首页 > 最新文献

Memory & Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Detecting the veracity of children's experiences through drawings. 通过绘画检测儿童经历的真实性。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2024-07-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6
Jennifer L Briere, Tobi Patkau-Ceh, Tammy A Marche

Research is lacking regarding adults' ability to determine whether children's drawings are based on an experience or not. Drawings are useful in professional settings to alleviate linguistic demands, facilitate memory, and have been used as evidence. Determining the accuracy of veracity assessments of children's drawings would inform professionals regarding their use as evidence of experiences. Twenty-eight children (14 younger, Mage = 7.53 years, SDage = 1.19; 14 older, Mage = 11.67 years, SD = 1.27) produced drawings of two events: one staged experienced, and one narrative-based not experienced event. Fifty (Study 1, Mage = 23.72 years, SDage = 9.70) and 63 (Study 2, Mage = 25.92, SDage = 12.79) adults indicated whether each drawing was based on experience and their confidence in each assessment. In Study 2, additional drawing quality assessments were collected. Results indicated that adults were more accurate at distinguishing experienced than not experienced drawings for older artists. An inverse relationship was observed between confidence and accuracy-participants were more confident when they were inaccurate, especially for younger artists. Drawing quality improved with age and for drawings of experienced events. Adults tended to rate drawings of higher quality as resulting from experience leading to the highest accuracy for drawings from older artists that were based on experience. Overall, results suggest that there may be some features of drawings that allow for above chance levels of accuracy (up to 75%). However, rates are not high enough across assessments (M = 53.93%, range: 39%-75%) to reliably use them as indicators of experience.

关于成人判断儿童绘画是否基于经历的能力还缺乏研究。绘画在专业环境中非常有用,可以减轻语言要求,促进记忆,并被用作证据。确定对儿童绘画的真实性评估的准确性将为专业人员提供有关将其用作经历证据的信息。28 名儿童(14 名年龄较小,年龄 = 7.53 岁,平均年龄 = 1.19 岁;14 名年龄较大,年龄 = 11.67 岁,平均年龄 = 1.27 岁)绘制了两个事件的图画:一个是经历过的事件,另一个是没有经历过的事件。50名(研究 1,年龄 = 23.72 岁,SD 年龄 = 9.70)和 63 名(研究 2,年龄 = 25.92 岁,SD 年龄 = 12.79)成人分别指出了每幅绘画是否基于经验,以及他们对每次评估的信心。在研究 2 中,还收集了额外的绘画质量评估。结果表明,对于年长的艺术家来说,成人能更准确地区分有经验的绘画作品和没有经验的绘画作品。在自信心和准确性之间发现了一种反比关系--参与者在不准确时更自信,尤其是年轻的艺术家。绘画质量随着年龄的增长而提高,经验事件的绘画质量也随之提高。成人倾向于评价因经验而绘制的画作质量更高,因此年长艺术家根据经验绘制的画作准确率最高。总体而言,结果表明,绘画的某些特征可能会使准确率高于偶然水平(高达 75%)。然而,在所有评估中,准确率都不够高(M = 53.93%,范围:39%-75%),因此不能可靠地将其作为经验指标。
{"title":"Detecting the veracity of children's experiences through drawings.","authors":"Jennifer L Briere, Tobi Patkau-Ceh, Tammy A Marche","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01527-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research is lacking regarding adults' ability to determine whether children's drawings are based on an experience or not. Drawings are useful in professional settings to alleviate linguistic demands, facilitate memory, and have been used as evidence. Determining the accuracy of veracity assessments of children's drawings would inform professionals regarding their use as evidence of experiences. Twenty-eight children (14 younger, M<sub>age</sub> = 7.53 years, SD<sub>age</sub> = 1.19; 14 older, M<sub>age</sub> = 11.67 years, SD = 1.27) produced drawings of two events: one staged experienced, and one narrative-based not experienced event. Fifty (Study 1, M<sub>age</sub> = 23.72 years, SD<sub>age</sub> = 9.70) and 63 (Study 2, M<sub>age</sub> = 25.92, SD<sub>age</sub> = 12.79) adults indicated whether each drawing was based on experience and their confidence in each assessment. In Study 2, additional drawing quality assessments were collected. Results indicated that adults were more accurate at distinguishing experienced than not experienced drawings for older artists. An inverse relationship was observed between confidence and accuracy-participants were more confident when they were inaccurate, especially for younger artists. Drawing quality improved with age and for drawings of experienced events. Adults tended to rate drawings of higher quality as resulting from experience leading to the highest accuracy for drawings from older artists that were based on experience. Overall, results suggest that there may be some features of drawings that allow for above chance levels of accuracy (up to 75%). However, rates are not high enough across assessments (M = 53.93%, range: 39%-75%) to reliably use them as indicators of experience.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"116-134"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141635030","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Drawing as a tool for investigating the nature of imagery representations of blind people: The case of the canonical size phenomenon. 绘画作为研究盲人图像表征本质的工具:规范尺寸现象的案例。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2025-01-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-023-01491-7
Magdalena Szubielska, Wojciech Kędziora, Paweł Augustynowicz, Delphine Picard

Several studies have shown that blind people, including those with congenital blindness, can use raised-line drawings, both for "reading" tactile graphics and for drawing unassisted. However, research on drawings produced by blind people has mainly been qualitative. The current experimental study was designed to investigate the under-researched issue of the size of drawings created by people with blindness. Participants (N = 59) varied in their visual status. Adventitiously blind people had previous visual experience and might use visual representations (e.g., when visualising objects in imagery/working memory). Congenitally blind people did not have any visual experience. The participant's task was to draw from memory common objects that vary in size in the real world. The findings revealed that both groups of participants produced larger drawings of objects that have larger actual sizes. This means that the size of familiar objects is a property of blind people's mental representations, regardless of their visual status. Our research also sheds light on the nature of the phenomenon of canonical size. Since we have found the canonical size effect in a group of people who are blind from birth, the assumption of the visual nature of this phenomenon - caused by the ocular-centric biases present in studies on drawing performance - should be revised.

几项研究表明,包括先天性失明在内的盲人可以使用凸起的线条来“阅读”触觉图形,也可以在没有帮助的情况下画画。然而,对盲人绘画的研究主要是定性的。目前的实验研究旨在调查盲人创作的绘画尺寸这一尚未得到充分研究的问题。参与者(N = 59)的视觉状态各不相同。先天失明的人有先前的视觉经验,可能会使用视觉表征(例如,当在意象/工作记忆中可视化物体时)。先天失明的人没有任何视觉体验。参与者的任务是从记忆中画出现实世界中大小不一的常见物体。研究结果显示,两组参与者都画出了实际尺寸更大的物体的更大的图画。这意味着熟悉物体的大小是盲人心理表征的属性,而不管他们的视觉状态如何。我们的研究也揭示了规范尺寸现象的本质。既然我们已经在一群天生失明的人身上发现了标准尺寸效应,那么这种现象的视觉本质的假设——由绘画表现研究中出现的以眼睛为中心的偏见引起的——应该被修正。
{"title":"Drawing as a tool for investigating the nature of imagery representations of blind people: The case of the canonical size phenomenon.","authors":"Magdalena Szubielska, Wojciech Kędziora, Paweł Augustynowicz, Delphine Picard","doi":"10.3758/s13421-023-01491-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-023-01491-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several studies have shown that blind people, including those with congenital blindness, can use raised-line drawings, both for \"reading\" tactile graphics and for drawing unassisted. However, research on drawings produced by blind people has mainly been qualitative. The current experimental study was designed to investigate the under-researched issue of the size of drawings created by people with blindness. Participants (N = 59) varied in their visual status. Adventitiously blind people had previous visual experience and might use visual representations (e.g., when visualising objects in imagery/working memory). Congenitally blind people did not have any visual experience. The participant's task was to draw from memory common objects that vary in size in the real world. The findings revealed that both groups of participants produced larger drawings of objects that have larger actual sizes. This means that the size of familiar objects is a property of blind people's mental representations, regardless of their visual status. Our research also sheds light on the nature of the phenomenon of canonical size. Since we have found the canonical size effect in a group of people who are blind from birth, the assumption of the visual nature of this phenomenon - caused by the ocular-centric biases present in studies on drawing performance - should be revised.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":"175-188"},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2025-01-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11779753/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138177536","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The role of anchoring information in judgments of learning. 锚定信息在学习判断中的作用。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-24 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01670-0
Kenji Ikeda, Yosuke Hattori, Yuichi Ito, Yuki Hamamoto

This study examined informative and uninformative anchoring effects on judgments of learning (JOLs), focusing on two hypotheses: the optimistic/pessimistic and differential-scaling hypotheses. The optimistic/pessimistic hypothesis states that anchoring information changes subjective confidence in memory, whereas the differential-scaling hypothesis states that anchoring information elicits a scaling bias in the conversion process of subjective internal confidence into scale JOLs (i.e., 0-100% responses). Experiment 1 focused on binary JOLs (i.e., Yes/No predictions). The results confirmed that the informative anchoring effect occurred (i.e., binary JOLs in the high anchor condition were higher than those in the low anchor condition), whereas the uninformative anchoring effect did not. Experiment 2 evaluated whether the difference in response scales between anchoring information and JOLs elicited the anchoring effect, demonstrating that the informative anchoring effect occurred when different response scales were used for the anchoring information (i.e., the number of words correctly recalled) and JOLs (i.e., 0-100% scale), and the uninformative anchoring effect did not. Experiment 3 examined whether the uninformative anchoring effect can be explained by numeric priming rather than scaling bias, demonstrating that anchoring information unrelated to test performance using a 0-100% scale did not elicit the uninformative anchoring effect. These findings suggest that the informative anchoring effect supports the optimistic/pessimistic hypothesis, whereas the uninformative anchoring effect supports the differential-scaling hypothesis. Thus, the nature of anchoring information affects the process of forming JOLs. Specifically, the uninformative anchor elicits only scaling bias, whereas the informative anchor changes subjective confidence in memory.

本研究考察了信息性锚定效应和非信息性锚定效应对学习判断(JOLs)的影响,主要关注两种假设:乐观/悲观和微分尺度假设。乐观/悲观假设认为,锚定信息改变了主观对记忆的信心,而差异标度假设认为,锚定信息在主观内部信心转化为尺度JOLs(即0-100%的反应)的过程中会引起标度偏差。实验1关注的是二元JOLs(即是/否预测)。结果证实,信息锚定效应(即高锚条件下的二元JOLs高于低锚条件下的二元JOLs)发生了,而非信息锚定效应没有发生。实验2评估了锚定信息和joll之间的反应尺度差异是否引发了锚定效应,结果表明,锚定信息(即正确回忆的单词数)和joll(即0-100%)使用不同的反应尺度时,会产生信息性锚定效应,而非信息性锚定效应则不产生。实验3检验了非信息性锚定效应是否可以用数字启动来解释,而不是用尺度偏差来解释,结果表明,使用0-100%的尺度,与测试性能无关的锚定信息不会引发非信息性锚定效应。研究结果表明,信息锚定效应支持乐观/悲观假设,而非信息锚定效应支持差分尺度假设。因此,锚定信息的性质影响了JOLs的形成过程。具体来说,非信息性锚点只会引起尺度偏差,而信息性锚点则会改变记忆中的主观信心。
{"title":"The role of anchoring information in judgments of learning.","authors":"Kenji Ikeda, Yosuke Hattori, Yuichi Ito, Yuki Hamamoto","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01670-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01670-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>This study examined informative and uninformative anchoring effects on judgments of learning (JOLs), focusing on two hypotheses: the optimistic/pessimistic and differential-scaling hypotheses. The optimistic/pessimistic hypothesis states that anchoring information changes subjective confidence in memory, whereas the differential-scaling hypothesis states that anchoring information elicits a scaling bias in the conversion process of subjective internal confidence into scale JOLs (i.e., 0-100% responses). Experiment 1 focused on binary JOLs (i.e., Yes/No predictions). The results confirmed that the informative anchoring effect occurred (i.e., binary JOLs in the high anchor condition were higher than those in the low anchor condition), whereas the uninformative anchoring effect did not. Experiment 2 evaluated whether the difference in response scales between anchoring information and JOLs elicited the anchoring effect, demonstrating that the informative anchoring effect occurred when different response scales were used for the anchoring information (i.e., the number of words correctly recalled) and JOLs (i.e., 0-100% scale), and the uninformative anchoring effect did not. Experiment 3 examined whether the uninformative anchoring effect can be explained by numeric priming rather than scaling bias, demonstrating that anchoring information unrelated to test performance using a 0-100% scale did not elicit the uninformative anchoring effect. These findings suggest that the informative anchoring effect supports the optimistic/pessimistic hypothesis, whereas the uninformative anchoring effect supports the differential-scaling hypothesis. Thus, the nature of anchoring information affects the process of forming JOLs. Specifically, the uninformative anchor elicits only scaling bias, whereas the informative anchor changes subjective confidence in memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-24","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142883480","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Magnitude-space representations in the n-back task: Long-term representations of magnitudes alter the working memory performance. n-back任务中的量空间表征:量的长期表征改变了工作记忆的表现。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01667-9
Ilgım Hepdarcan, Hakan Çetinkaya, Seda Dural

Prior research has predominantly examined the role of working memory (WM) in tasks involving numerical information and spatial properties, such as memorizing number sequences and performing parity judgment and magnitude comparison. In contrast to focusing solely on the effect of WM on number judgment tasks, our study investigates how magnitude-space associations affect WM task performance, emphasizing long-term representations, specifically the concept of mental number line (MNL) compatibility (small items on the left, large items on the right) in long-term memory (LTM). Moving from the idea of representations within LTM contribute to the functioning of WM during task execution, we explore the effects of congruent, incongruent, and negative congruent numerical and non-numerical magnitude-space associations on magnitude-based 1-back (low WM load) and 2-back (high WM load) tasks. MNL compatible n-back and test items are congruent, MNL compatible n-back and MNL incompatible (small on the right, large on the left) test items (or vice versa) are incongruent, and MNL incompatible n-back and test items are considered negative congruent. Because negative congruent and incongruent representations may not activate existing representations in LTM, as congruent representations, we expected worse WM performance in negative congruent and incongruent trials than in congruent trials. Results reveal that congruent and incongruent representations elicit more accurate and rapid responses than negative congruents, suggesting that congruent and incongruent representations contribute to task execution. Additionally, we observe a size effect for small numerical magnitudes and a reverse size effect for large physical magnitudes, pointing towards the coactivation of LTM and WM in magnitude-space relations.

之前的研究主要考察了工作记忆(WM)在涉及数字信息和空间属性的任务中的作用,如记忆数字序列、进行奇偶判断和大小比较等。与只关注工作记忆对数字判断任务的影响不同,我们的研究强调长期表征,特别是长时记忆(LTM)中的心理数字线(MNL)兼容性概念(左侧为小项目,右侧为大项目),探讨了大小空间关联如何影响工作记忆任务的表现。从 LTM 中的表征有助于任务执行过程中的 WM 功能这一观点出发,我们探讨了数字和非数字大小空间的一致、不一致和负一致关联对基于大小的 1 回(低 WM 负荷)和 2 回(高 WM 负荷)任务的影响。MNL兼容的n-back和测试项目是一致的,MNL兼容的n-back和MNL不兼容(右边小,左边大)的测试项目(或相反)是不一致的,MNL不兼容的n-back和测试项目被认为是负一致的。由于负同义和不同义表征可能不会像同义表征那样激活LTM中的现有表征,我们预计负同义和不同义试验的WM表现会比同义试验差。结果表明,与否定的同义表征相比,同义和不同义表征能引起更准确、更快速的反应,这表明同义和不同义表征有助于任务的执行。此外,我们还观察到小数字量级的大小效应和大物理量级的反向大小效应,这表明在量级空间关系中,LTM 和 WM 是共同激活的。
{"title":"Magnitude-space representations in the n-back task: Long-term representations of magnitudes alter the working memory performance.","authors":"Ilgım Hepdarcan, Hakan Çetinkaya, Seda Dural","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01667-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01667-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Prior research has predominantly examined the role of working memory (WM) in tasks involving numerical information and spatial properties, such as memorizing number sequences and performing parity judgment and magnitude comparison. In contrast to focusing solely on the effect of WM on number judgment tasks, our study investigates how magnitude-space associations affect WM task performance, emphasizing long-term representations, specifically the concept of mental number line (MNL) compatibility (small items on the left, large items on the right) in long-term memory (LTM). Moving from the idea of representations within LTM contribute to the functioning of WM during task execution, we explore the effects of congruent, incongruent, and negative congruent numerical and non-numerical magnitude-space associations on magnitude-based 1-back (low WM load) and 2-back (high WM load) tasks. MNL compatible n-back and test items are congruent, MNL compatible n-back and MNL incompatible (small on the right, large on the left) test items (or vice versa) are incongruent, and MNL incompatible n-back and test items are considered negative congruent. Because negative congruent and incongruent representations may not activate existing representations in LTM, as congruent representations, we expected worse WM performance in negative congruent and incongruent trials than in congruent trials. Results reveal that congruent and incongruent representations elicit more accurate and rapid responses than negative congruents, suggesting that congruent and incongruent representations contribute to task execution. Additionally, we observe a size effect for small numerical magnitudes and a reverse size effect for large physical magnitudes, pointing towards the coactivation of LTM and WM in magnitude-space relations.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-20","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142865769","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Transitional probabilities outweigh frequency of occurrence in statistical learning of simultaneously presented visual shapes. 在统计学习中,过渡概率大于同时呈现的视觉形状的出现频率。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-10 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01665-x
Ansgar D Endress

Statistical learning is a mechanism for detecting associations among co-occurring elements in many domains and species. A key controversy is whether it leads to memory for discrete chunks composed of these associated elements, or merely to pairwise associations among elements. Critical evidence for the mere-association view comes from the "phantom-word" phenomenon, where learners recognize statistically coherent but unattested items better than actually presented items with weaker internal associations, suggesting that they prioritize pairwise associations over memories for discrete units. However, this phenomenon has only been demonstrated for sequentially presented stimuli, but not for simultaneously presented visual shapes, where learners might prioritize discrete units over pairwise associations. Here, I ask whether the phantom-word phenomenon can be observed with simultaneously presented visual shapes. Learners were familiarized with scenes combining two triplets of visual shapes (hereafter "words"). They were then tested on their recognition of these words vs. part-words (attested items with weaker internal associations), of phantom-words (unattested items with strong internal associations) vs. part-words, and of words vs. phantom-words. Learners preferred both words and phantom-words over part-words and showed no preference for words over phantom-words. This suggests that, as for sequentially input, statistical learning in simultaneously presented shapes leads primarily to pairwise associations rather than to memories for discrete chunks. However, as, in some analyses, the preference for words over part-words was slightly higher than for phantom-words over part-words, the results do not rule out that, for simultaneous presented items, learners might have some limited sensitivity to frequency of occurrence.

统计学习是一种检测许多领域和物种中共同发生元素之间关联的机制。一个关键的争议是,它是否会导致由这些相关元素组成的离散块的记忆,或者仅仅是元素之间的成对关联。单纯关联观点的关键证据来自“幻影词”现象,即学习者对统计上连贯但未经证实的项目的识别能力强于实际呈现的内部关联较弱的项目,这表明他们优先考虑成对关联,而不是离散单元的记忆。然而,这种现象只在顺序呈现的刺激中得到了证明,而在同时呈现的视觉形状中没有得到证明,在这种情况下,学习者可能会优先考虑离散单元而不是成对联想。在这里,我想问的是,在同时呈现视觉形状的情况下,是否可以观察到幻字现象。学习者熟悉由两个三联体视觉形状组成的场景(以下简称“单词”)。然后测试他们对这些单词与部分单词(内部关联较弱的已证实项目)、幻词(内部关联较强的未证实项目)与部分单词、单词与幻词的识别能力。与部分词相比,学习者更喜欢单词和幻影词,并且对单词和幻影词没有表现出偏好。这表明,对于顺序输入,同时呈现形状的统计学习主要导致成对联想,而不是离散块的记忆。然而,由于在一些分析中,对单词的偏好略高于对部分单词的偏好,而对幻影单词的偏好略高于部分单词的偏好,结果并不排除,对于同时呈现的项目,学习者可能对出现频率有一些有限的敏感性。
{"title":"Transitional probabilities outweigh frequency of occurrence in statistical learning of simultaneously presented visual shapes.","authors":"Ansgar D Endress","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01665-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01665-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Statistical learning is a mechanism for detecting associations among co-occurring elements in many domains and species. A key controversy is whether it leads to memory for discrete chunks composed of these associated elements, or merely to pairwise associations among elements. Critical evidence for the mere-association view comes from the \"phantom-word\" phenomenon, where learners recognize statistically coherent but unattested items better than actually presented items with weaker internal associations, suggesting that they prioritize pairwise associations over memories for discrete units. However, this phenomenon has only been demonstrated for sequentially presented stimuli, but not for simultaneously presented visual shapes, where learners might prioritize discrete units over pairwise associations. Here, I ask whether the phantom-word phenomenon can be observed with simultaneously presented visual shapes. Learners were familiarized with scenes combining two triplets of visual shapes (hereafter \"words\"). They were then tested on their recognition of these words vs. part-words (attested items with weaker internal associations), of phantom-words (unattested items with strong internal associations) vs. part-words, and of words vs. phantom-words. Learners preferred both words and phantom-words over part-words and showed no preference for words over phantom-words. This suggests that, as for sequentially input, statistical learning in simultaneously presented shapes leads primarily to pairwise associations rather than to memories for discrete chunks. However, as, in some analyses, the preference for words over part-words was slightly higher than for phantom-words over part-words, the results do not rule out that, for simultaneous presented items, learners might have some limited sensitivity to frequency of occurrence.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-10","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142802882","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The benefits of elaborative encoding over retrieval practice for associative learning. 精练编码对联想学习的益处。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-06 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01671-z
Antônio Jaeger, Thuan Henrique Pedrosa Gomes Martins, João Pedro Parreira Rodrigues, Bruno Felipe Barbosa Muniz, Ana Luísa Santiago da Silveira Fonseca, Ariel de Oliveira Gonçalves

The practice of retrieval has been shown to be highly beneficial for memory retention, but it has seldom been compared with learning strategies other than repeated study. Here, we compared the benefits of retrieval practice (without feedback) with the benefits of two elaborative encoding tasks for word pair learning. Specifically, after studying series of randomly combined word pairs, participants performed an interactive-imagery (Experiments 1-2) or sentence-generation task (Experiments 3-5), retrieval practice, and a letter-counting or a rereading task. In Experiments 1-4, the word pairs were shown after a 24-h interval for testing in its original form or with the second word replaced by the second word from another pair, and participants performed recognition (old/new) followed by associative memory tests (intact/rearranged). In Experiment 5, memory was tested in a final cued-recall task administered shortly after initial learning. The interactive-imagery task was as beneficial as retrieval practice for recognition, but consistently more beneficial than retrieval for performance at the associative task. Sentence generation, on the other hand, produced greater performances than retrieval practice in recognition, associative memory, and cued-recall tests. These findings reveal that simple elaborative encoding tasks, such as imagining scenes or generating sentences, can be more beneficial for memory retention than retrieval practice without feedback.

检索练习已被证明对记忆保持非常有益,但除了重复学习之外,很少将其与学习策略进行比较。在这里,我们比较了检索练习(无反馈)和两种精细编码任务对单词对学习的好处。具体来说,在研究了一系列随机组合的单词对之后,参与者进行了互动意象(实验1-2)或句子生成任务(实验3-5),检索练习,以及字母计数或重读任务。在实验1-4中,每隔24小时对单词对进行原单词对和新单词对替换后的联想记忆测试,参与者先进行识别(旧/新),然后进行联想记忆测试(完整/重新排列)。在实验5中,在初始学习后不久进行的最后提示回忆任务中测试了记忆。交互意象任务与检索练习对识别的好处是一样的,但始终比检索对联想任务的表现更有益。另一方面,句子生成在识别、联想记忆和线索回忆测试中比检索练习产生了更好的表现。这些发现表明,简单的精细编码任务,如想象场景或生成句子,比没有反馈的检索练习更有利于记忆保留。
{"title":"The benefits of elaborative encoding over retrieval practice for associative learning.","authors":"Antônio Jaeger, Thuan Henrique Pedrosa Gomes Martins, João Pedro Parreira Rodrigues, Bruno Felipe Barbosa Muniz, Ana Luísa Santiago da Silveira Fonseca, Ariel de Oliveira Gonçalves","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01671-z","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01671-z","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The practice of retrieval has been shown to be highly beneficial for memory retention, but it has seldom been compared with learning strategies other than repeated study. Here, we compared the benefits of retrieval practice (without feedback) with the benefits of two elaborative encoding tasks for word pair learning. Specifically, after studying series of randomly combined word pairs, participants performed an interactive-imagery (Experiments 1-2) or sentence-generation task (Experiments 3-5), retrieval practice, and a letter-counting or a rereading task. In Experiments 1-4, the word pairs were shown after a 24-h interval for testing in its original form or with the second word replaced by the second word from another pair, and participants performed recognition (old/new) followed by associative memory tests (intact/rearranged). In Experiment 5, memory was tested in a final cued-recall task administered shortly after initial learning. The interactive-imagery task was as beneficial as retrieval practice for recognition, but consistently more beneficial than retrieval for performance at the associative task. Sentence generation, on the other hand, produced greater performances than retrieval practice in recognition, associative memory, and cued-recall tests. These findings reveal that simple elaborative encoding tasks, such as imagining scenes or generating sentences, can be more beneficial for memory retention than retrieval practice without feedback.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-06","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142792616","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The list-wide proportion congruency effect is larger when the distractor precedes the target: Evidence for conflict-independent control in the prime-probe task. 当分心者先于目标者时,全列表比例一致性效应更大:启动-探测任务中冲突无关控制的证据。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-05 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01669-7
Daniel H Weissman, Chloe E Saba

Stroop-like interference effects are smaller in blocks of mostly incongruent (MI) trials than in blocks of mostly congruent (MC) trials. It is unclear, though, how control processes trigger this list-wide proportion congruency effect (LWPCE). The attentional shift account posits that a memory of experiencing conflict more frequently in MI blocks than in MC blocks leads control processes to shift attention toward the target in MI blocks. The response modulation account posits that a memory of block-wide congruency statistics (e.g., mostly incongruent) leads control processes to form expectations about upcoming trial congruency independent of conflict and modulate distractor-related response activation to prepare an expected congruent response (in MC blocks) or incongruent response (in MI blocks) to the target. This modulation occurs, however, only if the system translates the distractor into a response before the target. We conducted two experiments with the prime-probe task (N = 120) to investigate the response modulation account's prediction that giving the distractor a "head start" in stimulus-response translation increases the LWPCE independent of conflict. Confirming this prediction, the LWPCE was larger when the distractor appeared before - versus simultaneously with - the target, even though the overall congruency (i.e., conflict) effect was equivalent in these conditions (Experiment 1) or smaller when the distractor appeared before the target (Experiment 2). We also observed a negative congruency effect in the MI blocks of Experiment 2, which is inconsistent with a shift of attention toward the target. We conclude that a modulation of response activation contributes to the LWPCE.

在大多数不一致(MI)组试验中,stroop样干扰效应比在大多数一致(MC)组试验中要小。然而,目前尚不清楚控制过程如何触发这种全列表比例一致性效应(LWPCE)。注意转移解释认为,在MI块中比在MC块中更频繁地经历冲突的记忆会导致控制过程将注意力转移到MI块中的目标。反应调节解释认为,对整个块的一致性统计(例如,大多数是不一致的)的记忆导致控制过程形成对即将到来的试验一致性的期望,而不受冲突的影响,并调节与干扰物相关的反应激活,以准备对目标的预期一致反应(在MC块中)或不一致反应(在MI块中)。然而,只有当系统在目标之前将干扰转换为响应时,这种调制才会发生。我们用启动-探针任务(N = 120)进行了两个实验,以研究反应调节说的预测,即在刺激-反应转换中给予分心者“领先优势”会增加LWPCE,而不受冲突的影响。证实这一预测的是,当分心物先于目标出现时,LWPCE比与目标同时出现时更大,尽管在这些条件下,整体一致性(即冲突)效应是相等的(实验1),而当分心物先于目标出现时,LWPCE则更小(实验2)。我们还在实验2的MI块中观察到负一致性效应,这与注意力向目标转移不一致。我们得出结论,反应激活的调制有助于LWPCE。
{"title":"The list-wide proportion congruency effect is larger when the distractor precedes the target: Evidence for conflict-independent control in the prime-probe task.","authors":"Daniel H Weissman, Chloe E Saba","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01669-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01669-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Stroop-like interference effects are smaller in blocks of mostly incongruent (MI) trials than in blocks of mostly congruent (MC) trials. It is unclear, though, how control processes trigger this list-wide proportion congruency effect (LWPCE). The attentional shift account posits that a memory of experiencing conflict more frequently in MI blocks than in MC blocks leads control processes to shift attention toward the target in MI blocks. The response modulation account posits that a memory of block-wide congruency statistics (e.g., mostly incongruent) leads control processes to form expectations about upcoming trial congruency independent of conflict and modulate distractor-related response activation to prepare an expected congruent response (in MC blocks) or incongruent response (in MI blocks) to the target. This modulation occurs, however, only if the system translates the distractor into a response before the target. We conducted two experiments with the prime-probe task (N = 120) to investigate the response modulation account's prediction that giving the distractor a \"head start\" in stimulus-response translation increases the LWPCE independent of conflict. Confirming this prediction, the LWPCE was larger when the distractor appeared before - versus simultaneously with - the target, even though the overall congruency (i.e., conflict) effect was equivalent in these conditions (Experiment 1) or smaller when the distractor appeared before the target (Experiment 2). We also observed a negative congruency effect in the MI blocks of Experiment 2, which is inconsistent with a shift of attention toward the target. We conclude that a modulation of response activation contributes to the LWPCE.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-05","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142787315","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The effect of instructed refreshing on working memory: Is the memory boost a function of refreshing frequency or refreshing duration? 指示性刷新对工作记忆的影响:记忆提升是刷新频率还是刷新时间的函数?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01666-w
Evie Vergauwe, Alessandra S Souza, Naomi Langerock, Klaus Oberauer

Refreshing is assumed to reactivate the contents of working memory in an attention-based way, resulting in a boost of the attended representations and hence improving their subsequent memory. Here, we examined whether the refreshing-induced memory boost is a constant or a gradual, time-dependent phenomenon. If the beneficial effect of refreshing on memory performance is due to the information being selected for refreshing (i.e., selection hypothesis), a constant memory boost is expected to occur each time an item is selected for refreshing, with better memory performance for items that are selected more often. If, however, the beneficial effect of refreshing on memory performance is due to spending time in the focus of attention during refreshing (i.e., duration hypothesis), a gradual memory boost is expected, with the size of the memory boost being a direct function of how long the item has been the object of focused attention. To distinguish between these hypotheses, we instructed and guided the use of refreshing during retention through the presentation of cues, and varied the number of refreshing steps and their duration independently. The number of refreshing steps, but not their duration, had an effect on recall, in agreement with the selection hypothesis. However, some of the results were less robust than anticipated, indicating that the effect of instructed refreshing is limited to certain task parameters.

刷新被认为是以一种以注意力为基础的方式重新激活工作记忆的内容,从而提高被关注的表征,从而改善他们随后的记忆。在这里,我们研究了刷新诱导的记忆增强是一个恒定的还是一个渐进的、依赖时间的现象。如果刷新对内存性能的有益影响是由于选择了要刷新的信息(即选择假设),那么每次选择一个项进行刷新时,预期会出现一个恒定的内存提升,选择更频繁的项具有更好的内存性能。然而,如果刷新对记忆性能的有益影响是由于在刷新期间花时间集中注意力(即持续时间假设),则预期记忆会逐渐增强,记忆增强的大小直接取决于该项目成为集中注意力对象的时间。为了区分这些假设,我们通过提示来指导和指导记忆过程中刷新的使用,并独立地改变刷新步骤的数量和持续时间。刷新步骤的次数,而不是持续时间,对回忆有影响,这与选择假说一致。然而,一些结果不如预期的健壮,这表明指示刷新的效果仅限于某些任务参数。
{"title":"The effect of instructed refreshing on working memory: Is the memory boost a function of refreshing frequency or refreshing duration?","authors":"Evie Vergauwe, Alessandra S Souza, Naomi Langerock, Klaus Oberauer","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01666-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01666-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Refreshing is assumed to reactivate the contents of working memory in an attention-based way, resulting in a boost of the attended representations and hence improving their subsequent memory. Here, we examined whether the refreshing-induced memory boost is a constant or a gradual, time-dependent phenomenon. If the beneficial effect of refreshing on memory performance is due to the information being selected for refreshing (i.e., selection hypothesis), a constant memory boost is expected to occur each time an item is selected for refreshing, with better memory performance for items that are selected more often. If, however, the beneficial effect of refreshing on memory performance is due to spending time in the focus of attention during refreshing (i.e., duration hypothesis), a gradual memory boost is expected, with the size of the memory boost being a direct function of how long the item has been the object of focused attention. To distinguish between these hypotheses, we instructed and guided the use of refreshing during retention through the presentation of cues, and varied the number of refreshing steps and their duration independently. The number of refreshing steps, but not their duration, had an effect on recall, in agreement with the selection hypothesis. However, some of the results were less robust than anticipated, indicating that the effect of instructed refreshing is limited to certain task parameters.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142773798","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Spontaneous associative thought may facilitate scene-gist memory via implicit scene-labeling. 自发联想思维可能通过内隐情景标签促进情景要点记忆。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01672-y
Shira Baror, Elissa Aminoff, Yoed N Kenett

Spontaneous associative processes (e.g., mind wandering, spontaneous memory recollection) are prevalent in everyday life, yet their influence on perceptual scene memory is under debate. Given that scene perception involves extraction of contextual associations, we hypothesized that associative thought would enhance scene memory by promoting encoding of contextual associations. In an online experiment (N = 75), participants viewed scenes, and following each scene either generated chained-free associations (associative processing), or, as control, listed words that begin with a specific letter (phonological processing). Scene memory was tested after an intermediate creativity task, which is also shown to rely on associative processes. Results revealed that associative thought, regardless of its conceptual (semantic) distances between responses, enhanced scene-gist memory, but hampered memory of scene details, implying that associative thought facilitates contextual encoding. In a follow-up experiment (N = 74), we found that the effect of associative thought on scene-gist memory was mediated by scene labeling. When participants were asked to explicitly label the scene before completing an associative processing or a phonological processing task, scene-gist memory was prioritized at the expense of scene details, eliminating the memory differences between tasks. These findings imply that labeling past perceived scenes, whether explicitly or implicitly during associative thought, facilitates scene-gist memory. Lastly, in both experiments, creativity was not correlated with scene memory but was positively correlated with the semantic distances between scene-based associations, extending past findings that link creativity with the breadth of associative processes. Together, these findings highlight the likely effect of post-perceptual associative processes on higher-order cognitive functions, such as memory consolidation and creative thought.

自发联想过程(如走神、自发记忆回忆)在日常生活中普遍存在,但它们对感知场景记忆的影响仍存在争议。鉴于场景感知涉及上下文关联的提取,我们假设联想思维通过促进上下文关联的编码来增强场景记忆。在一项在线实验中(N = 75),参与者观看场景,并跟随每个场景产生无链联想(联想处理),或者作为对照,列出以特定字母开头的单词(语音处理)。场景记忆是在中间创造性任务后测试的,这也表明依赖于联想过程。结果表明,无论反应之间的概念(语义)距离如何,联想思维都能增强情景要点记忆,但会阻碍场景细节的记忆,这表明联想思维促进了情境编码。在后续实验(N = 74)中,我们发现联想思维对情景要素记忆的影响是通过场景标记介导的。当参与者被要求在完成联想处理或语音处理任务之前明确地标记场景时,场景要点记忆优先于场景细节,消除了任务之间的记忆差异。这些发现表明,在联想思维过程中,无论是明确的还是隐含的,给过去感知到的场景贴上标签,都有助于场景要点记忆。最后,在两个实验中,创造力与场景记忆不相关,但与基于场景的联想之间的语义距离正相关,扩展了过去将创造力与联想过程广度联系起来的研究结果。总之,这些发现强调了后知觉联想过程对高阶认知功能(如记忆巩固和创造性思维)的可能影响。
{"title":"Spontaneous associative thought may facilitate scene-gist memory via implicit scene-labeling.","authors":"Shira Baror, Elissa Aminoff, Yoed N Kenett","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01672-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01672-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Spontaneous associative processes (e.g., mind wandering, spontaneous memory recollection) are prevalent in everyday life, yet their influence on perceptual scene memory is under debate. Given that scene perception involves extraction of contextual associations, we hypothesized that associative thought would enhance scene memory by promoting encoding of contextual associations. In an online experiment (N = 75), participants viewed scenes, and following each scene either generated chained-free associations (associative processing), or, as control, listed words that begin with a specific letter (phonological processing). Scene memory was tested after an intermediate creativity task, which is also shown to rely on associative processes. Results revealed that associative thought, regardless of its conceptual (semantic) distances between responses, enhanced scene-gist memory, but hampered memory of scene details, implying that associative thought facilitates contextual encoding. In a follow-up experiment (N = 74), we found that the effect of associative thought on scene-gist memory was mediated by scene labeling. When participants were asked to explicitly label the scene before completing an associative processing or a phonological processing task, scene-gist memory was prioritized at the expense of scene details, eliminating the memory differences between tasks. These findings imply that labeling past perceived scenes, whether explicitly or implicitly during associative thought, facilitates scene-gist memory. Lastly, in both experiments, creativity was not correlated with scene memory but was positively correlated with the semantic distances between scene-based associations, extending past findings that link creativity with the breadth of associative processes. Together, these findings highlight the likely effect of post-perceptual associative processes on higher-order cognitive functions, such as memory consolidation and creative thought.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142773795","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Rapid source forgetting across modalities: A problem for working memory models. 跨模态的快速源遗忘:工作记忆模型的一个问题。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-12-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01664-y
Molly A Delooze, Dominic Guitard, Nelson Cowan, Candice C Morey

Working memory is a cognitive system that enables the temporary retention (usually a few seconds) of a limited amount of information. However, recent evidence has posed challenges to the conventional understanding of working memory's persistence. Chen et al. (Psychological Science, 29(4), 645-655, 2018) demonstrated that participants can easily make judgments using a stimulus's identity but cannot recall from which source the information came (presented either as a written word or a color patch) just milliseconds earlier. This "Source Amnesia" carries substantial implications for working memory models but has yet to be explored within the realm of verbal information. We fill this gap by investigating the robustness and generalizability of this rapid forgetting phenomenon. We first replicate the observed effect within the visual domain (Experiment 1) and subsequently extend it to the verbal domain (Experiment 2). Finally, we test the idea that participants may instead encode a positional context (Experiment 3), in line with the Interference model (Oberauer & Lin, Psychological Review, 124(1), 21, 2017). Aligning with the work of Chen et al. (Psychological Science, 29(4), 645-655, 2018), our results consistently reveal a pronounced tendency for rapid forgetting, for both visual and verbal information regardless of whether the information is elicited for recall by format or position cues. The theoretical implications of these findings for current memory models are discussed.

工作记忆是一种认知系统,可以暂时保留有限数量的信息(通常是几秒钟)。然而,最近的证据对工作记忆的持久性的传统理解提出了挑战。Chen等人(心理科学,29(4),645-655,2018)证明,参与者可以很容易地根据刺激的身份做出判断,但无法回忆起信息来自何处(以书面文字或色块的形式呈现)。这种“源性失忆症”对工作记忆模型具有重大意义,但尚未在言语信息领域进行探索。我们通过研究这种快速遗忘现象的稳健性和普遍性来填补这一空白。我们首先在视觉领域复制观察到的效果(实验1),随后将其扩展到语言领域(实验2)。最后,我们根据干扰模型测试了参与者可能会编码位置上下文(实验3)的想法(Oberauer & Lin, Psychological Review, 124(1), 21,2017)。与Chen等人的研究结果一致(心理科学,29(4),645-655,2018),我们的研究结果一致表明,无论信息是通过格式线索还是位置线索唤起的,视觉信息和口头信息都有明显的快速遗忘倾向。讨论了这些发现对当前记忆模型的理论意义。
{"title":"Rapid source forgetting across modalities: A problem for working memory models.","authors":"Molly A Delooze, Dominic Guitard, Nelson Cowan, Candice C Morey","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01664-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01664-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Working memory is a cognitive system that enables the temporary retention (usually a few seconds) of a limited amount of information. However, recent evidence has posed challenges to the conventional understanding of working memory's persistence. Chen et al. (Psychological Science, 29(4), 645-655, 2018) demonstrated that participants can easily make judgments using a stimulus's identity but cannot recall from which source the information came (presented either as a written word or a color patch) just milliseconds earlier. This \"Source Amnesia\" carries substantial implications for working memory models but has yet to be explored within the realm of verbal information. We fill this gap by investigating the robustness and generalizability of this rapid forgetting phenomenon. We first replicate the observed effect within the visual domain (Experiment 1) and subsequently extend it to the verbal domain (Experiment 2). Finally, we test the idea that participants may instead encode a positional context (Experiment 3), in line with the Interference model (Oberauer & Lin, Psychological Review, 124(1), 21, 2017). Aligning with the work of Chen et al. (Psychological Science, 29(4), 645-655, 2018), our results consistently reveal a pronounced tendency for rapid forgetting, for both visual and verbal information regardless of whether the information is elicited for recall by format or position cues. The theoretical implications of these findings for current memory models are discussed.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":" ","pages":""},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-12-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142773741","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory & Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1