the sequence was worth more points than the rest. In other trials, all items were equally valuable. After a filled delay, participants completed a surprise long-term memory task. At working memory, a value effect was reliably observed on recognition accuracy, along with some evidence of faster response times for high-value items. However, there was little consistent evidence of this effect automatically persisting into long-term memory. Thus, the benefits of attentional prioritization in working memory do not always translate into longer-term performance. More broadly, this provides further evidence that manipulations that enhance working memory performance do not necessarily enhance long-term memory.

研究表明,在工作记忆和长时记忆任务中,个人可以将注意力引导到有价值的信息上,并对成绩产生可观察到的影响。然而,目前还不清楚在工作记忆任务中优先考虑一个项目是否会自动转化为对长时记忆的促进。我们使用相对较短的编码时间(每个项目 250 毫秒;实验 1)和较长的编码时间(每个项目 500 毫秒;实验 2)进行了两次实验,对这一问题进行了研究。受试者首先要完成一项视觉工作记忆任务,即向他们展示一系列日常物品的照片。经过短暂的延迟(1,000 毫秒)后,他们完成了一项四选一的强迫选择测试。在编码之前,参与者会被告知每个项目的相关点值。在某些试验中,序列中第一个项目的分值高于其他项目。在其他试验中,所有项目的分值相同。经过填充延迟后,参与者完成一项突击长期记忆任务。在工作记忆中,可以可靠地观察到价值效应对识别准确率的影响,同时有证据表明高价值项目的反应时间更快。然而,几乎没有一致的证据表明这种效应会自动持续到长时记忆中。因此,工作记忆中注意优先顺序的好处并不总是能转化为长期表现。更广泛地说,这进一步证明了提高工作记忆成绩的操作并不一定能提高长时记忆。
{"title":"Does value-based prioritization at working memory enhance long-term memory?","authors":"A L Atkinson, A H Waterman, R J Allen","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01532-9","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01532-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research has demonstrated that individuals can direct their attention to valuable information in both working memory and long-term memory tasks with observable effects on performance. However, it is currently unclear whether prioritising an item for a working memory task automatically translates into a boost at long-term memory. This was examined in two experiments using relatively short (250 ms per item; Experiment 1) and longer (500 ms per item; Experiment 2) encoding times. Participants first completed a visual working memory task, in which they were presented with series of photographs of everyday objects. Following a brief delay (1,000 ms), they completed a four-alternative forced-choice test. Prior to encoding, participants were informed of the point values associated with each item. In some trials, the first item in the sequence was worth more points than the rest. In other trials, all items were equally valuable. After a filled delay, participants completed a surprise long-term memory task. At working memory, a value effect was reliably observed on recognition accuracy, along with some evidence of faster response times for high-value items. However, there was little consistent evidence of this effect automatically persisting into long-term memory. Thus, the benefits of attentional prioritization in working memory do not always translate into longer-term performance. More broadly, this provides further evidence that manipulations that enhance working memory performance do not necessarily enhance long-term memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1983-1998"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588910/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139913776","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Reduced benefit from long-term item frequency contributes to short-term memory deficits in dyslexia. 从长期项目频率中获益的减少导致了阅读障碍患者的短期记忆缺陷。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01601-z
Eva Kimel, Luba Daikhin, Hilla Jakoby, Merav Ahissar

Dyslexia, a specific difficulty in acquiring proficient reading, is also characterized by reduced short-term memory (STM) capacity. Extensive research indicates that individuals with developmental dyslexia (IDDs) benefit less from exposure, and this hampers their long-term knowledge accumulation. It is well established that long-term knowledge has a great effect on performance in STM tasks, and thus IDDs' reduced benefit of exposure could potentially reduce their relative performance in such tasks, especially when frequent items, such as digit-words, are used. In this study we used a standard, widely used, STM assessment: the Digit Span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. The task was conducted twice: in native language and in second language. As exposure to native language is greater than exposure to second language, we predicted that IDDs' performance in the task administered in native language will reveal a larger group difference as compared to second language, due to IDDs' reduced benefit of item frequency. The prediction was confirmed, in line with the hypothesis that reduced STM in dyslexia to a large extent reflects reduced benefits from long-term item frequency and not a reduced STM per se.

诵读困难是获得熟练阅读能力的一种特殊困难,其特征还包括短时记忆(STM)能力下降。大量研究表明,发展性阅读障碍(IDDs)患者从接触中获益较少,这阻碍了他们的长期知识积累。长期知识对 STM 任务的表现有很大影响,这一点已得到公认,因此,IDDs 从接触中获益较少可能会降低他们在此类任务中的相对表现,尤其是在使用数字单词等频繁项目时。在这项研究中,我们使用了一种标准的、广泛使用的 STM 评估方法:韦氏成人智力量表中的数字跨度子测试。这项任务进行了两次:母语和第二语言。由于接触母语的机会多于接触第二语言的机会,我们预测 IDD 在以母语进行的任务中的表现与以第二语言进行的任务相比,会显示出更大的群体差异,这是因为 IDD 在项目频率方面的获益较少。这一预测得到了证实,与阅读障碍患者的 STM 减少在很大程度上反映了长期项目频率带来的益处减少,而非 STM 本身减少的假设相吻合。
{"title":"Reduced benefit from long-term item frequency contributes to short-term memory deficits in dyslexia.","authors":"Eva Kimel, Luba Daikhin, Hilla Jakoby, Merav Ahissar","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01601-z","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01601-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dyslexia, a specific difficulty in acquiring proficient reading, is also characterized by reduced short-term memory (STM) capacity. Extensive research indicates that individuals with developmental dyslexia (IDDs) benefit less from exposure, and this hampers their long-term knowledge accumulation. It is well established that long-term knowledge has a great effect on performance in STM tasks, and thus IDDs' reduced benefit of exposure could potentially reduce their relative performance in such tasks, especially when frequent items, such as digit-words, are used. In this study we used a standard, widely used, STM assessment: the Digit Span subtest from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. The task was conducted twice: in native language and in second language. As exposure to native language is greater than exposure to second language, we predicted that IDDs' performance in the task administered in native language will reveal a larger group difference as compared to second language, due to IDDs' reduced benefit of item frequency. The prediction was confirmed, in line with the hypothesis that reduced STM in dyslexia to a large extent reflects reduced benefits from long-term item frequency and not a reduced STM per se.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1928-1940"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588939/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141493969","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory. 视觉工作记忆块的主动干扰牵涉到长时记忆。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w
Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan

Visual working memory (VWM) is a limited cognitive resource that can be functionally expanded through chunking (Miller, 1956). For example, participants can hold an increasing number of colours in mind as they learn to chunk reliably paired combinations (Brady et al., 2009). We investigated whether this benefit is mediated through the in situ compression of VWM representations (Brady et al., 2009) or the offloading of chunks to long-term memory (LTM; Huang & Awh, 2018; Ngiam et al., 2019) by asking if a vulnerability of LTM - proactive interference - influences VWM performance. We adapted previous designs using deterministic (Experiment 1, N = 60) and probabilistic pairings (Experiments 2 and 3, N = 64 and 80, respectively), to include colour pairings that swapped in sequence along with pairings that were consistent in sequence. Generally, participants reported colours from consistent pairs more accurately than from swapping pairs, which we designed to drive interference in LTM (Experiments 1 and 2). The error profiles also pointed to proactive interference between swapping pairs in all three experiments. Moreover, participants who had explicit awareness of frequent colour pairings had higher VWM accuracy, and their errors reflected more proactive interference than their unaware counterparts (Experiment 3). This pattern of long-term proactive interference in a VWM task lends support for accounts of VWM chunking that propose LTM offloading.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)是一种有限的认知资源,可以通过分块的方式进行功能扩展(Miller,1956 年)。例如,当参与者学会对可靠的配对组合进行分块时,他们可以记住越来越多的颜色(Brady 等人,2009 年)。我们研究了这种益处是否通过原位压缩VWM表征(Brady等人,2009年)或将分块卸载到长时记忆(LTM;Huang和Awh,2018年;Ngiam等人,2019年)来介导,询问LTM的脆弱性--主动干扰--是否会影响VWM的表现。我们调整了之前使用确定性配对(实验 1,N = 60)和概率性配对(实验 2 和 3,N = 64 和 80)的设计,加入了顺序互换的颜色配对和顺序一致的配对。一般来说,参与者报告一致配对中的颜色比报告交换配对中的颜色更准确,我们设计交换配对的目的是为了在 LTM 中产生干扰(实验 1 和 2)。在所有三个实验中,错误特征也表明交换配对之间存在主动干扰。此外,明确意识到频繁颜色配对的参与者具有更高的 VWM 准确性,他们的错误比未意识到的参与者反映出更多的主动干扰(实验 3)。这种在VWM任务中长期主动干扰的模式为提出LTM卸载的VWM分块理论提供了支持。
{"title":"Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory.","authors":"Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual working memory (VWM) is a limited cognitive resource that can be functionally expanded through chunking (Miller, 1956). For example, participants can hold an increasing number of colours in mind as they learn to chunk reliably paired combinations (Brady et al., 2009). We investigated whether this benefit is mediated through the in situ compression of VWM representations (Brady et al., 2009) or the offloading of chunks to long-term memory (LTM; Huang & Awh, 2018; Ngiam et al., 2019) by asking if a vulnerability of LTM - proactive interference - influences VWM performance. We adapted previous designs using deterministic (Experiment 1, N = 60) and probabilistic pairings (Experiments 2 and 3, N = 64 and 80, respectively), to include colour pairings that swapped in sequence along with pairings that were consistent in sequence. Generally, participants reported colours from consistent pairs more accurately than from swapping pairs, which we designed to drive interference in LTM (Experiments 1 and 2). The error profiles also pointed to proactive interference between swapping pairs in all three experiments. Moreover, participants who had explicit awareness of frequent colour pairings had higher VWM accuracy, and their errors reflected more proactive interference than their unaware counterparts (Experiment 3). This pattern of long-term proactive interference in a VWM task lends support for accounts of VWM chunking that propose LTM offloading.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"2092-2106"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Recognition memory decisions made with short- and long-term retrieval. 通过短期和长期检索做出识别记忆决定。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-09 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01518-7
Shuchun Lea Lai, Rui Cao, Richard M Shiffrin

In the present research, we produce a coherent account of the storage and retrieval processes in short- and long-term event memory, and long-term knowledge, that produce response accuracy and response time in a wide variety of conditions in our studies of recognition memory. Two to nine pictures are studied sequentially followed by a target or foil test picture in four conditions used in Nosofsky et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 316-342, (2021) and in our new paradigm: VM: target and foil responses to a given stimulus change from trial to trial; CM: the responses do not change from trial to trial; AN: every trial uses new stimuli; MIXED: combinations of VM, CN, and AN occur on each trial. In the new paradigm a given picture is equally often tested as old or new, but only in CM is the response key the same and learnable. Our model has components that have appeared in a variety of prior accounts, including learning and familiarity, but are given support by our demonstration that accuracy and response time data from a large variety of conditions can be predicted by these processes acting together, with parameter values that largely are unchanged. A longer version of this article, containing information not found here due to space, is available online  https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/h8msp .The avalibility of the data (supplement materials), info and link is attached at the end section ( https://psyarxiv.com/h8msp .).

在本研究中,我们对短期和长期事件记忆以及长期知识的存储和检索过程进行了连贯的阐述,这些过程在我们的识别记忆研究中产生了各种条件下的反应准确性和反应时间。在诺索夫斯基等人的《实验心理学杂志》(Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 316-342, (2021) 和我们的新范式中使用的四种条件:VM:目标和箔片对给定刺激的反应从一次试验到另一次试验都会发生变化;CM:反应从一次试验到另一次试验都不会发生变化;AN:每次试验都使用新的刺激;MIXED:每次试验都会出现VM、CN和AN的组合。在新范式中,给定图片作为新旧图片进行测试的频率相同,但只有在 CM 范式中,反应键是相同的,并且是可学习的。我们的模型包含了之前各种说法中出现过的成分,包括学习和熟悉性,但我们的研究证明,这些过程一起作用时,参数值基本保持不变,因此可以预测各种条件下的准确率和反应时间数据,从而为我们的模型提供了支持。本文较长的版本因篇幅原因未在此提供,可在线查阅 https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/h8msp 。数据(补充材料)、信息和链接的可用性附在末尾部分 ( https://psyarxiv.com/h8msp .) 。
{"title":"Recognition memory decisions made with short- and long-term retrieval.","authors":"Shuchun Lea Lai, Rui Cao, Richard M Shiffrin","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01518-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01518-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the present research, we produce a coherent account of the storage and retrieval processes in short- and long-term event memory, and long-term knowledge, that produce response accuracy and response time in a wide variety of conditions in our studies of recognition memory. Two to nine pictures are studied sequentially followed by a target or foil test picture in four conditions used in Nosofsky et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 47, 316-342, (2021) and in our new paradigm: VM: target and foil responses to a given stimulus change from trial to trial; CM: the responses do not change from trial to trial; AN: every trial uses new stimuli; MIXED: combinations of VM, CN, and AN occur on each trial. In the new paradigm a given picture is equally often tested as old or new, but only in CM is the response key the same and learnable. Our model has components that have appeared in a variety of prior accounts, including learning and familiarity, but are given support by our demonstration that accuracy and response time data from a large variety of conditions can be predicted by these processes acting together, with parameter values that largely are unchanged. A longer version of this article, containing information not found here due to space, is available online  https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/h8msp .The avalibility of the data (supplement materials), info and link is attached at the end section ( https://psyarxiv.com/h8msp .).</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"2132-2155"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139742384","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does the extension of free time trigger spontaneous elaborative strategies in working memory? 自由时间的延长会触发工作记忆中的自发阐述策略吗?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7
Inès Leproult, Benoît Lemaire, Sophie Portrat

Elaboration has emerged as a potential maintenance mechanism involved in the substantial contribution of long-term memory (LTM) to working memory (WM) performance. The objective of the current study was to determine whether elaborative strategies could be spontaneously implemented under favorable conditions. Across four experiments, the distribution of free-time periods was manipulated in a complex span task, while keeping the total amount of free time and cognitive load constant. As elaboration requires time to be set up, Experiment 1 elicited better WM performance in a condition with fewer long free-time periods compared to a condition with many short free-time periods. However, because this benefit did not persist during delayed recall, the following experiments aimed to further investigate this effect by manipulating factors supposed to modulate elaboration. In Experiment 2, half of the participants received no specific instructions regarding strategies whereas the other half were encouraged to use elaborative strategies. In Experiment 3, the to-be-maintained stimuli did or did not have LTM representations that are essential for elaboration (i.e., words or pseudowords). Finally, the last experiment used a self-strategy report to better understand the nature of the WM maintenance strategies spontaneously employed by participants. Despite a consistent effect of free time manipulation on WM recall, the explanatory assumption of elaboration was challenged by the unexpected lack of effect on LTM recall and on the type of strategy reported. Alternative explanations stemming from well-known factors influencing WM performance are discussed, and emphasis is placed on the potential contribution of direct semantic maintenance in WM.

详细化已成为一种潜在的维持机制,它参与了长时记忆(LTM)对工作记忆(WM)表现的实质性贡献。本研究的目的是确定在有利条件下能否自发地实施详细化策略。在四次实验中,我们在保持自由时间总量和认知负荷不变的情况下,对复杂跨度任务中自由时间的分布进行了操作。由于精心设计需要时间来建立,因此实验 1 在较少长自由时间段的条件下比在较多短自由时间段的条件下获得了更好的 WM 成绩。然而,由于这种优势在延迟回忆中并不持续,因此接下来的实验旨在通过操纵那些被认为会调节详细化的因素来进一步研究这种效应。在实验 2 中,一半的被试没有接受任何具体的策略指导,而另一半的被试则被鼓励使用详细化策略。在实验 3 中,待保留的刺激物是否具有对阐释至关重要的 LTM 表征(即单词或假词)。最后,最后一项实验使用了自我策略报告,以更好地了解参与者自发使用的 WM 维持策略的性质。尽管自由时间操作对 WM 回忆的影响是一致的,但对 LTM 回忆和所报告策略类型的影响却出乎意料地缺乏,这对阐述的解释性假设提出了挑战。我们讨论了影响 WM 表现的众所周知的因素所产生的其他解释,并强调了直接语义维持在 WM 中的潜在贡献。
{"title":"Does the extension of free time trigger spontaneous elaborative strategies in working memory?","authors":"Inès Leproult, Benoît Lemaire, Sophie Portrat","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Elaboration has emerged as a potential maintenance mechanism involved in the substantial contribution of long-term memory (LTM) to working memory (WM) performance. The objective of the current study was to determine whether elaborative strategies could be spontaneously implemented under favorable conditions. Across four experiments, the distribution of free-time periods was manipulated in a complex span task, while keeping the total amount of free time and cognitive load constant. As elaboration requires time to be set up, Experiment 1 elicited better WM performance in a condition with fewer long free-time periods compared to a condition with many short free-time periods. However, because this benefit did not persist during delayed recall, the following experiments aimed to further investigate this effect by manipulating factors supposed to modulate elaboration. In Experiment 2, half of the participants received no specific instructions regarding strategies whereas the other half were encouraged to use elaborative strategies. In Experiment 3, the to-be-maintained stimuli did or did not have LTM representations that are essential for elaboration (i.e., words or pseudowords). Finally, the last experiment used a self-strategy report to better understand the nature of the WM maintenance strategies spontaneously employed by participants. Despite a consistent effect of free time manipulation on WM recall, the explanatory assumption of elaboration was challenged by the unexpected lack of effect on LTM recall and on the type of strategy reported. Alternative explanations stemming from well-known factors influencing WM performance are discussed, and emphasis is placed on the potential contribution of direct semantic maintenance in WM.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"2022-2052"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141917862","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Active maintenance in working memory reinforces bindings for future retrieval from episodic long-term memory. 工作记忆中的主动保持强化了未来从外显长期记忆中检索的绑定。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01596-7
Vanessa M Loaiza, Alessandra S Souza

Many theories assume that actively maintaining information in working memory (WM) predicts its retention in episodic long-term memory (LTM), as revealed by the beneficial effects of more WM time. In four experiments, we examined whether affording more time for intentional WM maintenance does indeed drive LTM. Sequences of four words were presented during trials of simple span (short time), slow span (long time), and complex span (long time with distraction; Experiments 1-2). Long time intervals entailed a pause of equivalent duration between the words that presented a blank screen (slow span) or an arithmetic problem to read aloud and solve (complex span). In Experiments 1-3, participants either serially recalled the words (intentional encoding) or completed a no-recall task (incidental encoding). In Experiment 4, all participants were instructed to intentionally encode the words, with the trials randomly ending in the serial-recall or no-recall task. To ensure similar processing of the words between encoding groups, participants silently decided whether each word was a living or nonliving thing via key press (i.e., an animacy judgment; Experiments 1 and 3-4) or read the words aloud and then pressed the space bar (Experiment 2). A surprise delayed memory test at the end of the experiment assessed LTM. Applying Bayesian cognitive models to disambiguate binding and item memory revealed consistent benefits of free time to binding memory that were specific to intentional encoding in WM. This suggests that time spent intentionally keeping information in WM is special for LTM because WM is a system that maintains bindings.

许多理论都认为,积极保持工作记忆(WM)中的信息可以预测其在外显长期记忆(LTM)中的保持情况,这一点可以从更多的工作记忆时间所产生的有益影响中看出。在四项实验中,我们考察了为有意保持工作记忆提供更多时间是否真的能促进长时记忆。在简单跨度(短时间)、慢速跨度(长时间)和复杂跨度(长时间且有干扰;实验 1-2)的试验中,我们分别呈现了四个单词的序列。长时间隔是指在单词之间有一个时间相等的停顿,即出现空白屏幕(慢跨度)或需要朗读和解决的算术问题(复杂跨度)。在实验 1-3 中,参与者要么连续回忆单词(有意编码),要么完成无回忆任务(偶然编码)。在实验 4 中,所有被试都被要求对单词进行有意编码,试验随机地以连续回忆或不回忆任务结束。为了确保不同编码组对单词的处理过程相似,参与者通过按键默默判断每个单词是生物还是非生物(即动画判断;实验 1 和 3-4),或者大声朗读单词,然后按空格键(实验 2)。实验结束时进行的突击延迟记忆测试评估了LTM。应用贝叶斯认知模型对绑定记忆和项目记忆进行区分,结果发现自由时间对绑定记忆的益处是一致的,这种益处是在 WM 中进行有意编码所特有的。这表明,有意将信息保存在 WM 中的时间对 LTM 有特殊意义,因为 WM 是一个维持绑定的系统。
{"title":"Active maintenance in working memory reinforces bindings for future retrieval from episodic long-term memory.","authors":"Vanessa M Loaiza, Alessandra S Souza","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01596-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01596-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Many theories assume that actively maintaining information in working memory (WM) predicts its retention in episodic long-term memory (LTM), as revealed by the beneficial effects of more WM time. In four experiments, we examined whether affording more time for intentional WM maintenance does indeed drive LTM. Sequences of four words were presented during trials of simple span (short time), slow span (long time), and complex span (long time with distraction; Experiments 1-2). Long time intervals entailed a pause of equivalent duration between the words that presented a blank screen (slow span) or an arithmetic problem to read aloud and solve (complex span). In Experiments 1-3, participants either serially recalled the words (intentional encoding) or completed a no-recall task (incidental encoding). In Experiment 4, all participants were instructed to intentionally encode the words, with the trials randomly ending in the serial-recall or no-recall task. To ensure similar processing of the words between encoding groups, participants silently decided whether each word was a living or nonliving thing via key press (i.e., an animacy judgment; Experiments 1 and 3-4) or read the words aloud and then pressed the space bar (Experiment 2). A surprise delayed memory test at the end of the experiment assessed LTM. Applying Bayesian cognitive models to disambiguate binding and item memory revealed consistent benefits of free time to binding memory that were specific to intentional encoding in WM. This suggests that time spent intentionally keeping information in WM is special for LTM because WM is a system that maintains bindings.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1999-2021"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588845/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141538705","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Semantic meaning enhances feature-binding but not quantity or precision of locations in visual working memory. 语义能增强视觉工作记忆中的特征结合,但不能增强位置的数量或精确度。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-30 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01611-x
Tomer Sahar, Nurit Gronau, Tal Makovski

Recent studies showed that real-world items are better remembered in visual working memory (VWM) than visually similar stimuli that are stripped of their semantic meaning. However, the exact nature of this advantage remains unclear. We used meaningful and meaningless stimuli in a location-reproduction VWM task. Employing a mixture-modeling analysis, we examined whether semantic meaning enables more item locations to be remembered, whether it improves the precision of the locations stored in memory, or whether it improves binding between the specific items and their locations. Participants were presented with streams of four (Experiments 1 & 2) or six (Experiment 3) real-world items, or their scrambled, meaningless counterparts. Each item was presented at a unique location, and the task was to reproduce one item's location. Overall, location memory was consistently better for real-world items compared with their scrambled counterparts. Furthermore, the results revealed that participants were less likely to make swap errors for the meaningful items, but there was no effect of conceptual meaning on the guess rate or the precision of the report. In line with previous findings, these results indicate that conceptual meaning enhances VWM for arbitrary stimulus properties such as item location, and this improvement is primarily due to a more efficient identity-location binding rather than an increase in the quantity or quality (precision) of the locations held in memory.

最近的研究表明,在视觉工作记忆(VWM)中,现实世界中的项目比去除了语义的视觉相似刺激物记忆效果更好。然而,这种优势的确切性质仍不清楚。我们在一项位置再现视觉工作记忆任务中使用了有意义和无意义的刺激物。通过混合模型分析,我们考察了语义是否能使更多的项目位置被记住,是否能提高记忆中存储位置的精确度,或者是否能提高特定项目与其位置之间的结合。参与者会看到由四个(实验 1 和 2)或六个(实验 3)真实世界项目组成的数据流,或者是它们的无意义混淆对应物。每个项目都呈现在一个独特的位置,任务是再现一个项目的位置。总体而言,真实物品的位置记忆始终优于乱码物品。此外,研究结果表明,对于有意义的项目,参与者较少出现交换错误,但概念意义对猜测率和报告的精确度没有影响。与之前的研究结果一致,这些结果表明概念意义增强了对任意刺激属性(如项目位置)的VWM,而这种改善主要是由于更有效的身份-位置结合,而不是记忆中位置的数量或质量(精确度)的提高。
{"title":"Semantic meaning enhances feature-binding but not quantity or precision of locations in visual working memory.","authors":"Tomer Sahar, Nurit Gronau, Tal Makovski","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01611-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01611-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Recent studies showed that real-world items are better remembered in visual working memory (VWM) than visually similar stimuli that are stripped of their semantic meaning. However, the exact nature of this advantage remains unclear. We used meaningful and meaningless stimuli in a location-reproduction VWM task. Employing a mixture-modeling analysis, we examined whether semantic meaning enables more item locations to be remembered, whether it improves the precision of the locations stored in memory, or whether it improves binding between the specific items and their locations. Participants were presented with streams of four (Experiments 1 & 2) or six (Experiment 3) real-world items, or their scrambled, meaningless counterparts. Each item was presented at a unique location, and the task was to reproduce one item's location. Overall, location memory was consistently better for real-world items compared with their scrambled counterparts. Furthermore, the results revealed that participants were less likely to make swap errors for the meaningful items, but there was no effect of conceptual meaning on the guess rate or the precision of the report. In line with previous findings, these results indicate that conceptual meaning enhances VWM for arbitrary stimulus properties such as item location, and this improvement is primarily due to a more efficient identity-location binding rather than an increase in the quantity or quality (precision) of the locations held in memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"2107-2118"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588879/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141856761","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Neighborhood frequency effects in simple and complex span: Do high-frequency neighbors help or hurt? 简单和复杂跨度中的邻频效应:高频邻近地区是有帮助还是有害?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2024-11-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01658-w
Molly B MacMillan, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys

A word's orthographic neighborhood is the set of words that differ from the target word by one letter. Both Roodenrys (2009) and Robert et al. (Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 119-125, 2015) posit that orthographic neighbors are activated when the target word is encountered in tasks such as simple and complex span. The two accounts differ in that the former predicts a beneficial effect of this activation, because it produces feedback activation that helps redintegrate the target word, whereas the latter predicts a detrimental effect, because the need to overcome the greater interference from the larger number of higher-frequency items reduces the processing resources available. Four experiments assess the predictions of these two accounts. Experiments 1 and 2 found a beneficial effect of having a higher- compared with a lower-frequency neighborhood in both a simple and a complex span task. Experiments 3 and 4 found no detrimental effect of having one or more neighbors with higher frequency than the target in both a simple and complex span task. Implications for the two theories are discussed.

单词的正字法邻域是指与目标单词相差一个字母的单词集合。Roodenrys(2009 年)和 Robert 等人(《心理语言学研究杂志》,44, 119-125, 2015 年)都认为,在简单和复杂跨度等任务中遇到目标词时,正字法邻域会被激活。这两种说法的不同之处在于,前者预测这种激活会产生有益的效果,因为它产生的反馈激活有助于重新整合目标词;而后者预测会产生有害的效果,因为需要克服来自大量高频项目的更大干扰,从而减少了可用的处理资源。四个实验对这两种说法的预测进行了评估。实验 1 和 2 发现,在简单和复杂的跨度任务中,高频邻域比低频邻域具有有利影响。实验 3 和 4 发现,在简单和复杂跨度任务中,有一个或多个比目标频率更高的邻域不会产生不利影响。本文讨论了这两种理论的意义。
{"title":"Neighborhood frequency effects in simple and complex span: Do high-frequency neighbors help or hurt?","authors":"Molly B MacMillan, Ian Neath, Steven Roodenrys","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01658-w","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01658-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A word's orthographic neighborhood is the set of words that differ from the target word by one letter. Both Roodenrys (2009) and Robert et al. (Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 44, 119-125, 2015) posit that orthographic neighbors are activated when the target word is encountered in tasks such as simple and complex span. The two accounts differ in that the former predicts a beneficial effect of this activation, because it produces feedback activation that helps redintegrate the target word, whereas the latter predicts a detrimental effect, because the need to overcome the greater interference from the larger number of higher-frequency items reduces the processing resources available. Four experiments assess the predictions of these two accounts. Experiments 1 and 2 found a beneficial effect of having a higher- compared with a lower-frequency neighborhood in both a simple and a complex span task. Experiments 3 and 4 found no detrimental effect of having one or more neighbors with higher frequency than the target in both a simple and complex span task. Implications for the two theories are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1871-1881"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588882/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142584368","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Delayed memory for complex visual stimuli does not benefit from distraction during encoding. 复杂视觉刺激的延迟记忆不会受益于编码过程中的分心。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-11-01 Epub Date: 2023-09-29 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01471-x
Lea M Bartsch, Philipp Musfeld

The covert retrieval model (McCabe, Journal of Memory and Language 58(2), 480-494, 2008) postulates that delayed memory performance is enhanced when the encoding of memoranda in working memory (WM) is interrupted by distraction. When subjects are asked to remember stimuli for an immediate memory test, they usually remember them better when the items are presented without distraction, compared to a condition in which a distraction occurs following each item. In a delayed memory test, this effect has been shown to be reversed: Memory performance is better for items followed by distraction than without. Yet, this so-called McCabe effect has not been consistently replicated in the past. In an extensive replication attempt of a previous study showing the effect for complex visual stimuli, we investigated five potential boundary conditions of the predictions of the covert retrieval model: (1) Type of Stimuli (doors vs. faces), (2) type of distractor (pictures vs. math equations), (3) expectation about task difficulty (mixed vs. blocked lists), (4) memory load in WM (small vs. large), and (5) expectation about the long-term memory (LTM) test (intentional vs. incidental encoding). Across four experiments we failed to replicate the original findings and show that delayed memory for faces and other complex visual stimuli does not benefit from covert retrieval during encoding - as suggested as being induced by distractors. Our results indicate that the transfer of information from WM to LTM does not seem to be influenced by covert retrieval processes, but rather that a fixed proportion of information is laid down as a more permanent trace.

隐蔽检索模型(McCabe,Journal of Memory and Language 58(2),480-492008)假设,当工作记忆(WM)中备忘录的编码因分心而中断时,延迟记忆性能会增强。当受试者被要求在即时记忆测试中记住刺激时,与每个项目后都会分心的情况相比,当项目在没有分心的情况下呈现时,他们通常会更好地记住它们。在延迟记忆测试中,这种影响被证明是相反的:分心后的项目比没有分心的项目记忆表现更好。然而,这种所谓的麦凯布效应在过去并没有得到一贯的复制。在对先前一项显示复杂视觉刺激效果的研究进行广泛复制的尝试中,我们研究了隐蔽检索模型预测的五个潜在边界条件:(1)刺激类型(门与脸),(2)干扰物类型(图片与数学方程),(3)对任务难度的期望(混合列表与封锁列表),(4)WM中的记忆负载(小与大),以及(5)对长期记忆(LTM)测试的期望(有意与偶然编码)。在四个实验中,我们未能复制最初的发现,并表明面部和其他复杂视觉刺激的延迟记忆不会从编码过程中的隐蔽检索中受益——这被认为是由干扰物引起的。我们的结果表明,从WM到LTM的信息传递似乎不受隐蔽检索过程的影响,而是固定比例的信息被作为更永久的轨迹。
{"title":"Delayed memory for complex visual stimuli does not benefit from distraction during encoding.","authors":"Lea M Bartsch, Philipp Musfeld","doi":"10.3758/s13421-023-01471-x","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-023-01471-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The covert retrieval model (McCabe, Journal of Memory and Language 58(2), 480-494, 2008) postulates that delayed memory performance is enhanced when the encoding of memoranda in working memory (WM) is interrupted by distraction. When subjects are asked to remember stimuli for an immediate memory test, they usually remember them better when the items are presented without distraction, compared to a condition in which a distraction occurs following each item. In a delayed memory test, this effect has been shown to be reversed: Memory performance is better for items followed by distraction than without. Yet, this so-called McCabe effect has not been consistently replicated in the past. In an extensive replication attempt of a previous study showing the effect for complex visual stimuli, we investigated five potential boundary conditions of the predictions of the covert retrieval model: (1) Type of Stimuli (doors vs. faces), (2) type of distractor (pictures vs. math equations), (3) expectation about task difficulty (mixed vs. blocked lists), (4) memory load in WM (small vs. large), and (5) expectation about the long-term memory (LTM) test (intentional vs. incidental encoding). Across four experiments we failed to replicate the original findings and show that delayed memory for faces and other complex visual stimuli does not benefit from covert retrieval during encoding - as suggested as being induced by distractors. Our results indicate that the transfer of information from WM to LTM does not seem to be influenced by covert retrieval processes, but rather that a fixed proportion of information is laid down as a more permanent trace.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"1833-1851"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-11-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11588953/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41154351","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory & Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1