首页 > 最新文献

Memory & Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Examining the time course of post collaborative benefits across word lists and prose passages. 研究合作后在单词表和散文段落中获益的时间过程。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-26 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01609-5
Yunfeng Wei, Brooke Z Charbonneau, Michelle L Meade, Keith A Hutchison

In the current study, we investigated how long the effects of one single collaboration session continue to influence individual memory. Participants learned categorized word lists and prose passages individually, and then they were instructed to recall learned materials either collaboratively or individually. Following initial recall, participants completed an individual recall test after a delay of 5 min, 48 h, or 1 week. On the initial recall test, we found that collaboration reduced recall of correct items on both word lists and prose passages (collaborative inhibition), and that collaboration reduced false recall on both word lists and prose passages (error correction). However, on the subsequent individual memory test after a delay, the pattern of post collaborative effects differed across veridical and false recall. For both word lists and prose passages, post collaborative benefits on correct recall lasted 1 week. However, there were no lasting effects of error correction on subsequent false recall. These results suggest that the time course of post collaborative benefits can be long lasting, but they are selective to veridical recall. The results are explained by theories of reexposure and error correction.

在本研究中,我们调查了单次协作的效果会在多长时间内持续影响个体记忆。受试者单独学习了分类词表和散文段落,然后他们被要求以合作或单独的方式回忆已学过的材料。初次回忆后,受试者分别在延迟5分钟、48小时或1周后完成个人回忆测试。在最初的回忆测试中,我们发现合作减少了对单词表和散文段落中正确项目的回忆(合作抑制),合作减少了对单词表和散文段落中错误项目的回忆(错误纠正)。然而,在延迟后进行的个人记忆测试中,协作后效应的模式在正确记忆和错误记忆中有所不同。对于单词表和散文段落,合作后对正确记忆的益处持续了一周。然而,错误纠正对随后的错误回忆没有持续的影响。这些结果表明,合作后的益处可以持续很长时间,但它们对正确回忆是有选择性的。重新暴露和错误纠正理论可以解释这些结果。
{"title":"Examining the time course of post collaborative benefits across word lists and prose passages.","authors":"Yunfeng Wei, Brooke Z Charbonneau, Michelle L Meade, Keith A Hutchison","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01609-5","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01609-5","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>In the current study, we investigated how long the effects of one single collaboration session continue to influence individual memory. Participants learned categorized word lists and prose passages individually, and then they were instructed to recall learned materials either collaboratively or individually. Following initial recall, participants completed an individual recall test after a delay of 5 min, 48 h, or 1 week. On the initial recall test, we found that collaboration reduced recall of correct items on both word lists and prose passages (collaborative inhibition), and that collaboration reduced false recall on both word lists and prose passages (error correction). However, on the subsequent individual memory test after a delay, the pattern of post collaborative effects differed across veridical and false recall. For both word lists and prose passages, post collaborative benefits on correct recall lasted 1 week. However, there were no lasting effects of error correction on subsequent false recall. These results suggest that the time course of post collaborative benefits can be long lasting, but they are selective to veridical recall. The results are explained by theories of reexposure and error correction.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-26","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141767644","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Errorful learning of trivia questions and answers: The role of study time. 错误地学习小问题和答案:学习时间的作用
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-25 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01608-6
Ewa Butowska-Buczyńska, Maciej Hanczakowski, Katarzyna Zawadzka

Errorful learning-asking questions and forcing responding even before the correct answers are presented for study-has recently been proposed as a way of maximizing the effectiveness of study. However, much support for the superiority of errorful learning over standard learning via reading comes from studies employing pairs of words as study materials, which remain of little educational relevance. Studies using materials affording richer semantic processing, such as trivia questions and their answers, have shown benefits of errorful learning only when the errorful learning condition is granted additional time for formulating guesses. In the present study, we systematically examined the role of timing when comparing errorful learning and reading strategies applied to study of trivia questions and their answers. In Experiments 1 and 2, we obtained evidence for the superiority of errorful learning over reading when additional time was given to formulate guesses, but this superiority was abolished when the overall time to study was equated between the two learning strategies. We further examined the role of answer familiarity in Experiment 3, showing that incorrect guessing produced no benefit for learning regardless of whether the to-be-learned concepts were familiar or not. In Experiments 4 and 5, no benefits of errorful learning emerged when participants were required to guess responses to two different questions that shared a common set of possible answers. We conclude that the benefits of errorful learning for trivia questions emerge only when guessing gives more time to process target questions.

最近,有人提出了错误学习法,即在学习正确答案之前就提出问题并强迫回答,以此来最大限度地提高学习效率。然而,错误学习优于通过阅读进行的标准学习的观点,大多来自于使用成对单词作为学习材料的研究,而这些材料的教育意义仍然不大。使用能提供更丰富语义加工的材料(如琐事问题及其答案)进行的研究表明,只有在错误学习条件下给予错误学习者更多的时间来形成猜测时,错误学习才会带来益处。在本研究中,我们系统地考察了在比较错误学习策略和阅读策略在小问题及其答案研究中的应用时,时间的作用。在实验一和实验二中,我们得到的证据表明,当给予额外的时间来形成猜测时,错误学习优于阅读,但当两种学习策略的总体学习时间相等时,这种优越性就不复存在了。我们在实验 3 中进一步研究了答案熟悉程度的作用,结果表明,无论要学习的概念是否熟悉,错误的猜测对学习都没有好处。在实验 4 和 5 中,当要求被试猜测两个不同问题的答案时,错误学习并没有带来任何益处。我们的结论是,只有当猜测给参与者提供了更多的时间来处理目标问题时,错误学习才会给琐事问题带来益处。
{"title":"Errorful learning of trivia questions and answers: The role of study time.","authors":"Ewa Butowska-Buczyńska, Maciej Hanczakowski, Katarzyna Zawadzka","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01608-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01608-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Errorful learning-asking questions and forcing responding even before the correct answers are presented for study-has recently been proposed as a way of maximizing the effectiveness of study. However, much support for the superiority of errorful learning over standard learning via reading comes from studies employing pairs of words as study materials, which remain of little educational relevance. Studies using materials affording richer semantic processing, such as trivia questions and their answers, have shown benefits of errorful learning only when the errorful learning condition is granted additional time for formulating guesses. In the present study, we systematically examined the role of timing when comparing errorful learning and reading strategies applied to study of trivia questions and their answers. In Experiments 1 and 2, we obtained evidence for the superiority of errorful learning over reading when additional time was given to formulate guesses, but this superiority was abolished when the overall time to study was equated between the two learning strategies. We further examined the role of answer familiarity in Experiment 3, showing that incorrect guessing produced no benefit for learning regardless of whether the to-be-learned concepts were familiar or not. In Experiments 4 and 5, no benefits of errorful learning emerged when participants were required to guess responses to two different questions that shared a common set of possible answers. We conclude that the benefits of errorful learning for trivia questions emerge only when guessing gives more time to process target questions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141767643","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Inside Ockham's razor: A mechanism driving preferences for simpler explanations. 奥卡姆剃刀的内部:更简单解释的驱动机制。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01604-w
Thalia H Vrantsidis, Tania Lombrozo

People often prefer simpler explanations, defined as those that posit the presence of fewer causes (e.g., positing the presence of a single cause, Cause A, rather than two causes, Causes B and C, to explain observed effects). Here, we test one hypothesis about the mechanisms underlying this preference: that people tend to reason as if they are using "agnostic" explanations, which remain neutral about the presence/absence of additional causes (e.g., comparing "A" vs. "B and C," while remaining neutral about the status of B and C when considering "A," or of A when considering "B and C"), even in cases where "atheist" explanations, which specify the absence of additional causes (e.g., "A and not B or C" vs. "B and C and not A"), are more appropriate. Three studies with US-based samples (total N = 982) tested this idea by using scenarios for which agnostic and atheist strategies produce diverging simplicity/complexity preferences, and asking participants to compare explanations provided in atheist form. Results suggest that people tend to ignore absent causes, thus overgeneralizing agnostic strategies, which can produce preferences for simpler explanations even when the complex explanation is objectively more probable. However, these unwarranted preferences were reduced by manipulations that encouraged participants to consider absent causes: making absences necessary to produce the effects (Study 2), or describing absences as causes that produce alternative effects (Study 3). These results shed light on the mechanisms driving preferences for simpler explanations, and on when these mechanisms are likely to lead people astray.

人们通常更喜欢简单的解释,即假设存在较少原因的解释(例如,假设存在单一原因 A,而不是两个原因 B 和 C,来解释观察到的效果)。在此,我们将验证有关这种偏好机制的一个假设:人们倾向于使用 "不可知论 "解释来进行推理,这种解释对额外原因的存在/不存在保持中立(例如,比较 "A "和 "C")、比较 "A "与 "B 和 C",同时在考虑 "A "时对 B 和 C 的地位保持中立,或在考虑 "B 和 C "时对 A 的地位保持中立),甚至在 "无神论 "解释更合适的情况下也是如此,"无神论 "解释明确指出没有其他原因(例如,"A 而不是 B 或 C "与 "B 和 C 而不是 A")。三项以美国为样本的研究(总人数 = 982)通过使用不可知论和无神论策略产生不同的简单性/复杂性偏好的情景,并要求参与者比较以无神论形式提供的解释,对这一观点进行了检验。结果表明,人们倾向于忽略不存在的原因,从而过度概括不可知论策略,即使复杂的解释客观上更有可能,人们也会产生对简单解释的偏好。然而,通过鼓励参与者考虑缺失原因的操作,这些不必要的偏好得到了缓解:将缺失作为产生效果的必要条件(研究 2),或将缺失描述为产生替代效果的原因(研究 3)。这些结果揭示了偏好更简单解释的驱动机制,以及这些机制何时可能将人们引入歧途。
{"title":"Inside Ockham's razor: A mechanism driving preferences for simpler explanations.","authors":"Thalia H Vrantsidis, Tania Lombrozo","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01604-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01604-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>People often prefer simpler explanations, defined as those that posit the presence of fewer causes (e.g., positing the presence of a single cause, Cause A, rather than two causes, Causes B and C, to explain observed effects). Here, we test one hypothesis about the mechanisms underlying this preference: that people tend to reason as if they are using \"agnostic\" explanations, which remain neutral about the presence/absence of additional causes (e.g., comparing \"A\" vs. \"B and C,\" while remaining neutral about the status of B and C when considering \"A,\" or of A when considering \"B and C\"), even in cases where \"atheist\" explanations, which specify the absence of additional causes (e.g., \"A and not B or C\" vs. \"B and C and not A\"), are more appropriate. Three studies with US-based samples (total N = 982) tested this idea by using scenarios for which agnostic and atheist strategies produce diverging simplicity/complexity preferences, and asking participants to compare explanations provided in atheist form. Results suggest that people tend to ignore absent causes, thus overgeneralizing agnostic strategies, which can produce preferences for simpler explanations even when the complex explanation is objectively more probable. However, these unwarranted preferences were reduced by manipulations that encouraged participants to consider absent causes: making absences necessary to produce the effects (Study 2), or describing absences as causes that produce alternative effects (Study 3). These results shed light on the mechanisms driving preferences for simpler explanations, and on when these mechanisms are likely to lead people astray.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761785","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Selectively maintaining an object's feature in visual working memory: A comparison between highly discriminable and fine-grained features. 在视觉工作记忆中选择性地保持物体特征:高辨别度特征与细粒度特征之间的比较。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01612-w
Wei Chen, Shujuan Ye, Xiaowei Ding, Mowei Shen, Zaifeng Gao

Selectively maintaining information is an essential function of visual working memory (VWM). Recent VWM studies have mainly focused on selective maintenance of objects, leaving the mechanisms of selectively maintaining an object's feature in VWM unknown. Based on the interactive model of perception and VWM, we hypothesized that there are distinct selective maintenance mechanisms for objects containing fine-grained features versus objects containing highly discriminable features. To test this hypothesis, we first required participants to memorize a dual-feature object (colored simple shapes vs. colored polygons), and informed them about the target feature via a retro-cue. Then a visual search task was added to examine the fate of the irrelevant feature. The selective maintenance of an object's feature predicted that the irrelevant feature should be removed from the active state of VWM and should not capture attention when presented as a distractor in the visual search task. We found that irrelevant simple shapes impaired performance in the visual search task (Experiment 1). However, irrelevant polygons did not affect visual search performance (Experiment 2), and this could not be explained by decay of polygons (Experiment 3) or by polygons not capturing attention (Experiment 4). These findings suggest that VWM adopts dissociable mechanisms to selectively maintain an object's feature, depending on the feature's perceptual characteristics.

选择性保持信息是视觉工作记忆(VWM)的一项基本功能。最近的视觉工作记忆研究主要集中在对物体的选择性保持上,而对物体特征在视觉工作记忆中的选择性保持机制尚不清楚。基于感知和视觉工作记忆的交互模型,我们假设,对于包含细粒度特征的物体和包含高度可分辨特征的物体,存在不同的选择性保持机制。为了验证这一假设,我们首先要求被试记忆一个具有双重特征的物体(彩色的简单形状与彩色的多边形),并通过回溯线索告知他们目标特征。然后,我们增加了一项视觉搜索任务,以考察无关特征的命运。根据对物体特征选择性保持的预测,无关特征应从 VWM 的活跃状态中移除,并且在视觉搜索任务中作为干扰物出现时不会吸引注意力。我们发现,无关的简单形状会影响视觉搜索任务的成绩(实验 1)。然而,不相关的多边形并不影响视觉搜索成绩(实验 2),这不能用多边形的衰减(实验 3)或多边形不吸引注意力(实验 4)来解释。这些发现表明,VWM 采用了可分离的机制来选择性地保持物体的特征,这取决于该特征的知觉特性。
{"title":"Selectively maintaining an object's feature in visual working memory: A comparison between highly discriminable and fine-grained features.","authors":"Wei Chen, Shujuan Ye, Xiaowei Ding, Mowei Shen, Zaifeng Gao","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01612-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01612-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Selectively maintaining information is an essential function of visual working memory (VWM). Recent VWM studies have mainly focused on selective maintenance of objects, leaving the mechanisms of selectively maintaining an object's feature in VWM unknown. Based on the interactive model of perception and VWM, we hypothesized that there are distinct selective maintenance mechanisms for objects containing fine-grained features versus objects containing highly discriminable features. To test this hypothesis, we first required participants to memorize a dual-feature object (colored simple shapes vs. colored polygons), and informed them about the target feature via a retro-cue. Then a visual search task was added to examine the fate of the irrelevant feature. The selective maintenance of an object's feature predicted that the irrelevant feature should be removed from the active state of VWM and should not capture attention when presented as a distractor in the visual search task. We found that irrelevant simple shapes impaired performance in the visual search task (Experiment 1). However, irrelevant polygons did not affect visual search performance (Experiment 2), and this could not be explained by decay of polygons (Experiment 3) or by polygons not capturing attention (Experiment 4). These findings suggest that VWM adopts dissociable mechanisms to selectively maintain an object's feature, depending on the feature's perceptual characteristics.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141761786","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Load effect of visual working memory on distractor interference: An investigation with two replication experiments. 视觉工作记忆对分心干扰的负载效应:通过两次重复实验进行调查。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-22 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01610-y
Hasan Gunduz, Arzu Ozkan Ceylan

Konstantinou et al. (Experiment 1B; Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1985-1997, 2014) reported that an increase in visual short-term memory (VSTM) load reduced distractor interference in the flanker task. Yao et al. (Experiment 3; Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3291-3313, 2020) replicated the design of Konstantinou et al.'s experiment and showed that the VSTM load did not modulate the distractor interference effect, contradicting the original findings. However, it is unknown whether differences in task-design between the two experiments contributed to the inconsistent results. Therefore, we first replicated the original two studies with Experiment 1 (N = 54) and Experiment 2 (N = 54) and performed a statistical comparison between the data from these two experiments. In a third experiment (N = 28), we incorporated articulatory suppression into the design to exclude possible effects of verbalization. According to the ANOVA analyses, the VSTM load did not change the level of distractor interference in all three experiments, indicating that differences in task design alone do not explain the inconsistency.

Konstantinou等人(实验1B;《注意、知觉与心理物理学》,76,1985-1997,2014年)报告说,视觉短时记忆(VSTM)负荷的增加减少了侧翼任务中的分心干扰。Yao等人(实验3;Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3291-3313, 2020)复制了Konstantinou等人的实验设计,结果表明VSTM负荷并没有调节分心干扰效应,这与最初的研究结果相矛盾。然而,两次实验中任务设计的不同是否导致了结果的不一致,目前还不得而知。因此,我们首先用实验 1(N = 54)和实验 2(N = 54)复制了原来的两项研究,并对这两项实验的数据进行了统计比较。在第三个实验(人数= 28)中,我们在设计中加入了发音抑制,以排除口头表达可能产生的影响。根据方差分析,在所有三个实验中,VSTM 负载并没有改变分心干扰的水平,这表明任务设计的差异本身并不能解释这种不一致性。
{"title":"Load effect of visual working memory on distractor interference: An investigation with two replication experiments.","authors":"Hasan Gunduz, Arzu Ozkan Ceylan","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01610-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01610-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Konstantinou et al. (Experiment 1B; Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 76, 1985-1997, 2014) reported that an increase in visual short-term memory (VSTM) load reduced distractor interference in the flanker task. Yao et al. (Experiment 3; Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 82, 3291-3313, 2020) replicated the design of Konstantinou et al.'s experiment and showed that the VSTM load did not modulate the distractor interference effect, contradicting the original findings. However, it is unknown whether differences in task-design between the two experiments contributed to the inconsistent results. Therefore, we first replicated the original two studies with Experiment 1 (N = 54) and Experiment 2 (N = 54) and performed a statistical comparison between the data from these two experiments. In a third experiment (N = 28), we incorporated articulatory suppression into the design to exclude possible effects of verbalization. According to the ANOVA analyses, the VSTM load did not change the level of distractor interference in all three experiments, indicating that differences in task design alone do not explain the inconsistency.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141749355","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Detecting the veracity of children's experiences through drawings. 通过绘画检测儿童经历的真实性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6
Jennifer L Briere, Tobi Patkau-Ceh, Tammy A Marche

Research is lacking regarding adults' ability to determine whether children's drawings are based on an experience or not. Drawings are useful in professional settings to alleviate linguistic demands, facilitate memory, and have been used as evidence. Determining the accuracy of veracity assessments of children's drawings would inform professionals regarding their use as evidence of experiences. Twenty-eight children (14 younger, Mage = 7.53 years, SDage = 1.19; 14 older, Mage = 11.67 years, SD = 1.27) produced drawings of two events: one staged experienced, and one narrative-based not experienced event. Fifty (Study 1, Mage = 23.72 years, SDage = 9.70) and 63 (Study 2, Mage = 25.92, SDage = 12.79) adults indicated whether each drawing was based on experience and their confidence in each assessment. In Study 2, additional drawing quality assessments were collected. Results indicated that adults were more accurate at distinguishing experienced than not experienced drawings for older artists. An inverse relationship was observed between confidence and accuracy-participants were more confident when they were inaccurate, especially for younger artists. Drawing quality improved with age and for drawings of experienced events. Adults tended to rate drawings of higher quality as resulting from experience leading to the highest accuracy for drawings from older artists that were based on experience. Overall, results suggest that there may be some features of drawings that allow for above chance levels of accuracy (up to 75%). However, rates are not high enough across assessments (M = 53.93%, range: 39%-75%) to reliably use them as indicators of experience.

关于成人判断儿童绘画是否基于经历的能力还缺乏研究。绘画在专业环境中非常有用,可以减轻语言要求,促进记忆,并被用作证据。确定对儿童绘画的真实性评估的准确性将为专业人员提供有关将其用作经历证据的信息。28 名儿童(14 名年龄较小,年龄 = 7.53 岁,平均年龄 = 1.19 岁;14 名年龄较大,年龄 = 11.67 岁,平均年龄 = 1.27 岁)绘制了两个事件的图画:一个是经历过的事件,另一个是没有经历过的事件。50名(研究 1,年龄 = 23.72 岁,SD 年龄 = 9.70)和 63 名(研究 2,年龄 = 25.92 岁,SD 年龄 = 12.79)成人分别指出了每幅绘画是否基于经验,以及他们对每次评估的信心。在研究 2 中,还收集了额外的绘画质量评估。结果表明,对于年长的艺术家来说,成人能更准确地区分有经验的绘画作品和没有经验的绘画作品。在自信心和准确性之间发现了一种反比关系--参与者在不准确时更自信,尤其是年轻的艺术家。绘画质量随着年龄的增长而提高,经验事件的绘画质量也随之提高。成人倾向于评价因经验而绘制的画作质量更高,因此年长艺术家根据经验绘制的画作准确率最高。总体而言,结果表明,绘画的某些特征可能会使准确率高于偶然水平(高达 75%)。然而,在所有评估中,准确率都不够高(M = 53.93%,范围:39%-75%),因此不能可靠地将其作为经验指标。
{"title":"Detecting the veracity of children's experiences through drawings.","authors":"Jennifer L Briere, Tobi Patkau-Ceh, Tammy A Marche","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research is lacking regarding adults' ability to determine whether children's drawings are based on an experience or not. Drawings are useful in professional settings to alleviate linguistic demands, facilitate memory, and have been used as evidence. Determining the accuracy of veracity assessments of children's drawings would inform professionals regarding their use as evidence of experiences. Twenty-eight children (14 younger, M<sub>age</sub> = 7.53 years, SD<sub>age</sub> = 1.19; 14 older, M<sub>age</sub> = 11.67 years, SD = 1.27) produced drawings of two events: one staged experienced, and one narrative-based not experienced event. Fifty (Study 1, M<sub>age</sub> = 23.72 years, SD<sub>age</sub> = 9.70) and 63 (Study 2, M<sub>age</sub> = 25.92, SD<sub>age</sub> = 12.79) adults indicated whether each drawing was based on experience and their confidence in each assessment. In Study 2, additional drawing quality assessments were collected. Results indicated that adults were more accurate at distinguishing experienced than not experienced drawings for older artists. An inverse relationship was observed between confidence and accuracy-participants were more confident when they were inaccurate, especially for younger artists. Drawing quality improved with age and for drawings of experienced events. Adults tended to rate drawings of higher quality as resulting from experience leading to the highest accuracy for drawings from older artists that were based on experience. Overall, results suggest that there may be some features of drawings that allow for above chance levels of accuracy (up to 75%). However, rates are not high enough across assessments (M = 53.93%, range: 39%-75%) to reliably use them as indicators of experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Examining the role of stimulus complexity in item and associative memory. 研究刺激复杂性在项目记忆和联想记忆中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01590-z
Ricarda Endemann, Siri-Maria Kamp

Episodic memory comprises memory for individual information units (item memory) and for the connections among them (associative memory). In two experiments using an object pair learning task, we examined the effect of visual stimulus complexity on memory encoding and retrieval mechanisms and on item and associative memory performance. Subjects encoded pairs of black monochrome object images (low complexity, LC condition) or color photographs of objects (high complexity, HC condition) via interactive imagery, and subsequently item and associative recognition were tested. In Experiment 1, event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed an enhanced frontal N2 during encoding and an enhanced late posterior negativity (LPN) during item recognition in the HC condition, suggesting that memory traces containing visually more complex objects elicited a stronger effort in reconstructing the past episode. Item memory was consistently superior in the HC compared to the LC condition. Associative memory was either statistically unaffected by complexity (Experiment 1) or improved (Experiment 2) in the HC condition, speaking against a tradeoff between resources allocated to item versus associative memory, and hence contradicting results of some prior studies. In Experiment 2, in both young and older adults, both item and associative memory benefitted from stimulus complexity, such that the magnitude of the age-related associative deficit was not influenced by stimulus complexity. Together, these results suggest that if familiar objects are presented in a form that exhibits a higher visual complexity, which may support semantic processing, complexity can benefit both item and associative memory. Stimulus properties that enhance item memory can scaffold associative memory in this situation.

外显记忆包括对单个信息单元的记忆(项目记忆)和对它们之间联系的记忆(联想记忆)。在两个使用物体配对学习任务的实验中,我们考察了视觉刺激复杂性对记忆编码和检索机制以及对项目记忆和联想记忆表现的影响。受试者通过交互式图像对黑色单色物体图像(低复杂度,LC 条件)或彩色物体照片(高复杂度,HC 条件)进行编码,然后测试项目识别和联想识别。在实验 1 中,事件相关电位(ERPs)显示,在 HC 条件下,编码时的额叶 N2 增强,而在项目识别时的晚期后部负性(LPN)增强,这表明包含视觉上更复杂物体的记忆痕迹在重建过去的情节时会引起更大的努力。与 LC 条件相比,HC 条件下的项目记忆始终较好。在高频条件下,联想记忆在统计学上不受复杂性的影响(实验 1)或得到改善(实验 2),这说明在分配给项目记忆和联想记忆的资源之间存在权衡,因此与之前的一些研究结果相矛盾。在实验 2 中,无论是年轻人还是老年人,项目记忆和联想记忆都受益于刺激的复杂性,因此与年龄相关的联想记忆缺陷的程度不受刺激复杂性的影响。总之,这些结果表明,如果熟悉的物体以视觉复杂度较高的形式呈现,可能会支持语义加工,那么复杂度会同时有利于项目记忆和联想记忆。在这种情况下,能增强项目记忆的刺激特性可以为联想记忆提供支架。
{"title":"Examining the role of stimulus complexity in item and associative memory.","authors":"Ricarda Endemann, Siri-Maria Kamp","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01590-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01590-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Episodic memory comprises memory for individual information units (item memory) and for the connections among them (associative memory). In two experiments using an object pair learning task, we examined the effect of visual stimulus complexity on memory encoding and retrieval mechanisms and on item and associative memory performance. Subjects encoded pairs of black monochrome object images (low complexity, LC condition) or color photographs of objects (high complexity, HC condition) via interactive imagery, and subsequently item and associative recognition were tested. In Experiment 1, event-related potentials (ERPs) revealed an enhanced frontal N2 during encoding and an enhanced late posterior negativity (LPN) during item recognition in the HC condition, suggesting that memory traces containing visually more complex objects elicited a stronger effort in reconstructing the past episode. Item memory was consistently superior in the HC compared to the LC condition. Associative memory was either statistically unaffected by complexity (Experiment 1) or improved (Experiment 2) in the HC condition, speaking against a tradeoff between resources allocated to item versus associative memory, and hence contradicting results of some prior studies. In Experiment 2, in both young and older adults, both item and associative memory benefitted from stimulus complexity, such that the magnitude of the age-related associative deficit was not influenced by stimulus complexity. Together, these results suggest that if familiar objects are presented in a form that exhibits a higher visual complexity, which may support semantic processing, complexity can benefit both item and associative memory. Stimulus properties that enhance item memory can scaffold associative memory in this situation.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-18","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141724760","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Influence of degree of learning on rate of forgetting of tonal sequences. 学习程度对音调序列遗忘率的影响
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01597-6
Karim Rivera-Lares, Alan Baddeley, Sergio Della Sala

Initial performance is frequently equated in studies that compare forgetting rates across groups. However, since the encoding capacity of different groups can be different, some procedures to match initial degree of learning need to be implemented, adding confounding variables such as longer exposures to the material, which would create memories of a different age. Slamecka and McElree Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 384-397, (1983) and our previous work found that the rate of forgetting was independent from initial degree of learning using verbal material. The present study seeks to determine whether this pattern holds true when undertaken with nonverbal material. In two experiments, we manipulate initial degree of learning by varying the number of presentations of the material and studying the effect on the forgetting rates. A set of 30 tonal sequences were presented to young, healthy participants either once or three times. Forgetting was evaluated in a yes/no recognition paradigm immediately and 1 hour or 24 hours after the study phase. A different subset of 10 sequences was tested along with 10 nontargets at each retention interval. The results of these experiments showed that initial acquisition was modulated by the number of repetitions. However, the forgetting rates were independent of initial degree of learning. These results are in keeping with the pattern found by Slamecka and McElree, and in our own previous studies. They suggest that the pattern of parallel forgetting after different levels of initial learning is not limited to verbal material.

在比较各组遗忘率的研究中,初始表现经常被等同起来。然而,由于不同群体的编码能力可能不同,因此需要实施一些程序来匹配最初的学习程度,同时增加一些混杂变量,如接触材料的时间更长,这会产生不同年龄段的记忆。Slamecka 和 McElree 《实验心理学杂志》:学习、记忆和认知》,9,384-397,(1983 年)和我们以前的工作发现,遗忘率与使用语言材料的初始学习程度无关。本研究试图确定这种模式在使用非语言材料时是否成立。在两个实验中,我们通过改变材料的呈现次数来操纵初始学习程度,并研究其对遗忘率的影响。我们向年轻、健康的参与者展示了一组 30 个音调序列,展示次数为一次或三次。在研究阶段结束后的1小时或24小时内,分别用 "是/否 "识别范式对遗忘情况进行评估。在每个保留间隔期,10 个序列的不同子集与 10 个非目标一起接受测试。实验结果表明,初始习得受重复次数的调节。然而,遗忘率与初始学习程度无关。这些结果与 Slamecka 和 McElree 以及我们之前的研究发现的模式一致。他们的研究表明,不同初始学习程度后的平行遗忘模式并不局限于言语材料。
{"title":"Influence of degree of learning on rate of forgetting of tonal sequences.","authors":"Karim Rivera-Lares, Alan Baddeley, Sergio Della Sala","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01597-6","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01597-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Initial performance is frequently equated in studies that compare forgetting rates across groups. However, since the encoding capacity of different groups can be different, some procedures to match initial degree of learning need to be implemented, adding confounding variables such as longer exposures to the material, which would create memories of a different age. Slamecka and McElree Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 384-397, (1983) and our previous work found that the rate of forgetting was independent from initial degree of learning using verbal material. The present study seeks to determine whether this pattern holds true when undertaken with nonverbal material. In two experiments, we manipulate initial degree of learning by varying the number of presentations of the material and studying the effect on the forgetting rates. A set of 30 tonal sequences were presented to young, healthy participants either once or three times. Forgetting was evaluated in a yes/no recognition paradigm immediately and 1 hour or 24 hours after the study phase. A different subset of 10 sequences was tested along with 10 nontargets at each retention interval. The results of these experiments showed that initial acquisition was modulated by the number of repetitions. However, the forgetting rates were independent of initial degree of learning. These results are in keeping with the pattern found by Slamecka and McElree, and in our own previous studies. They suggest that the pattern of parallel forgetting after different levels of initial learning is not limited to verbal material.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635031","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The day-of-the-week effect is resilient to routine change. 周日效应对常规变化具有弹性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01606-8
Anna M A Wagelmans, Virginie van Wassenhove

Temporal landmarks are salient events that structure the way humans think about time. They may be personal events, such as one's birthday, or shared cultural events, such as the COVID-19 pandemic. Due to societal habits, the cyclical weekly structure - for example, working on weekdays, resting on the weekends - helps individuals orient themselves in time. In the "day-of-the-week effect," individuals are faster at reporting which day of the week it is on weekends than they are on weekdays. Herein, we hypothesized that the disruption of social habits during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns may have weakened this effect, thereby accounting for the "Blursday" phenomenon. In the current study, speeded responses to the question "What day of the week is it?" were collected online from 1,742 French participants, during and after the lockdown periods. We found that reaction times for days of the weekends remained faster than for weekdays during the lockdown, although the overall reaction times were significantly slower during lockdown. We also found that responses were slower as governmental stringency rules and restrictions in mobility increased. Our results suggest that the weekend landmark remains a stable temporal anchor in French culture despite the experienced temporal distortions induced by the disruption of social habits during the pandemic. We conclude that cultural temporal landmarks shape socially shared temporal cognitive maps.

时间地标是构建人类时间思维方式的显著事件。它们可能是个人事件,如生日,也可能是共同的文化事件,如 COVID-19 大流行。由于社会习惯,周期性的周结构--例如,工作日工作,周末休息--帮助个人确定时间方向。在 "星期效应 "中,个人在报告周末是星期几时时比平日更快。在此,我们假设 COVID-19 大流行封锁期间社会习惯的破坏可能会削弱这种效应,从而解释 "模糊星期四 "现象。在本研究中,我们在线收集了 1742 名法国参与者在封锁期间和封锁后对 "今天是星期几 "这一问题的快速反应。我们发现,在封锁期间,对周末的反应时间仍然比平日快,尽管在封锁期间整体反应时间明显较慢。我们还发现,随着政府严格规定和行动限制的增加,反应速度也会变慢。我们的研究结果表明,尽管大流行病期间社会习惯的破坏导致了时间上的扭曲,但周末地标在法国文化中仍然是一个稳定的时间锚。我们的结论是,文化时间地标塑造了社会共享的时间认知地图。
{"title":"The day-of-the-week effect is resilient to routine change.","authors":"Anna M A Wagelmans, Virginie van Wassenhove","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01606-8","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01606-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Temporal landmarks are salient events that structure the way humans think about time. They may be personal events, such as one's birthday, or shared cultural events, such as the COVID-19 pandemic. Due to societal habits, the cyclical weekly structure - for example, working on weekdays, resting on the weekends - helps individuals orient themselves in time. In the \"day-of-the-week effect,\" individuals are faster at reporting which day of the week it is on weekends than they are on weekdays. Herein, we hypothesized that the disruption of social habits during the COVID-19 pandemic lockdowns may have weakened this effect, thereby accounting for the \"Blursday\" phenomenon. In the current study, speeded responses to the question \"What day of the week is it?\" were collected online from 1,742 French participants, during and after the lockdown periods. We found that reaction times for days of the weekends remained faster than for weekdays during the lockdown, although the overall reaction times were significantly slower during lockdown. We also found that responses were slower as governmental stringency rules and restrictions in mobility increased. Our results suggest that the weekend landmark remains a stable temporal anchor in French culture despite the experienced temporal distortions induced by the disruption of social habits during the pandemic. We conclude that cultural temporal landmarks shape socially shared temporal cognitive maps.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141628108","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Surface matches prevail over distant analogs during retrieval. 在检索过程中,表面匹配优先于远距离类似物。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-07-11 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01605-9
Ricardo A Minervino, Máximo Trench

Laboratory studies using a reception paradigm have found that memory items sharing similar entities and relations with a working memory cue (surface matches) are easier to retrieve than items sharing only a system of abstract relations (structural matches). However, the naturalistic approach has contended that the observed supremacy of superficial similarity could have originated in a shallow processing of somewhat inconsequential stories, as well as in the inadvertent inclusion of structural similarity during the construction of surface matches. We addressed the question of which kind of similarity dominates retrieval through a hybrid paradigm that combines the ecological validity of the naturalistic production paradigm with the experimental control of the reception paradigm. In Experiment 1 we presented participants with a target story that maintained either superficial or structural similarities with two popular movies that had received a careful processing prior to the experimental session. Experiment 2 replicated the same procedure with highly viralized public events. In line with traditional laboratory results, surface matches were significantly better retrieved than structural matches, confirming the supremacy of superficial similarities during retrieval.

使用接收范式进行的实验室研究发现,与工作记忆线索(表面匹配)共享相似实体和关系的记忆项目,比仅共享抽象关系系统(结构匹配)的记忆项目更容易检索。然而,自然主义方法认为,所观察到的表面相似性的优势可能源于对一些无关紧要的故事的浅层处理,以及在构建表面匹配时无意中包含了结构相似性。我们通过一种混合范式来解决哪种相似性主导检索的问题,该范式结合了自然生产范式的生态有效性和接收范式的实验控制。在实验 1 中,我们向参与者展示了一个目标故事,该故事与两部流行电影在表面上或结构上保持相似,而这两部电影在实验前都经过了仔细加工。实验 2 以高度病毒化的公共事件复制了相同的程序。与传统的实验室结果一致,表面匹配的检索效果明显好于结构匹配的检索效果,这证实了表面相似性在检索过程中的优势。
{"title":"Surface matches prevail over distant analogs during retrieval.","authors":"Ricardo A Minervino, Máximo Trench","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01605-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01605-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Laboratory studies using a reception paradigm have found that memory items sharing similar entities and relations with a working memory cue (surface matches) are easier to retrieve than items sharing only a system of abstract relations (structural matches). However, the naturalistic approach has contended that the observed supremacy of superficial similarity could have originated in a shallow processing of somewhat inconsequential stories, as well as in the inadvertent inclusion of structural similarity during the construction of surface matches. We addressed the question of which kind of similarity dominates retrieval through a hybrid paradigm that combines the ecological validity of the naturalistic production paradigm with the experimental control of the reception paradigm. In Experiment 1 we presented participants with a target story that maintained either superficial or structural similarities with two popular movies that had received a careful processing prior to the experimental session. Experiment 2 replicated the same procedure with highly viralized public events. In line with traditional laboratory results, surface matches were significantly better retrieved than structural matches, confirming the supremacy of superficial similarities during retrieval.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-07-11","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141591773","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory & Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1