首页 > 最新文献

Memory & Cognition最新文献

英文 中文
Can you help me? Using others to offload cognition. 你能帮我吗?利用他人卸载认知
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01621-9
Kristy L Armitage, Jonathan Redshaw

One of the most ancient and widely used forms of cognitive offloading is the outsourcing of cognitive operations onto other humans. Here, we explore whether humans preferentially seek out and use information from more competent compared with less competent others in an ongoing cognitive task. Participants (N = 120) completed a novel computerised visuospatial working memory task where each trial required them to remember either one, five, or ten target locations and recall them after a brief delay. Next, participants watched two virtual people compete in a distinct memory game, where one performed relatively well, demonstrating a stronger memory, and the other performed relatively poorly, demonstrating a weaker memory. Finally, participants completed the initial memory task again, but this time, either the strong-memory person or the weak-memory person was available to help with recall on each trial. Our results showed that, through observation and without direct instruction, participants acquired beliefs about the virtual people's cognitive proficiencies and could readily draw upon these beliefs to inform offloading decisions. Participants were typically more likely to ask for help from the strong-memory person, and this tendency was independent from other factors known to drive cognitive offloading more generally, like task difficulty, unaided cognitive ability, and metacognitive confidence.

将认知操作外包给其他人是一种最古老、最广泛使用的认知卸载形式。在这里,我们要探讨的是,在一项正在进行的认知任务中,与能力较弱的人相比,人类是否会优先寻找和使用能力较强的人提供的信息。参与者(120 人)完成了一项新颖的计算机视觉空间工作记忆任务,每次试验要求他们记住一个、五个或十个目标位置,并在短暂延迟后回忆起它们。接下来,参与者观看了两个虚拟人在一个不同的记忆游戏中的较量,其中一个表现相对较好,显示出较强的记忆力,而另一个表现相对较差,显示出较弱的记忆力。最后,参与者再次完成最初的记忆任务,但这一次,记忆力强的人或记忆力弱的人都可以在每次试验中帮助回忆。我们的研究结果表明,通过观察,在没有直接指导的情况下,参与者获得了关于虚拟人认知能力的信念,并能很容易地利用这些信念做出卸载决定。通常情况下,参与者更倾向于向记忆力强的人寻求帮助,而且这种倾向与其他已知的驱动认知卸载的因素无关,如任务难度、非辅助认知能力和元认知信心。
{"title":"Can you help me? Using others to offload cognition.","authors":"Kristy L Armitage, Jonathan Redshaw","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01621-9","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01621-9","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>One of the most ancient and widely used forms of cognitive offloading is the outsourcing of cognitive operations onto other humans. Here, we explore whether humans preferentially seek out and use information from more competent compared with less competent others in an ongoing cognitive task. Participants (N = 120) completed a novel computerised visuospatial working memory task where each trial required them to remember either one, five, or ten target locations and recall them after a brief delay. Next, participants watched two virtual people compete in a distinct memory game, where one performed relatively well, demonstrating a stronger memory, and the other performed relatively poorly, demonstrating a weaker memory. Finally, participants completed the initial memory task again, but this time, either the strong-memory person or the weak-memory person was available to help with recall on each trial. Our results showed that, through observation and without direct instruction, participants acquired beliefs about the virtual people's cognitive proficiencies and could readily draw upon these beliefs to inform offloading decisions. Participants were typically more likely to ask for help from the strong-memory person, and this tendency was independent from other factors known to drive cognitive offloading more generally, like task difficulty, unaided cognitive ability, and metacognitive confidence.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019125","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Flexible word position coding in reading: Roles for attention and memory. 阅读中灵活的单词位置编码:注意力和记忆的作用
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-22 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01623-7
Joshua Snell

Readers may fail to notice the error in 'Do love you me?': this is the recently established transposed-word (TW) phenomenon. Word position coding is a novel cognitive construct, and researchers are presently debating the underlying mechanisms. Here I investigated roles for attention and memory. Participants (N = 54) made grammaticality judgements to four-word sequences that formed correct sentences ('The man can run', 'The dog was here'), TW sentences ('The can man run', 'The was dog here'), or ungrammatical control sentences ('The man dog run', 'The was can here'). Sequences were replaced by post-masks after 200 ms, and that post-mask was accompanied by a 50-ms retro-cue in the form of an 'X' presented at a critical location (where one could have locally inferred grammaticality; e.g., between the first and second word of 'The was dog here') or a non-critical location (e.g., between the third and fourth word of 'The was dog here'). TW sentences were harder to reject than control sentences - the classic TW effect - and crucially, this effect was modulated by cue validity, with valid cues attenuating TW effects compared to invalid cues. The present results suggest that focused attention aids the process of binding words to locations. Furthermore, as cues appeared after sentence offset, these results suggest that word position coding may take place in memory.

读者可能没有注意到 "Do love you me? "中的错误:这就是最近确立的换位词(TW)现象。词位编码是一种新的认知结构,研究人员目前正在对其潜在机制进行争论。在这里,我研究了注意力和记忆的作用。参与者(N = 54)对组成正确句子("人可以跑"、"狗在这里")、TW 句子("人可以跑"、"狗在这里")或无语法对照句子("人狗跑"、"人可以在这里")的四词序列进行语法判断。序列在 200 毫秒后被后置掩码取代,后置掩码伴随着 50 毫秒的 "X "回溯提示,提示出现在关键位置(可以局部推断语法性的位置,如 "The was dog here "的第一个词和第二个词之间)或非关键位置(如 "The was dog here "的第三个词和第四个词之间)。TW句子比对照句子更难被拒绝--这就是典型的TW效应--而且关键的是,这种效应受线索有效性的调节,与无效线索相比,有效线索会减弱TW效应。本研究结果表明,集中注意力有助于将单词与位置结合起来。此外,由于线索出现在句子偏移之后,这些结果表明单词位置编码可能发生在记忆中。
{"title":"Flexible word position coding in reading: Roles for attention and memory.","authors":"Joshua Snell","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01623-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01623-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Readers may fail to notice the error in 'Do love you me?': this is the recently established transposed-word (TW) phenomenon. Word position coding is a novel cognitive construct, and researchers are presently debating the underlying mechanisms. Here I investigated roles for attention and memory. Participants (N = 54) made grammaticality judgements to four-word sequences that formed correct sentences ('The man can run', 'The dog was here'), TW sentences ('The can man run', 'The was dog here'), or ungrammatical control sentences ('The man dog run', 'The was can here'). Sequences were replaced by post-masks after 200 ms, and that post-mask was accompanied by a 50-ms retro-cue in the form of an 'X' presented at a critical location (where one could have locally inferred grammaticality; e.g., between the first and second word of 'The was dog here') or a non-critical location (e.g., between the third and fourth word of 'The was dog here'). TW sentences were harder to reject than control sentences - the classic TW effect - and crucially, this effect was modulated by cue validity, with valid cues attenuating TW effects compared to invalid cues. The present results suggest that focused attention aids the process of binding words to locations. Furthermore, as cues appeared after sentence offset, these results suggest that word position coding may take place in memory.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-22","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142019126","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Decisional components of motor responses are not related to online response control: Evidence from lexical decision and speed-accuracy tradeoff manipulations. 运动反应的决策成分与在线反应控制无关:来自词汇决策和速度-准确性权衡操作的证据。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-19 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01619-3
Michele Scaltritti, Elena Greatti, Simone Sulpizio

Evidence suggests that decision processes can propagate to motor-response execution. However, the functional characterization of motor decisional components is not yet fully understood. By combining a classic lexical decision experiment with manipulations of speed-accuracy tradeoff (SAT), the present experiment assessed the hypothesis that decisional effects on chronometric measures of motor-response execution are related to online response control. The electromyographic (EMG) signal associated with manual button-press responses was used to dissociate the premotor component (from stimulus onset until the onset of the EMG activity) from the motor component (from EMG onset until the button-press), thus enabling the assessment of decision-related effects in terms of motor-response duration within single-trial reaction times. Other than replicating all the previously reported SAT effects, the experiment revealed hindered control processes when the instructions emphasized speed over accuracy, as indicated by measures of response control such as partial errors, fast errors, and correction likelihood. Nonetheless, the lexicality effect on motor responses, consisting of slower motor times for pseudowords compared to words, was impervious to any SAT modulation. The results suggest that SAT-induced variations in decision and response control policies may not be the prominent determinant of decision-related effects on motor times, highlighting the multiple "cognitive" components that affect peripheral response execution.

有证据表明,决策过程可以传播到运动反应的执行过程中。然而,人们对运动决策成分的功能特征尚未完全了解。通过将经典的词汇决策实验与速度-准确性权衡(SAT)操作相结合,本实验评估了这样一个假设,即决策对运动反应执行的计时测量的影响与在线反应控制有关。与手动按键反应相关的肌电图(EMG)信号被用来将前运动部分(从刺激开始到肌电图活动开始)与运动部分(从肌电图开始到按键)分离开来,从而能够在单次试验反应时间内评估运动反应持续时间的决策相关效应。除了复制之前报道的所有 SAT 效应外,实验还显示,当指令强调速度而非准确性时,控制过程会受到阻碍,这一点可以从部分错误、快速错误和纠正可能性等反应控制测量中看出。然而,词性效应对运动反应的影响,包括假词的运动时间比词慢,却不受任何 SAT 调节的影响。这些结果表明,由 SAT 引起的决策和反应控制策略的变化可能并不是决策相关效应对运动时间影响的主要决定因素,从而突出了影响外围反应执行的多种 "认知 "成分。
{"title":"Decisional components of motor responses are not related to online response control: Evidence from lexical decision and speed-accuracy tradeoff manipulations.","authors":"Michele Scaltritti, Elena Greatti, Simone Sulpizio","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01619-3","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01619-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Evidence suggests that decision processes can propagate to motor-response execution. However, the functional characterization of motor decisional components is not yet fully understood. By combining a classic lexical decision experiment with manipulations of speed-accuracy tradeoff (SAT), the present experiment assessed the hypothesis that decisional effects on chronometric measures of motor-response execution are related to online response control. The electromyographic (EMG) signal associated with manual button-press responses was used to dissociate the premotor component (from stimulus onset until the onset of the EMG activity) from the motor component (from EMG onset until the button-press), thus enabling the assessment of decision-related effects in terms of motor-response duration within single-trial reaction times. Other than replicating all the previously reported SAT effects, the experiment revealed hindered control processes when the instructions emphasized speed over accuracy, as indicated by measures of response control such as partial errors, fast errors, and correction likelihood. Nonetheless, the lexicality effect on motor responses, consisting of slower motor times for pseudowords compared to words, was impervious to any SAT modulation. The results suggest that SAT-induced variations in decision and response control policies may not be the prominent determinant of decision-related effects on motor times, highlighting the multiple \"cognitive\" components that affect peripheral response execution.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-19","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"142001074","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Does the extension of free time trigger spontaneous elaborative strategies in working memory? 自由时间的延长会触发工作记忆中的自发阐述策略吗?
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7
Inès Leproult, Benoît Lemaire, Sophie Portrat

Elaboration has emerged as a potential maintenance mechanism involved in the substantial contribution of long-term memory (LTM) to working memory (WM) performance. The objective of the current study was to determine whether elaborative strategies could be spontaneously implemented under favorable conditions. Across four experiments, the distribution of free-time periods was manipulated in a complex span task, while keeping the total amount of free time and cognitive load constant. As elaboration requires time to be set up, Experiment 1 elicited better WM performance in a condition with fewer long free-time periods compared to a condition with many short free-time periods. However, because this benefit did not persist during delayed recall, the following experiments aimed to further investigate this effect by manipulating factors supposed to modulate elaboration. In Experiment 2, half of the participants received no specific instructions regarding strategies whereas the other half were encouraged to use elaborative strategies. In Experiment 3, the to-be-maintained stimuli did or did not have LTM representations that are essential for elaboration (i.e., words or pseudowords). Finally, the last experiment used a self-strategy report to better understand the nature of the WM maintenance strategies spontaneously employed by participants. Despite a consistent effect of free time manipulation on WM recall, the explanatory assumption of elaboration was challenged by the unexpected lack of effect on LTM recall and on the type of strategy reported. Alternative explanations stemming from well-known factors influencing WM performance are discussed, and emphasis is placed on the potential contribution of direct semantic maintenance in WM.

详细化已成为一种潜在的维持机制,它参与了长时记忆(LTM)对工作记忆(WM)表现的实质性贡献。本研究的目的是确定在有利条件下能否自发地实施详细化策略。在四次实验中,我们在保持自由时间总量和认知负荷不变的情况下,对复杂跨度任务中自由时间的分布进行了操作。由于精心设计需要时间来建立,因此实验 1 在较少长自由时间段的条件下比在较多短自由时间段的条件下获得了更好的 WM 成绩。然而,由于这种优势在延迟回忆中并不持续,因此接下来的实验旨在通过操纵那些被认为会调节详细化的因素来进一步研究这种效应。在实验 2 中,一半的被试没有接受任何具体的策略指导,而另一半的被试则被鼓励使用详细化策略。在实验 3 中,待保留的刺激物是否具有对阐释至关重要的 LTM 表征(即单词或假词)。最后,最后一项实验使用了自我策略报告,以更好地了解参与者自发使用的 WM 维持策略的性质。尽管自由时间操作对 WM 回忆的影响是一致的,但对 LTM 回忆和所报告策略类型的影响却出乎意料地缺乏,这对阐述的解释性假设提出了挑战。我们讨论了影响 WM 表现的众所周知的因素所产生的其他解释,并强调了直接语义维持在 WM 中的潜在贡献。
{"title":"Does the extension of free time trigger spontaneous elaborative strategies in working memory?","authors":"Inès Leproult, Benoît Lemaire, Sophie Portrat","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01615-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Elaboration has emerged as a potential maintenance mechanism involved in the substantial contribution of long-term memory (LTM) to working memory (WM) performance. The objective of the current study was to determine whether elaborative strategies could be spontaneously implemented under favorable conditions. Across four experiments, the distribution of free-time periods was manipulated in a complex span task, while keeping the total amount of free time and cognitive load constant. As elaboration requires time to be set up, Experiment 1 elicited better WM performance in a condition with fewer long free-time periods compared to a condition with many short free-time periods. However, because this benefit did not persist during delayed recall, the following experiments aimed to further investigate this effect by manipulating factors supposed to modulate elaboration. In Experiment 2, half of the participants received no specific instructions regarding strategies whereas the other half were encouraged to use elaborative strategies. In Experiment 3, the to-be-maintained stimuli did or did not have LTM representations that are essential for elaboration (i.e., words or pseudowords). Finally, the last experiment used a self-strategy report to better understand the nature of the WM maintenance strategies spontaneously employed by participants. Despite a consistent effect of free time manipulation on WM recall, the explanatory assumption of elaboration was challenged by the unexpected lack of effect on LTM recall and on the type of strategy reported. Alternative explanations stemming from well-known factors influencing WM performance are discussed, and emphasis is placed on the potential contribution of direct semantic maintenance in WM.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-12","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141917862","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Play it again, but more sadly: Influence of timbre, mode, and musical experience in melody processing. 再弹一遍,但更悲伤:音色、模式和音乐经验对旋律处理的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-02 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01614-8
James Armitage, Tuomas Eerola, Andrea R Halpern

The emotional properties of music are influenced by a host of factors, such as timbre, mode, harmony, and tempo. In this paper, we consider how two of these factors, mode (major vs. minor) and timbre interact to influence ratings of perceived valence, reaction time, and recognition memory. More specifically, we considered the notion of congruence-that is, we used a set of melodies that crossed modes typically perceived as happy and sad (i.e., major and minor) in Western cultures with instruments typically perceived as happy and sad (i.e., marimba and viola). In a reaction-time experiment, participants were asked to classify melodies as happy or sad as quickly as possible. There was a clear congruency effect-that is, when the mode and timbre were congruent (major/marimba or minor/viola), reaction times were shorter than when the mode and timbre were incongruent (major/viola or minor/marimba). In Experiment 2, participants first rated the melodies for valence, before completing a recognition task. Melodies that were initially presented in incongruent conditions in the rating task were subsequently recognized better in the recognition task. The recognition advantage for melodies presented in incongruent conditions is discussed in the context of desirable difficulty.

音乐的情感属性受到一系列因素的影响,如音色、模式、和声和节奏。在本文中,我们考虑了其中的两个因素--模式(大调与小调)和音色--如何相互作用,影响对感知价值、反应时间和识别记忆的评分。更具体地说,我们考虑了一致性的概念--也就是说,我们使用了一组在西方文化中通常被认为是快乐和悲伤的模式(即大调和小调)与通常被认为是快乐和悲伤的乐器(即马林巴琴和中提琴)。在反应时间实验中,参与者被要求以最快的速度将旋律划分为快乐或悲伤。实验中出现了明显的一致效应,即当模式和音色一致时(大调/马林巴琴或小调/中提琴),反应时间比模式和音色不一致时(大调/中提琴或小调/马林巴琴)短。在实验 2 中,受试者在完成识别任务之前,首先对旋律的情感进行评分。在评级任务中,最初在不协调条件下出现的旋律在随后的识别任务中识别率更高。我们将从理想难度的角度来讨论在不协调条件下呈现的旋律的识别优势。
{"title":"Play it again, but more sadly: Influence of timbre, mode, and musical experience in melody processing.","authors":"James Armitage, Tuomas Eerola, Andrea R Halpern","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01614-8","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-024-01614-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The emotional properties of music are influenced by a host of factors, such as timbre, mode, harmony, and tempo. In this paper, we consider how two of these factors, mode (major vs. minor) and timbre interact to influence ratings of perceived valence, reaction time, and recognition memory. More specifically, we considered the notion of congruence-that is, we used a set of melodies that crossed modes typically perceived as happy and sad (i.e., major and minor) in Western cultures with instruments typically perceived as happy and sad (i.e., marimba and viola). In a reaction-time experiment, participants were asked to classify melodies as happy or sad as quickly as possible. There was a clear congruency effect-that is, when the mode and timbre were congruent (major/marimba or minor/viola), reaction times were shorter than when the mode and timbre were incongruent (major/viola or minor/marimba). In Experiment 2, participants first rated the melodies for valence, before completing a recognition task. Melodies that were initially presented in incongruent conditions in the rating task were subsequently recognized better in the recognition task. The recognition advantage for melodies presented in incongruent conditions is discussed in the context of desirable difficulty.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-02","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"141879549","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Meta-memory (prediction) of specific autobiographical recall: An experimental approach using a modified autobiographical memory test. 特定自传回忆的元记忆(预测):使用改良自传体记忆测试的实验方法。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-02-27 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01541-8
Noboru Matsumoto

Autobiographical memory specificity is known to contribute to better mental health, social problem-solving, and episodic future thinking. While numerous studies have addressed variables that affect autobiographical memory specificity, little is known regarding the meta-memory processes that underpin memory retrieval. In this study, we introduced two meta-memory constructs, ease of retrieval judgments and anticipation of negative emotion evoked, which potentially affect autobiographical memory specificity. Participants (N = 109) first rated the ease of retrieval and anticipated emotions for positive and negative words used in a subsequent autobiographical memory test. We used the Optional Instructions of the Autobiographical Memory Test, in which participants were instructed that "specific memories are better, but other memories are permissible," allowing them to adjust how much cognitive effort they spent on generative retrieval after a failure of direct retrieval. They also self-judged whether each retrieval was generative (using additional cues with cognitive effort) or direct (immediate recall without much cognitive effort). Results showed that for generative retrieval, ease of retrieval was associated with greater specific and general memories and fewer omissions. A more negative anticipated emotion was associated with fewer specific memories and greater omissions, but was not with general memories. These results suggest that low retrievability and anticipated negative emotion prevent individuals from devoting efforts to generative retrieval. The lack of association between anticipated negative emotion and general memory calls into question the functional avoidance hypothesis regarding autobiographical memory specificity. We discussed how participants judged these meta-memories and directions for future research.

众所周知,自传体记忆的特异性有助于改善心理健康、社会问题的解决以及对未来的历时性思考。虽然许多研究都探讨了影响自传体记忆特异性的变量,但对支撑记忆检索的元记忆过程却知之甚少。在本研究中,我们引入了两个元记忆建构,即检索判断的难易程度和对负面情绪诱发的预期,它们可能会影响自传体记忆的特异性。受试者(109 人)首先对随后进行的自传体记忆测试中使用的正面和负面词语的检索难易程度和预期情绪进行评分。我们使用了自传体记忆测试的 "可选说明",其中指示受试者 "特定记忆更好,但其他记忆也是允许的",允许他们在直接检索失败后调整用于生成性检索的认知努力程度。他们还自我判断每次检索是生成性检索(使用额外的线索并付出认知努力)还是直接检索(无需付出太多认知努力即可立即回忆)。结果表明,在生成性检索中,检索的容易程度与更多的具体记忆和一般记忆以及更少的遗漏有关。更消极的预期情绪与更少的特定记忆和更多的遗漏有关,但与一般记忆无关。这些结果表明,可检索性低和预期的负面情绪会阻碍个体将精力投入到生成性检索中。预期的负面情绪与一般记忆之间缺乏关联,这让我们对有关自传体记忆特异性的功能性回避假说产生了质疑。我们讨论了参与者如何判断这些元记忆以及未来的研究方向。
{"title":"Meta-memory (prediction) of specific autobiographical recall: An experimental approach using a modified autobiographical memory test.","authors":"Noboru Matsumoto","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01541-8","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01541-8","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Autobiographical memory specificity is known to contribute to better mental health, social problem-solving, and episodic future thinking. While numerous studies have addressed variables that affect autobiographical memory specificity, little is known regarding the meta-memory processes that underpin memory retrieval. In this study, we introduced two meta-memory constructs, ease of retrieval judgments and anticipation of negative emotion evoked, which potentially affect autobiographical memory specificity. Participants (N = 109) first rated the ease of retrieval and anticipated emotions for positive and negative words used in a subsequent autobiographical memory test. We used the Optional Instructions of the Autobiographical Memory Test, in which participants were instructed that \"specific memories are better, but other memories are permissible,\" allowing them to adjust how much cognitive effort they spent on generative retrieval after a failure of direct retrieval. They also self-judged whether each retrieval was generative (using additional cues with cognitive effort) or direct (immediate recall without much cognitive effort). Results showed that for generative retrieval, ease of retrieval was associated with greater specific and general memories and fewer omissions. A more negative anticipated emotion was associated with fewer specific memories and greater omissions, but was not with general memories. These results suggest that low retrievability and anticipated negative emotion prevent individuals from devoting efforts to generative retrieval. The lack of association between anticipated negative emotion and general memory calls into question the functional avoidance hypothesis regarding autobiographical memory specificity. We discussed how participants judged these meta-memories and directions for future research.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139974008","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Exploring the accuracy of musical tempo memory: The effects of reproduction method, reference tempo, and musical expertise. 探索音乐节奏记忆的准确性:再现方法、参考节奏和音乐专业知识的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-20 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01543-6
Julia Vigl, Friederike Koehler, Heike Henning

Although people commonly remember and recreate the tempo of musical pieces with high accuracy, comparatively less is known regarding sources of potential variation in musical tempo memory. This study therefore aimed to investigate musical tempo memory accuracy and the effects of reference tempo, reproduction method, musical expertise, and their interaction. A sample of 403 individuals with varying levels of musical training participated in the experimental online study, including nonmusicians, amateur musicians, and professional musicians. Participants were tasked with reproducing the tempos of 19 popular pop/rock songs using two methods: tempo tapping and adjusting the tempo of the audio file based on the previously tapped tempo. Results from multilevel models revealed overall high accuracy in tempo memory, with tempo adjusting yielding greater accuracy compared with tempo tapping. Higher musical expertise was associated with increased accuracy in tempo production. In addition, we observed a quadratic effect of reference tempo, with the greatest accuracy in tempo reproduction around 120 bpm. Gender, age, familiarity with the pieces, and accompaniment strategies were also associated with greater accuracy. These findings provide insights into the factors influencing musical tempo memory and have implications for understanding the cognitive processes involved in tempo perception and reproduction.

尽管人们对音乐作品节奏的记忆和再现通常具有很高的准确性,但对音乐节奏记忆潜在差异的来源却知之甚少。因此,本研究旨在调查音乐节奏记忆的准确性,以及参考节奏、再现方法、音乐专业知识及其相互作用的影响。403 名受过不同程度音乐训练的人参加了在线实验研究,其中包括非音乐家、业余音乐家和专业音乐家。参与者的任务是使用两种方法重现 19 首流行/摇滚歌曲的节奏:敲击节奏和根据先前敲击的节奏调整音频文件的节奏。多层次模型的结果显示,节奏记忆的总体准确率很高,与敲击节奏相比,调整节奏的准确率更高。音乐专业知识越高,节奏制作的准确性也越高。此外,我们还观察到参考节奏的四次方效应,120 bpm 左右的节奏重现准确性最高。性别、年龄、对乐曲的熟悉程度和伴奏策略也与更高的准确性有关。这些发现为音乐节奏记忆的影响因素提供了启示,并对理解节奏感知和再现的认知过程具有重要意义。
{"title":"Exploring the accuracy of musical tempo memory: The effects of reproduction method, reference tempo, and musical expertise.","authors":"Julia Vigl, Friederike Koehler, Heike Henning","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01543-6","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01543-6","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Although people commonly remember and recreate the tempo of musical pieces with high accuracy, comparatively less is known regarding sources of potential variation in musical tempo memory. This study therefore aimed to investigate musical tempo memory accuracy and the effects of reference tempo, reproduction method, musical expertise, and their interaction. A sample of 403 individuals with varying levels of musical training participated in the experimental online study, including nonmusicians, amateur musicians, and professional musicians. Participants were tasked with reproducing the tempos of 19 popular pop/rock songs using two methods: tempo tapping and adjusting the tempo of the audio file based on the previously tapped tempo. Results from multilevel models revealed overall high accuracy in tempo memory, with tempo adjusting yielding greater accuracy compared with tempo tapping. Higher musical expertise was associated with increased accuracy in tempo production. In addition, we observed a quadratic effect of reference tempo, with the greatest accuracy in tempo reproduction around 120 bpm. Gender, age, familiarity with the pieces, and accompaniment strategies were also associated with greater accuracy. These findings provide insights into the factors influencing musical tempo memory and have implications for understanding the cognitive processes involved in tempo perception and reproduction.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362532/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140177243","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Short-term retention of words as a function of encoding depth. 单词的短期保留是编码深度的函数。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-12 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01546-3
Cayden O Lawrence, Dominic Guitard, Nelson Cowan

The traditional short- and long-term storage view of information processing and the levels-of-processing view both discuss the forgetting of information over time. In the traditional stage view, there is loss of at least poorly encoded information across several seconds when the information cannot be rehearsed (e.g., Ricker et al., 2020, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 60-76). In the levels-of-processing approach, information that is encoded in a shallow manner is lost more quickly over time than deeply-encoded information (Craik & Lockhart, 1972, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.). Previous studies of the depth of encoding, however, have mostly been conducted using delayed tests, so there are few studies directly comparing the rate of forgetting over time for information as a function of different depths of encoding. We manipulated the level of processing with immediate recall in a modified Brown-Peterson task. An effect of the level of processing was robust, but evidence of forgetting across retention intervals was not always observed. When encoding time was curtailed (in Experiments 3 and 4), we found main effects of both the level of processing and the retention interval, but no interaction between the two variables. The results suggest that the depth-of-encoding effect may occur during the initial encoding of items, but without differential forgetting within the range of retention intervals that we examined (0-18 s), in contrast to the suggestion by Craik and Lockhart. Further work is needed to determine whether the depth-of-processing effect would grow over longer intervals.

信息加工的传统短期和长期储存观点以及加工层次观点都讨论了信息随时间遗忘的问题。在传统的阶段性观点中,当信息无法重新演练时,至少会在几秒钟内丢失编码较差的信息(例如,Ricker 等人,2020,《学习、记忆与认知》,46,60-76)。在处理水平法中,浅层编码的信息随着时间的推移比深层编码的信息丢失得更快(Craik & Lockhart,1972,Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior,11,671-684)。然而,以往对编码深度的研究大多采用延迟测试,因此很少有研究直接比较不同编码深度的信息随时间的遗忘率。我们在一项改良的布朗-彼得森任务中用即时回忆操纵了处理水平。处理水平的影响是稳健的,但并不总能观察到遗忘跨越保留间隔的证据。当编码时间被缩短时(在实验 3 和 4 中),我们发现处理水平和保留间隔都有主效应,但这两个变量之间没有交互作用。这些结果表明,编码深度效应可能发生在项目的初始编码过程中,但在我们研究的保留时间间隔范围内(0-18 秒)并没有不同的遗忘,这与 Craik 和 Lockhart 的观点相反。要确定处理深度效应是否会在更长的时间间隔内增长,还需要进一步的研究。
{"title":"Short-term retention of words as a function of encoding depth.","authors":"Cayden O Lawrence, Dominic Guitard, Nelson Cowan","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01546-3","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01546-3","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The traditional short- and long-term storage view of information processing and the levels-of-processing view both discuss the forgetting of information over time. In the traditional stage view, there is loss of at least poorly encoded information across several seconds when the information cannot be rehearsed (e.g., Ricker et al., 2020, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 60-76). In the levels-of-processing approach, information that is encoded in a shallow manner is lost more quickly over time than deeply-encoded information (Craik & Lockhart, 1972, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684.). Previous studies of the depth of encoding, however, have mostly been conducted using delayed tests, so there are few studies directly comparing the rate of forgetting over time for information as a function of different depths of encoding. We manipulated the level of processing with immediate recall in a modified Brown-Peterson task. An effect of the level of processing was robust, but evidence of forgetting across retention intervals was not always observed. When encoding time was curtailed (in Experiments 3 and 4), we found main effects of both the level of processing and the retention interval, but no interaction between the two variables. The results suggest that the depth-of-encoding effect may occur during the initial encoding of items, but without differential forgetting within the range of retention intervals that we examined (0-18 s), in contrast to the suggestion by Craik and Lockhart. Further work is needed to determine whether the depth-of-processing effect would grow over longer intervals.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140111900","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Impact of process interference on memory encoding and retrieval processes in dual-task situations. 过程干扰对双任务情况下记忆编码和检索过程的影响。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-04 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01539-2
Sandra Hensen, Iring Koch, Patricia Hirsch

Dual-tasks at the memory encoding stage have been shown to decrease recall performance and impair concurrent task performance. In contrast, studies on the effect of dual-tasks at the memory retrieval stage observed mixed results. Which cognitive mechanisms are underlying this dual-task interference is still an unresolved question. In the present study, we investigated the influence of a concurrent reaction-time task on the performance in a long-term memory task in two experiments. In Experiment 1, participants performed an auditory-verbal free recall memory task and a visual-manual spatial Stroop task in a single or dual-task condition, either at the encoding or retrieval stage of the memory task. In Experiment 2, we examined the influence of processing conflicts in a concurrent RT task on memory encoding. Both experiments showed detrimental effects on recall accuracy and concurrent RT task performance in dual-task conditions at the encoding stage. Dual-task conditions at the retrieval stage led to a slowdown in recall latency and impaired concurrent RT task performance, but recall accuracy was maintained. In addition, we observed larger Stroop congruency effects in the dual-task conditions, indicating an increased processing conflict. However, in Experiment 2, we analyzed the effect of the processing conflict in a time-locked manner and could not find a significant influence on success of memory encoding. These findings suggest that processes in both tasks share the same limited capacity and are slowed down due to parallel processing, but we could not find evidence that this is further influenced by task-specific processing conflicts.

研究表明,记忆编码阶段的双重任务会降低记忆成绩,并影响并发任务的成绩。与此相反,关于记忆检索阶段双重任务影响的研究结果不一。双任务干扰的认知机制是什么仍是一个悬而未决的问题。在本研究中,我们通过两个实验研究了同时进行的反应时任务对长期记忆任务成绩的影响。在实验 1 中,被试在记忆任务的编码或检索阶段,在单任务或双任务条件下完成听觉-言语自由回忆记忆任务和视觉-手动空间 Stroop 任务。在实验 2 中,我们研究了同时进行的 RT 任务中的处理冲突对记忆编码的影响。这两项实验都表明,在编码阶段的双任务条件下,回忆的准确性和并发RT任务的表现都会受到不利影响。检索阶段的双任务条件导致了回忆潜伏期的减慢,并影响了并发 RT 任务的表现,但回忆的准确性得以保持。此外,我们在双任务条件下观察到了更大的 Stroop 一致性效应,这表明处理冲突增加了。然而,在实验 2 中,我们以时间锁定的方式分析了加工冲突的影响,结果没有发现加工冲突对记忆编码的成功率有显著影响。这些发现表明,两个任务中的处理过程共享相同的有限能力,并且由于并行处理而减慢了速度,但我们找不到证据表明这受到特定任务处理冲突的进一步影响。
{"title":"Impact of process interference on memory encoding and retrieval processes in dual-task situations.","authors":"Sandra Hensen, Iring Koch, Patricia Hirsch","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01539-2","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01539-2","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Dual-tasks at the memory encoding stage have been shown to decrease recall performance and impair concurrent task performance. In contrast, studies on the effect of dual-tasks at the memory retrieval stage observed mixed results. Which cognitive mechanisms are underlying this dual-task interference is still an unresolved question. In the present study, we investigated the influence of a concurrent reaction-time task on the performance in a long-term memory task in two experiments. In Experiment 1, participants performed an auditory-verbal free recall memory task and a visual-manual spatial Stroop task in a single or dual-task condition, either at the encoding or retrieval stage of the memory task. In Experiment 2, we examined the influence of processing conflicts in a concurrent RT task on memory encoding. Both experiments showed detrimental effects on recall accuracy and concurrent RT task performance in dual-task conditions at the encoding stage. Dual-task conditions at the retrieval stage led to a slowdown in recall latency and impaired concurrent RT task performance, but recall accuracy was maintained. In addition, we observed larger Stroop congruency effects in the dual-task conditions, indicating an increased processing conflict. However, in Experiment 2, we analyzed the effect of the processing conflict in a time-locked manner and could not find a significant influence on success of memory encoding. These findings suggest that processes in both tasks share the same limited capacity and are slowed down due to parallel processing, but we could not find evidence that this is further influenced by task-specific processing conflicts.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11362513/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140023054","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
The limited memory of value following value directed encoding. 值定向编码后的值记忆有限。
IF 2.2 3区 心理学 Q2 PSYCHOLOGY, EXPERIMENTAL Pub Date : 2024-08-01 Epub Date: 2024-03-18 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-024-01550-7
Gizem Filiz, Ian G Dobbins

Items associated with higher values during encoding are later recognized and recalled better than are lower valued items. During recall paradigms, these value directed encoding (VDE) effects heavily depend upon learned strategies acquired during repeated testing with earnings feedback. However, because VDE effects also occur in single test recognition designs, precluding such learning, it has been suggested that high value may automatically induce good encoding. We tested this by manipulating encoding instructions (Experiments 1a and 1b) and manipulating concurrent levels of processing (LOP) requirements during encoding (Experiment 2a and 2b). Two main findings emerged. First, subject initiated strategies played a dominant role in VDE effects with little evidence for automaticity. This was demonstrated in Experiment 1 by a more than three-fold increase in the VDE recognition effect when instructions specifically encouraged selective elaboration of high-value items. It was also shown by the complete elimination of VDE recognition effects in Experiment 2 when LOP tasks were concurrently performed during encoding. Critically, the blocking of VDE effects occurred even though a catch trial procedure verified that value was being processed during encoding and remained even when subjects had unlimited time to process the materials during encoding. Second, the data showed, for the first time, that when subjects attempted to specify the value of recognized items, they heavily depended upon a recognition heuristic in which increases in recognition strength, even when nondiagnostic, were inferred to reflect high encoding value. The tendency for subjects to conflate recognition strength and value may have important implications for behavioral economics.

与价值较低的项目相比,在编码过程中与价值较高的项目后来会被更好地识别和回忆。在回忆范式中,这些价值定向编码(VDE)效应在很大程度上取决于在有收益反馈的重复测试中习得的策略。然而,由于在单次测试识别设计中也会出现 VDE 效应,从而排除了这种学习,因此有人认为高价值可能会自动诱发良好的编码。我们通过操纵编码指令(实验 1a 和 1b)和操纵编码过程中的并发处理水平(LOP)要求(实验 2a 和 2b)来测试这一点。得出了两个主要发现。首先,受试者启动的策略在 VDE 效应中起主导作用,几乎没有自动性的证据。在实验 1 中,当指令特别鼓励选择性地阐述高价值项目时,VDE 识别效果增加了三倍多,这就证明了这一点。在实验 2 中,当编码期间同时进行 LOP 任务时,VDE 识别效应完全消失,也证明了这一点。重要的是,即使捕捉试验程序验证了被试在编码过程中对价值进行了处理,VDE 效应的阻断仍然发生;即使被试在编码过程中有无限的时间对材料进行处理,VDE 效应的阻断仍然存在。其次,数据首次表明,当被试试图明确识别项目的价值时,他们在很大程度上依赖于一种识别启发式,在这种启发式中,识别强度的增加,即使是非诊断性的,也被推断为反映了高编码价值。被试将识别强度和价值混为一谈的倾向可能对行为经济学有重要影响。
{"title":"The limited memory of value following value directed encoding.","authors":"Gizem Filiz, Ian G Dobbins","doi":"10.3758/s13421-024-01550-7","DOIUrl":"10.3758/s13421-024-01550-7","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Items associated with higher values during encoding are later recognized and recalled better than are lower valued items. During recall paradigms, these value directed encoding (VDE) effects heavily depend upon learned strategies acquired during repeated testing with earnings feedback. However, because VDE effects also occur in single test recognition designs, precluding such learning, it has been suggested that high value may automatically induce good encoding. We tested this by manipulating encoding instructions (Experiments 1a and 1b) and manipulating concurrent levels of processing (LOP) requirements during encoding (Experiment 2a and 2b). Two main findings emerged. First, subject initiated strategies played a dominant role in VDE effects with little evidence for automaticity. This was demonstrated in Experiment 1 by a more than three-fold increase in the VDE recognition effect when instructions specifically encouraged selective elaboration of high-value items. It was also shown by the complete elimination of VDE recognition effects in Experiment 2 when LOP tasks were concurrently performed during encoding. Critically, the blocking of VDE effects occurred even though a catch trial procedure verified that value was being processed during encoding and remained even when subjects had unlimited time to process the materials during encoding. Second, the data showed, for the first time, that when subjects attempted to specify the value of recognized items, they heavily depended upon a recognition heuristic in which increases in recognition strength, even when nondiagnostic, were inferred to reflect high encoding value. The tendency for subjects to conflate recognition strength and value may have important implications for behavioral economics.</p>","PeriodicalId":48398,"journal":{"name":"Memory & Cognition","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":2.2,"publicationDate":"2024-08-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"140159276","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":3,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Memory & Cognition
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1