首页 > 最新文献

Memory & Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Knowledge revision processes during reading: How pictures influence the activation of outdated information. 阅读过程中的知识修正过程:图片如何影响过时信息的激活。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01586-9
Pauline Frick, Panayiota Kendeou, Anne Schüler

Outdated information (i.e., information that is not or no longer accurate) continues to be automatically activated during reading and can hinder learning processes. Thus, it is important to understand which factors influence the activation of outdated information and, therefore, knowledge revision processes. In three online experiments, we investigated how illustrating updated or outdated information via pictures influences the activation of outdated information. In Experiments 1 (N = 421) and 2 (N = 422), we varied whether participants read texts containing outdated information that was later updated (outdated text) or texts containing only updated information (consistent text). In addition, the updated information was or was not illustrated by a picture. In Experiment 3 (N = 441), participants read outdated texts, and we varied whether the outdated, the updated, or no information was illustrated. In all experiments, we measured reading times for a target sentence referring to the updated information and the sentence following the target sentence. Results showed that target sentences' reading times were faster for illustrated than for non-illustrated texts (Experiments 1 and 2). Moreover, reading times were longer when the outdated information was illustrated than when the updated information was illustrated (Experiment 3). These results suggest that pictures overall facilitate cognitive processes during reading, but their content matters: Pictures showing the updated information had a greater impact on reading times than pictures showing the outdated information. The results extend existing theories on knowledge revision but also reading comprehension, by demonstrating how pictures might influence cognitive processes during reading.

过时的信息(即不准确或不再准确的信息)会在阅读过程中继续被自动激活,从而阻碍学习进程。因此,了解哪些因素会影响过时信息的激活,进而影响知识的修正过程就显得尤为重要。在三个在线实验中,我们研究了通过图片说明更新或过时信息如何影响过时信息的激活。在实验 1(421 人)和实验 2(422 人)中,我们改变了参与者阅读的文本是包含后来更新的过时信息的文本(过时文本)还是只包含更新信息的文本(一致文本)。此外,更新的信息是否配有图片说明。在实验 3(N=441)中,受试者阅读的是过时的文本,我们改变了过时信息、更新信息或无图示信息的阅读方式。在所有实验中,我们都测量了目标句中提及更新信息的句子和目标句后面句子的阅读时间。结果表明,有插图文本比无插图文本的目标句阅读时间更快(实验 1 和 2)。此外,当过时信息配有插图时,阅读时间要比更新信息配有插图时长(实验 3)。这些结果表明,图片总体上促进了阅读过程中的认知过程,但其内容也很重要:显示更新信息的图片比显示过时信息的图片对阅读时间的影响更大。这些结果不仅扩展了现有的知识修正理论,也扩展了阅读理解理论,证明了图片是如何影响阅读过程中的认知过程的。
{"title":"Knowledge revision processes during reading: How pictures influence the activation of outdated information.","authors":"Pauline Frick, Panayiota Kendeou, Anne Schüler","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01586-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01586-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Outdated information (i.e., information that is not or no longer accurate) continues to be automatically activated during reading and can hinder learning processes. Thus, it is important to understand which factors influence the activation of outdated information and, therefore, knowledge revision processes. In three online experiments, we investigated how illustrating updated or outdated information via pictures influences the activation of outdated information. In Experiments 1 (N = 421) and 2 (N = 422), we varied whether participants read texts containing outdated information that was later updated (outdated text) or texts containing only updated information (consistent text). In addition, the updated information was or was not illustrated by a picture. In Experiment 3 (N = 441), participants read outdated texts, and we varied whether the outdated, the updated, or no information was illustrated. In all experiments, we measured reading times for a target sentence referring to the updated information and the sentence following the target sentence. Results showed that target sentences' reading times were faster for illustrated than for non-illustrated texts (Experiments 1 and 2). Moreover, reading times were longer when the outdated information was illustrated than when the updated information was illustrated (Experiment 3). These results suggest that pictures overall facilitate cognitive processes during reading, but their content matters: Pictures showing the updated information had a greater impact on reading times than pictures showing the outdated information. The results extend existing theories on knowledge revision but also reading comprehension, by demonstrating how pictures might influence cognitive processes during reading.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141237649","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does sleep benefit source memory? Investigating 12-h retention intervals with a multinomial modeling approach. 睡眠是否有益于源记忆?用多项式建模法研究 12 小时的保持间隔。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01579-8
Sabrina Berres, Edgar Erdfelder, Beatrice G Kuhlmann

For retention intervals of up to 12 h, the active systems consolidation hypothesis predicts that sleep compared to wakefulness strengthens the context binding of memories previously established during encoding. Sleep should thus improve source memory. By comparing retention intervals filled with natural night sleep versus daytime wakefulness, we tested this prediction in two online source-monitoring experiments using intentionally learned pictures as items and incidentally learned screen positions and frame colors as source dimensions. In Experiment 1, we examined source memory by varying the spatial position of pictures on the computer screen. Multinomial modeling analyses revealed a significant sleep benefit in source memory. In Experiment 2, we manipulated both the spatial position and the frame color of pictures orthogonally to investigate source memory for two different source dimensions at the same time, also allowing exploration of bound memory for both source dimensions. The sleep benefit on spatial source memory replicated. In contrast, no source memory sleep benefit was observed for either frame color or bound memory of both source dimensions, probably as a consequence of a floor effect in incidental encoding of color associations. In sum, the results of both experiments show that sleep within a 12-h retention interval improves source memory for spatial positions, supporting the prediction of the active systems consolidation hypothesis. However, additional research is required to clarify the impact of sleep on source memory for other context features and bound memories of multiple source dimensions.

根据 "主动系统巩固假说 "的预测,对于长达 12 小时的保留时间间隔,睡眠比清醒时更能加强先前在编码过程中建立的记忆的上下文结合。因此,睡眠应能改善源记忆。通过比较夜间自然睡眠与白天清醒状态下的保留间隔,我们在两个在线源监控实验中检验了这一预测,实验中使用了有意学习的图片作为项目,并使用偶然学习的屏幕位置和框架颜色作为源维度。在实验 1 中,我们通过改变电脑屏幕上图片的空间位置来检验源记忆。多项式建模分析表明,源记忆对睡眠有显著益处。在实验 2 中,我们正交操纵了图片的空间位置和边框颜色,以同时研究两个不同来源维度的来源记忆,这也使我们能够探索两个来源维度的约束记忆。对空间源记忆的睡眠益处得到了复制。与此相反,无论是框架颜色还是两个源维度的约束记忆,都没有观察到源记忆的睡眠益处,这可能是颜色联想偶然编码的底限效应造成的。总之,这两项实验的结果表明,12 小时保留间隔内的睡眠能改善空间位置的源记忆,这支持了主动系统巩固假说的预测。然而,要明确睡眠对其他情境特征的源记忆和多源维度的绑定记忆的影响,还需要更多的研究。
{"title":"Does sleep benefit source memory? Investigating 12-h retention intervals with a multinomial modeling approach.","authors":"Sabrina Berres, Edgar Erdfelder, Beatrice G Kuhlmann","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01579-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01579-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>For retention intervals of up to 12 h, the active systems consolidation hypothesis predicts that sleep compared to wakefulness strengthens the context binding of memories previously established during encoding. Sleep should thus improve source memory. By comparing retention intervals filled with natural night sleep versus daytime wakefulness, we tested this prediction in two online source-monitoring experiments using intentionally learned pictures as items and incidentally learned screen positions and frame colors as source dimensions. In Experiment 1, we examined source memory by varying the spatial position of pictures on the computer screen. Multinomial modeling analyses revealed a significant sleep benefit in source memory. In Experiment 2, we manipulated both the spatial position and the frame color of pictures orthogonally to investigate source memory for two different source dimensions at the same time, also allowing exploration of bound memory for both source dimensions. The sleep benefit on spatial source memory replicated. In contrast, no source memory sleep benefit was observed for either frame color or bound memory of both source dimensions, probably as a consequence of a floor effect in incidental encoding of color associations. In sum, the results of both experiments show that sleep within a 12-h retention interval improves source memory for spatial positions, supporting the prediction of the active systems consolidation hypothesis. However, additional research is required to clarify the impact of sleep on source memory for other context features and bound memories of multiple source dimensions.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141237450","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Multisensory processing impacts memory for objects and their sources. 多感官处理会影响对物体及其来源的记忆。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-06-03 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01592-x
Shea E Duarte, Andrew P Yonelinas, Simona Ghetti, Joy J Geng

Multisensory object processing improves recognition memory for individual objects, but its impact on memory for neighboring visual objects and scene context remains largely unknown. It is therefore unclear how multisensory processing impacts episodic memory for information outside of the object itself. We conducted three experiments to test the prediction that the presence of audiovisual objects at encoding would improve memory for nearby visual objects, and improve memory for the environmental context in which they occurred. In Experiments 1a and 1b, participants viewed audiovisual-visual object pairs or visual-visual object pairs with a control sound during encoding and were subsequently tested on their memory for each object individually. In Experiment 2, objects were paired with semantically congruent or meaningless control sounds and appeared within four different scene environments. Memory for the environment was tested. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b showed that encoding a congruent audiovisual object did not significantly benefit memory for neighboring visual objects, but Experiment 2 showed that encoding a congruent audiovisual object did improve memory for the environments in which those objects were encoded. These findings suggest that multisensory processing can influence memory beyond the objects themselves and that it has a unique role in episodic memory formation. This is particularly important for understanding how memories and associations are formed in real-world situations, in which objects and their surroundings are often multimodal.

多感官物体加工可以改善对单个物体的识别记忆,但它对邻近视觉物体和场景背景记忆的影响在很大程度上仍是未知数。因此,我们还不清楚多感官加工是如何影响对物体本身以外信息的记忆的。我们进行了三项实验来验证我们的预测,即编码时视听对象的存在会改善对邻近视觉对象的记忆,并改善对发生这些对象的环境背景的记忆。在实验 1a 和 1b 中,受试者在编码时观看了视听对象对或带有控制声音的视听对象对,随后受试者分别接受了对每个对象的记忆测试。在实验 2 中,物体与语义一致或无意义的控制声音配对,并出现在四个不同的场景环境中。测试的内容是对环境的记忆。实验 1a 和 1b 的结果表明,编码同义视听对象并不能显著改善对相邻视觉对象的记忆,但实验 2 表明,编码同义视听对象确实能改善对这些对象所在环境的记忆。这些研究结果表明,多感官加工对记忆的影响超出了对象本身,它在外显记忆的形成中具有独特的作用。这对于理解记忆和联想是如何在现实世界中形成的尤为重要,因为在现实世界中,物体及其周围环境往往是多模态的。
{"title":"Multisensory processing impacts memory for objects and their sources.","authors":"Shea E Duarte, Andrew P Yonelinas, Simona Ghetti, Joy J Geng","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01592-x","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01592-x","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multisensory object processing improves recognition memory for individual objects, but its impact on memory for neighboring visual objects and scene context remains largely unknown. It is therefore unclear how multisensory processing impacts episodic memory for information outside of the object itself. We conducted three experiments to test the prediction that the presence of audiovisual objects at encoding would improve memory for nearby visual objects, and improve memory for the environmental context in which they occurred. In Experiments 1a and 1b, participants viewed audiovisual-visual object pairs or visual-visual object pairs with a control sound during encoding and were subsequently tested on their memory for each object individually. In Experiment 2, objects were paired with semantically congruent or meaningless control sounds and appeared within four different scene environments. Memory for the environment was tested. Results from Experiments 1a and 1b showed that encoding a congruent audiovisual object did not significantly benefit memory for neighboring visual objects, but Experiment 2 showed that encoding a congruent audiovisual object did improve memory for the environments in which those objects were encoded. These findings suggest that multisensory processing can influence memory beyond the objects themselves and that it has a unique role in episodic memory formation. This is particularly important for understanding how memories and associations are formed in real-world situations, in which objects and their surroundings are often multimodal.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-06-03","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141237726","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Using drawings and deep neural networks to characterize the building blocks of human visual similarity. 利用图画和深度神经网络描述人类视觉相似性的构成要素。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-30 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01580-1
Kushin Mukherjee, Timothy T Rogers

Early in life and without special training, human beings discern resemblance between abstract visual stimuli, such as drawings, and the real-world objects they represent. We used this capacity for visual abstraction as a tool for evaluating deep neural networks (DNNs) as models of human visual perception. Contrasting five contemporary DNNs, we evaluated how well each explains human similarity judgments among line drawings of recognizable and novel objects. For object sketches, human judgments were dominated by semantic category information; DNN representations contributed little additional information. In contrast, such features explained significant unique variance perceived similarity of abstract drawings. In both cases, a vision transformer trained to blend representations of images and their natural language descriptions showed the greatest ability to explain human perceptual similarity-an observation consistent with contemporary views of semantic representation and processing in the human mind and brain. Together, the results suggest that the building blocks of visual similarity may arise within systems that learn to use visual information, not for specific classification, but in service of generating semantic representations of objects.

人类很早就能辨别抽象视觉刺激(如图画)与其所代表的真实世界物体之间的相似性,而无需经过特殊训练。我们将这种视觉抽象能力作为评估深度神经网络(DNN)作为人类视觉感知模型的工具。通过对比五种当代 DNN,我们评估了每种 DNN 在多大程度上解释了人类对可识别物体和新物体的线描之间的相似性判断。对于物体素描,人类的判断主要受语义类别信息的影响;DNN 表征几乎不提供额外的信息。与此相反,这些特征却能解释抽象素描的相似性感知的显著独特差异。在这两种情况下,经过训练的视觉转换器都能融合图像表征和自然语言描述,显示出最大的解释人类感知相似性的能力--这一观察结果与当代人类心智和大脑中语义表征和处理的观点一致。总之,这些结果表明,视觉相似性的基石可能产生于学习使用视觉信息的系统中,这些系统不是为了具体分类,而是为了生成物体的语义表征。
{"title":"Using drawings and deep neural networks to characterize the building blocks of human visual similarity.","authors":"Kushin Mukherjee, Timothy T Rogers","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01580-1","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01580-1","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Early in life and without special training, human beings discern resemblance between abstract visual stimuli, such as drawings, and the real-world objects they represent. We used this capacity for visual abstraction as a tool for evaluating deep neural networks (DNNs) as models of human visual perception. Contrasting five contemporary DNNs, we evaluated how well each explains human similarity judgments among line drawings of recognizable and novel objects. For object sketches, human judgments were dominated by semantic category information; DNN representations contributed little additional information. In contrast, such features explained significant unique variance perceived similarity of abstract drawings. In both cases, a vision transformer trained to blend representations of images and their natural language descriptions showed the greatest ability to explain human perceptual similarity-an observation consistent with contemporary views of semantic representation and processing in the human mind and brain. Together, the results suggest that the building blocks of visual similarity may arise within systems that learn to use visual information, not for specific classification, but in service of generating semantic representations of objects.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-30","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141176688","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Drawing from name in semantic dementia reveals graded object knowledge representations in anterior temporal lobe. 从语义痴呆症患者的名字中得出结论,揭示了前颞叶中的分级对象知识表征。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-22 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01578-9
Tanmay Anand, Karalyn Patterson, James B Rowe, Thomas E Cope

Semantic dementia (SD) is characterized by progressive impairment in conceptual knowledge due to anterior temporal lobe (ATL) neurodegeneration. Extended neuropsychological assessments can quantitatively demonstrate the semantic impairment, but this graded loss of knowledge can also be readily observed in the qualitative observation of patients' recall of single concepts. Here, we present the results of a simple task of object drawing-from-name, by patients with SD (N = 19), who have isolated atrophy of the ATL bilaterally. Both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, patient drawings demonstrated a pattern of degradation in which rare and distinctive features (such as the hump on a camel) were lost earliest in disease course, and there was an increase in the intrusion of prototypical features (such as the typical small ears of most mammals on an elephant) with more advanced disease. Crucially, patient drawings showed a continuum of conceptual knowledge loss rather than a binary 'present' or 'absent' state. Overall, we demonstrate that qualitative evaluation of line drawings of animals and objects provides fascinating insights into the transmodal semantic deficit in SD. Our results are consistent with a distributed-plus-hub model of semantic memory. The graded nature of the deficit in semantic performance observed in our subset of longitudinally observed patients suggests that the temporal lobe binds feature-based semantic attributes in its central convergence zone.

语义痴呆症(SD)的特征是由于前颞叶(ATL)神经变性导致的概念性知识的进行性损害。广泛的神经心理学评估可以定量地显示语义损伤,但这种分级的知识损失也可以通过定性地观察患者对单个概念的回忆很容易地观察到。在此,我们展示了双侧ATL孤立性萎缩的SD患者(19人)进行简单的 "根据名称绘制物体 "任务的结果。无论从横向还是纵向来看,患者的绘画都显示出一种退化模式,即罕见和独特的特征(如骆驼的驼峰)在病程中最早消失,而随着病程的延长,原型特征(如大象身上大多数哺乳动物的典型小耳朵)的侵入也在增加。最重要的是,病人的图画显示出概念性知识丧失的连续性,而不是二元的 "存在 "或 "缺失 "状态。总之,我们的研究表明,对动物和物体的线描进行定性评估,可以帮助我们深入了解 SD 的跨模态语义缺失。我们的研究结果与语义记忆的分布式加枢纽模型是一致的。我们在纵向观察的患者子集中观察到的语义表现缺陷的分级性质表明,颞叶将基于特征的语义属性绑定在其中央汇聚区。
{"title":"Drawing from name in semantic dementia reveals graded object knowledge representations in anterior temporal lobe.","authors":"Tanmay Anand, Karalyn Patterson, James B Rowe, Thomas E Cope","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01578-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01578-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Semantic dementia (SD) is characterized by progressive impairment in conceptual knowledge due to anterior temporal lobe (ATL) neurodegeneration. Extended neuropsychological assessments can quantitatively demonstrate the semantic impairment, but this graded loss of knowledge can also be readily observed in the qualitative observation of patients' recall of single concepts. Here, we present the results of a simple task of object drawing-from-name, by patients with SD (N = 19), who have isolated atrophy of the ATL bilaterally. Both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, patient drawings demonstrated a pattern of degradation in which rare and distinctive features (such as the hump on a camel) were lost earliest in disease course, and there was an increase in the intrusion of prototypical features (such as the typical small ears of most mammals on an elephant) with more advanced disease. Crucially, patient drawings showed a continuum of conceptual knowledge loss rather than a binary 'present' or 'absent' state. Overall, we demonstrate that qualitative evaluation of line drawings of animals and objects provides fascinating insights into the transmodal semantic deficit in SD. Our results are consistent with a distributed-plus-hub model of semantic memory. The graded nature of the deficit in semantic performance observed in our subset of longitudinally observed patients suggests that the temporal lobe binds feature-based semantic attributes in its central convergence zone.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141082771","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Noisy speech impairs retention of previously heard information only at short time scales. 嘈杂的语音只会在短时间内影响对先前听到的信息的记忆。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y
Violet A Brown, Katrina Sewell, Jed Villanueva, Julia F Strand

When speech is presented in noise, listeners must recruit cognitive resources to resolve the mismatch between the noisy input and representations in memory. A consequence of this effortful listening is impaired memory for content presented earlier. In the first study on effortful listening, Rabbitt, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 241-248 (1968; Experiment 2) found that recall for a list of digits was poorer when subsequent digits were presented with masking noise than without. Experiment 3 of that study extended this effect to more naturalistic, passage-length materials. Although the findings of Rabbitt's Experiment 2 have been replicated multiple times, no work has assessed the robustness of Experiment 3. We conducted a replication attempt of Rabbitt's Experiment 3 at three signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs). Results at one of the SNRs (Experiment 1a of the current study) were in the opposite direction from what Rabbitt, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 241-248, (1968) reported - that is, speech was recalled more accurately when it was followed by speech presented in noise rather than in the clear - and results at the other two SNRs showed no effect of noise (Experiments 1b and 1c). In addition, reanalysis of a replication of Rabbitt's seminal finding in his second experiment showed that the effect of effortful listening on previously presented information is transient. Thus, effortful listening caused by noise appears to only impair memory for information presented immediately before the noise, which may account for our finding that noise in the second-half of a long passage did not impair recall of information presented in the first half of the passage.

当语音在噪声中出现时,听者必须动用认知资源来解决噪声输入与记忆表象之间的不匹配问题。这种费力倾听的后果之一就是削弱了对先前呈现内容的记忆。在第一项关于费力倾听的研究中,Rabbitt,《实验心理学季刊》,20,241-248(1968 年;实验 2)发现,当后面的数字在有掩蔽噪声的情况下比没有掩蔽噪声的情况下记忆力更差。该研究的实验 3 将这一效应扩展到了更自然、段落更长的材料上。虽然拉比特的实验 2 已被多次重复,但还没有研究对实验 3 的稳健性进行评估。我们在三个信噪比(SNR)下对 Rabbitt 的实验 3 进行了复制尝试。其中一个信噪比(本研究的实验 1a)的结果与 Rabbitt(《实验心理学季刊》,20,241-248,(1968 年))报告的结果相反--即当语音之后出现的是噪音而非清晰语音时,语音的回忆更为准确--而另外两个信噪比的结果显示噪音没有影响(实验 1b 和 1c)。此外,对 Rabbitt 在第二个实验中的开创性发现的重复分析表明,努力倾听对先前呈现的信息的影响是短暂的。因此,由噪音引起的费力倾听似乎只会损害对噪音前信息的记忆,这可能是我们发现长段落后半部分的噪音不会损害对段落前半部分信息的记忆的原因。
{"title":"Noisy speech impairs retention of previously heard information only at short time scales.","authors":"Violet A Brown, Katrina Sewell, Jed Villanueva, Julia F Strand","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>When speech is presented in noise, listeners must recruit cognitive resources to resolve the mismatch between the noisy input and representations in memory. A consequence of this effortful listening is impaired memory for content presented earlier. In the first study on effortful listening, Rabbitt, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 241-248 (1968; Experiment 2) found that recall for a list of digits was poorer when subsequent digits were presented with masking noise than without. Experiment 3 of that study extended this effect to more naturalistic, passage-length materials. Although the findings of Rabbitt's Experiment 2 have been replicated multiple times, no work has assessed the robustness of Experiment 3. We conducted a replication attempt of Rabbitt's Experiment 3 at three signal-to-noise ratios (SNRs). Results at one of the SNRs (Experiment 1a of the current study) were in the opposite direction from what Rabbitt, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 20, 241-248, (1968) reported - that is, speech was recalled more accurately when it was followed by speech presented in noise rather than in the clear - and results at the other two SNRs showed no effect of noise (Experiments 1b and 1c). In addition, reanalysis of a replication of Rabbitt's seminal finding in his second experiment showed that the effect of effortful listening on previously presented information is transient. Thus, effortful listening caused by noise appears to only impair memory for information presented immediately before the noise, which may account for our finding that noise in the second-half of a long passage did not impair recall of information presented in the first half of the passage.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960221","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Noisy speech impairs retention of previously heard information only at short time scales. 嘈杂的语音只会在短时间内影响对先前听到的信息的记忆。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-17 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y
V. Brown, Katrina Sewell, Jed Villanueva, Julia F. Strand
{"title":"Noisy speech impairs retention of previously heard information only at short time scales.","authors":"V. Brown, Katrina Sewell, Jed Villanueva, Julia F. Strand","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01583-y","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-17","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140962691","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
No evidence for cross-paradigm transfer of abstract task knowledge in adults and school-aged children. 没有证据表明成人和学龄儿童的抽象任务知识存在跨范式迁移。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01581-0
Kaichi Yanaoka, Félice van 't Wout, Satoru Saito, Christopher Jarrold

Cognitive control is a hallmark of human cognition. A large number of studies have focused on the plasticity of cognitive control and examined how repeated task experience leads to the improvement of cognitive control in novel task environments. However, it has been demonstrated that training-induced changes are very selective and that transfer occurs within one task paradigm but not across different task paradigms. The current study tested the possibility that cross-paradigm transfer would occur if a common cognitive control strategy is employed across different task paradigms. Specifically, we examined whether prior experience of using reactive control in one task paradigm (i.e., either the cued task-switching paradigm or the AX-CPT) makes adults (N = 137) and 9- to 10-year-olds (N = 126) respond in a reactive way in a subsequent condition of another task paradigm in which proactive control could have been engaged. Bayesian generalized mixed-effects models revealed clear evidence of an absence of cross-paradigm transfer of reactive control in both adults and school-aged children. Based on these findings, we discuss to what extent learned control could be transferred across different task contexts and the task-specificity of proactive/reactive control strategies.

认知控制是人类认知的标志。大量研究都集中在认知控制的可塑性上,并探讨了重复任务经验如何导致认知控制在新任务环境中的改善。然而,研究表明,训练引起的变化具有很强的选择性,而且迁移发生在一个任务范式中,而不是不同任务范式之间。本研究测试了如果在不同任务范式中采用共同的认知控制策略,是否会发生跨范式迁移。具体来说,我们研究了在一个任务范式(即提示任务切换范式或AX-CPT)中使用被动控制的先前经验是否会使成人(137人)和9至10岁的儿童(126人)在另一个任务范式的后续条件中以被动的方式做出反应,而在另一个任务范式中本可以使用主动控制。贝叶斯广义混合效应模型显示,成人和学龄儿童的反应性控制没有发生跨范式转移。基于这些发现,我们讨论了所学控制在不同任务情境中的转移程度,以及主动/反应控制策略的任务特异性。
{"title":"No evidence for cross-paradigm transfer of abstract task knowledge in adults and school-aged children.","authors":"Kaichi Yanaoka, Félice van 't Wout, Satoru Saito, Christopher Jarrold","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01581-0","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01581-0","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Cognitive control is a hallmark of human cognition. A large number of studies have focused on the plasticity of cognitive control and examined how repeated task experience leads to the improvement of cognitive control in novel task environments. However, it has been demonstrated that training-induced changes are very selective and that transfer occurs within one task paradigm but not across different task paradigms. The current study tested the possibility that cross-paradigm transfer would occur if a common cognitive control strategy is employed across different task paradigms. Specifically, we examined whether prior experience of using reactive control in one task paradigm (i.e., either the cued task-switching paradigm or the AX-CPT) makes adults (N = 137) and 9- to 10-year-olds (N = 126) respond in a reactive way in a subsequent condition of another task paradigm in which proactive control could have been engaged. Bayesian generalized mixed-effects models revealed clear evidence of an absence of cross-paradigm transfer of reactive control in both adults and school-aged children. Based on these findings, we discuss to what extent learned control could be transferred across different task contexts and the task-specificity of proactive/reactive control strategies.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140945130","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory. 视觉工作记忆块的主动干扰牵涉到长时记忆。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w
Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan

Visual working memory (VWM) is a limited cognitive resource that can be functionally expanded through chunking (Miller, 1956). For example, participants can hold an increasing number of colours in mind as they learn to chunk reliably paired combinations (Brady et al., 2009). We investigated whether this benefit is mediated through the in situ compression of VWM representations (Brady et al., 2009) or the offloading of chunks to long-term memory (LTM; Huang & Awh, 2018; Ngiam et al., 2019) by asking if a vulnerability of LTM - proactive interference - influences VWM performance. We adapted previous designs using deterministic (Experiment 1, N = 60) and probabilistic pairings (Experiments 2 and 3, N = 64 and 80, respectively), to include colour pairings that swapped in sequence along with pairings that were consistent in sequence. Generally, participants reported colours from consistent pairs more accurately than from swapping pairs, which we designed to drive interference in LTM (Experiments 1 and 2). The error profiles also pointed to proactive interference between swapping pairs in all three experiments. Moreover, participants who had explicit awareness of frequent colour pairings had higher VWM accuracy, and their errors reflected more proactive interference than their unaware counterparts (Experiment 3). This pattern of long-term proactive interference in a VWM task lends support for accounts of VWM chunking that propose LTM offloading.

视觉工作记忆(VWM)是一种有限的认知资源,可以通过分块的方式进行功能扩展(Miller,1956 年)。例如,当参与者学会对可靠的配对组合进行分块时,他们可以记住越来越多的颜色(Brady 等人,2009 年)。我们研究了这种益处是否通过原位压缩VWM表征(Brady等人,2009年)或将分块卸载到长时记忆(LTM;Huang和Awh,2018年;Ngiam等人,2019年)来介导,询问LTM的脆弱性--主动干扰--是否会影响VWM的表现。我们调整了之前使用确定性配对(实验 1,N = 60)和概率性配对(实验 2 和 3,N = 64 和 80)的设计,加入了顺序互换的颜色配对和顺序一致的配对。一般来说,参与者报告一致配对中的颜色比报告交换配对中的颜色更准确,我们设计交换配对的目的是为了在 LTM 中产生干扰(实验 1 和 2)。在所有三个实验中,错误特征也表明交换配对之间存在主动干扰。此外,明确意识到频繁颜色配对的参与者具有更高的 VWM 准确性,他们的错误比未意识到的参与者反映出更多的主动干扰(实验 3)。这种在VWM任务中长期主动干扰的模式为提出LTM卸载的VWM分块理论提供了支持。
{"title":"Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory.","authors":"Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Visual working memory (VWM) is a limited cognitive resource that can be functionally expanded through chunking (Miller, 1956). For example, participants can hold an increasing number of colours in mind as they learn to chunk reliably paired combinations (Brady et al., 2009). We investigated whether this benefit is mediated through the in situ compression of VWM representations (Brady et al., 2009) or the offloading of chunks to long-term memory (LTM; Huang & Awh, 2018; Ngiam et al., 2019) by asking if a vulnerability of LTM - proactive interference - influences VWM performance. We adapted previous designs using deterministic (Experiment 1, N = 60) and probabilistic pairings (Experiments 2 and 3, N = 64 and 80, respectively), to include colour pairings that swapped in sequence along with pairings that were consistent in sequence. Generally, participants reported colours from consistent pairs more accurately than from swapping pairs, which we designed to drive interference in LTM (Experiments 1 and 2). The error profiles also pointed to proactive interference between swapping pairs in all three experiments. Moreover, participants who had explicit awareness of frequent colour pairings had higher VWM accuracy, and their errors reflected more proactive interference than their unaware counterparts (Experiment 3). This pattern of long-term proactive interference in a VWM task lends support for accounts of VWM chunking that propose LTM offloading.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140960177","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory. 视觉工作记忆块的主动干扰牵涉到长时记忆。
IF 2.4 3区 心理学 Q1 Arts and Humanities Pub Date : 2024-05-16 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w
Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan
{"title":"Proactive interference of visual working memory chunks implicates long-term memory.","authors":"Logan Doyle, Susanne Ferber, Katherine D Duncan","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01585-w","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.4,"publicationDate":"2024-05-16","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140966990","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory & Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1