首页 > 最新文献

Psychology and Aging最新文献

英文 中文
Age differences in the experience of everyday happiness: The role of thinking about the future. 日常幸福体验中的年龄差异:思考未来的作用。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-26 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000782
Yoonseok Choi, Jennifer Lay, Minjie Lu, Da Jiang, Matthew Peng, Helene H Fung, Peter Graf, Christiane A Hoppmann

Happiness can be experienced differently in young as compared to older adulthood, possibly due to shifts in temporal focus and differences in preferences for high- versus low-arousal affective states. The current project aimed to replicate initial evidence on age-related differences in the experience of happiness by investigating the positive affective correlates of everyday happiness; we further explored the role of thinking about the future in moderating such associations. We used daily life assessments from 257 participants (Mage = 48.3, SDage = 24.6; 68% female; 77% Asian [East Asian, South Asian, and Southeast Asian]; 73% postsecondary educated), combining four data sets collected at two locations (Vancouver, Canada; Hong Kong) with different age samples (older and younger adults). Participants provided up to 30 repeated daily life assessments of momentary affective states and thoughts about the future, over 10 days. Results replicate previous findings by showing that happiness was more strongly associated with low-arousal positive affect and more weakly associated with high-arousal positive affect among older compared to younger adults. Engagement in thinking about the future was higher among younger compared to older adults in general, but its role in moderating the association between happiness and positive affect varying in arousal levels was confounded by the age moderation. Separate analyses conducted for each age group indicate different roles of everyday thinking about the future in shaping happiness experiences for different age groups. Age and future thinking-related contours of happiness are discussed in the context of emotional aging theories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

与老年人相比,年轻人的幸福体验可能有所不同,这可能是由于时间焦点的变化以及对高唤醒和低唤醒情感状态的偏好不同。目前的项目旨在通过调查日常幸福的积极情感相关性,复制与年龄相关的幸福体验差异的初步证据;我们进一步探讨了思考未来在调节这种关联中的作用。我们使用了257名参与者的日常生活评估(Mage=48.3,SDage=24.6;68%为女性;77%为亚洲人[东亚、南亚和东南亚];73%受过高等教育),将在两个地点(加拿大温哥华;香港)收集的四个数据集与不同年龄样本(老年人和年轻人)相结合。参与者在10天内对短暂的情感状态和对未来的想法进行了多达30次重复的日常生活评估。研究结果重复了之前的研究结果,表明与年轻人相比,老年人的幸福感与低唤醒积极情绪的相关性更强,与高唤醒积极情绪相关性更弱。总体而言,与老年人相比,年轻人对未来的思考参与度更高,但其在调节幸福感和积极情感之间的联系方面的作用因年龄的调节而混淆。对每个年龄组进行的单独分析表明,日常对未来的思考在塑造不同年龄组的幸福体验方面发挥着不同的作用。在情感衰老理论的背景下,讨论了与年龄和未来思维相关的幸福轮廓。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Age differences in the experience of everyday happiness: The role of thinking about the future.","authors":"Yoonseok Choi, Jennifer Lay, Minjie Lu, Da Jiang, Matthew Peng, Helene H Fung, Peter Graf, Christiane A Hoppmann","doi":"10.1037/pag0000782","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000782","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Happiness can be experienced differently in young as compared to older adulthood, possibly due to shifts in temporal focus and differences in preferences for high- versus low-arousal affective states. The current project aimed to replicate initial evidence on age-related differences in the experience of happiness by investigating the positive affective correlates of everyday happiness; we further explored the role of thinking about the future in moderating such associations. We used daily life assessments from 257 participants (<i>M</i><sub>age</sub> = 48.3, <i>SD</i><sub>age</sub> = 24.6; 68% female; 77% Asian [East Asian, South Asian, and Southeast Asian]; 73% postsecondary educated), combining four data sets collected at two locations (Vancouver, Canada; Hong Kong) with different age samples (older and younger adults). Participants provided up to 30 repeated daily life assessments of momentary affective states and thoughts about the future, over 10 days. Results replicate previous findings by showing that happiness was more strongly associated with low-arousal positive affect and more weakly associated with high-arousal positive affect among older compared to younger adults. Engagement in thinking about the future was higher among younger compared to older adults in general, but its role in moderating the association between happiness and positive affect varying in arousal levels was confounded by the age moderation. Separate analyses conducted for each age group indicate different roles of everyday thinking about the future in shaping happiness experiences for different age groups. Age and future thinking-related contours of happiness are discussed in the context of emotional aging theories. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"50163297","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Adult age-related differences in susceptibility to social conformity pressures in self-control over daily desires. 成人对日常欲望控制中社会从众压力易感性的年龄相关差异。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-12-07 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000790
Jaime J Castrellon, David H Zald, Gregory R Samanez-Larkin, Kendra L Seaman

Developmental literature suggests that susceptibility to social conformity pressure peaks in adolescence and disappears with maturity into early adulthood. Predictions about these behaviors are less clear for middle-aged and older adults. On the one hand, while age-related increases in prioritization of socioemotional goals might predict greater susceptibility to social conformity pressures, aging is also associated with enhanced emotion regulation that could support resistance to conformity pressures. In this exploratory research study, we used mobile experience sampling surveys to naturalistically track how 157 healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 80 practice self-control over spontaneous desires in daily life. Many of these desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Results showed that middle-aged and older adults were better at controlling their desires than younger adults when desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Consistent with the literature on improved emotion regulation with age, these results provide evidence that the ability to resist social conformity pressure is enhanced across the adult life span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

发展文献表明,对社会从众压力的敏感性在青春期达到顶峰,并随着成熟进入成年早期而消失。对中老年人的这些行为的预测就不那么明确了。一方面,虽然与年龄相关的社会情感目标优先级的增加可能预示着对社会从众压力的更大敏感性,但年龄增长也与增强的情绪调节有关,这可能支持对从众压力的抵抗。在这项探索性研究中,我们采用移动体验抽样调查的方法,自然地跟踪了157名年龄在18岁至80岁之间的健康成年人在日常生活中如何对自发的欲望进行自我控制。这些欲望中的许多都是在其他人在场的情况下实现的。结果表明,当欲望是在其他人的参与下体验时,中老年人比年轻人更善于控制自己的欲望。与有关情绪调节能力随年龄增长而提高的文献一致,这些结果提供了证据,表明在整个成人生活中,抵抗社会从众压力的能力得到增强。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Adult age-related differences in susceptibility to social conformity pressures in self-control over daily desires.","authors":"Jaime J Castrellon, David H Zald, Gregory R Samanez-Larkin, Kendra L Seaman","doi":"10.1037/pag0000790","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000790","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Developmental literature suggests that susceptibility to social conformity pressure peaks in adolescence and disappears with maturity into early adulthood. Predictions about these behaviors are less clear for middle-aged and older adults. On the one hand, while age-related increases in prioritization of socioemotional goals might predict greater susceptibility to social conformity pressures, aging is also associated with enhanced emotion regulation that could support resistance to conformity pressures. In this exploratory research study, we used mobile experience sampling surveys to naturalistically track how 157 healthy adults between the ages of 18 and 80 practice self-control over spontaneous desires in daily life. Many of these desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Results showed that middle-aged and older adults were better at controlling their desires than younger adults when desires were experienced in the presence of others enacting that desire. Consistent with the literature on improved emotion regulation with age, these results provide evidence that the ability to resist social conformity pressure is enhanced across the adult life span. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10922454/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"138499844","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differential impacts of healthy cognitive aging on directed and random exploration. 健康认知老化对定向探索和随机探索的不同影响。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000791
Jack-Morgan Mizell, Siyu Wang, Alec Frisvold, Lily Alvarado, Alex Farrell-Skupny, Waitsang Keung, Caroline E Phelps, Mark H Sundman, Mary-Kathryn Franchetti, Ying-Hui Chou, Gene E Alexander, Robert C Wilson

Deciding whether to explore unknown opportunities or exploit well-known options is a ubiquitous part of our everyday lives. Extensive work in college students suggests that young people make explore-exploit decisions using a mixture of information seeking and random behavioral variability. Whether, and to what extent, older adults use the same strategies is unknown. To address this question, 51 older adults (ages 65-74) and 32 younger adults (ages 18-25) completed the Horizon Task, a gambling task that quantifies information seeking and behavioral variability as well as how these strategies are controlled for the purposes of exploration. Qualitatively, we found that older adults performed similar to younger adults on this task, increasing both their information seeking and behavioral variability when it was adaptive to explore. Quantitively, however, there were substantial differences between the age groups, with older adults showing less information seeking overall and less reliance on variability as a means to explore. In addition, we found a subset of approximately 26% of older adults whose information seeking was close to zero, avoiding informative options even when they were clearly the better choice. Unsurprisingly, these "information avoiders" performed worse on the task. In contrast, task performance in the remaining "information seeking" older adults was comparable to that of younger adults suggesting that age-related differences in explore-exploit decision making may be adaptive except when they are taken to extremes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

决定是探索未知的机会还是利用众所周知的选择是我们日常生活中无处不在的一部分。针对大学生的大量研究表明,年轻人在做出探索-利用决策时,会混合使用信息搜寻和随机行为变异。至于老年人是否以及在多大程度上使用同样的策略,我们还不得而知。为了解决这个问题,51 名老年人(65-74 岁)和 32 名年轻人(18-25 岁)完成了地平线任务,这是一项赌博任务,可以量化信息寻求和行为变异以及如何控制这些策略以达到探索的目的。我们发现,老年人在这项任务中的表现与年轻人相似,在适应探索的情况下,他们的信息搜寻和行为变异性都会增加。然而,从数量上看,两个年龄组之间存在着很大的差异,老年人在整体上表现出较少的信息寻求,也较少依赖变异性作为探索的手段。此外,我们还发现约有 26% 的老年人对信息的寻求接近于零,即使信息选项明显是更好的选择,他们也会回避。毫不奇怪,这些 "信息回避者 "在任务中的表现更差。相比之下,其余 "寻求信息 "的老年人在任务中的表现与年轻人相当,这表明与年龄有关的探索-利用决策差异可能是适应性的,除非它们被推向极端。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Differential impacts of healthy cognitive aging on directed and random exploration.","authors":"Jack-Morgan Mizell, Siyu Wang, Alec Frisvold, Lily Alvarado, Alex Farrell-Skupny, Waitsang Keung, Caroline E Phelps, Mark H Sundman, Mary-Kathryn Franchetti, Ying-Hui Chou, Gene E Alexander, Robert C Wilson","doi":"10.1037/pag0000791","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000791","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Deciding whether to explore unknown opportunities or exploit well-known options is a ubiquitous part of our everyday lives. Extensive work in college students suggests that young people make explore-exploit decisions using a mixture of information seeking and random behavioral variability. Whether, and to what extent, older adults use the same strategies is unknown. To address this question, 51 older adults (ages 65-74) and 32 younger adults (ages 18-25) completed the Horizon Task, a gambling task that quantifies information seeking and behavioral variability as well as how these strategies are controlled for the purposes of exploration. Qualitatively, we found that older adults performed similar to younger adults on this task, increasing both their information seeking and behavioral variability when it was adaptive to explore. Quantitively, however, there were substantial differences between the age groups, with older adults showing less information seeking overall and less reliance on variability as a means to explore. In addition, we found a subset of approximately 26% of older adults whose information seeking was close to zero, avoiding informative options even when they were clearly the better choice. Unsurprisingly, these \"information avoiders\" performed worse on the task. In contrast, task performance in the remaining \"information seeking\" older adults was comparable to that of younger adults suggesting that age-related differences in explore-exploit decision making may be adaptive except when they are taken to extremes. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC10871551/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736407","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Differences in the content and coherence of autobiographical memories between younger and older adults: Insights from text analysis. 年轻人和老年人自传式记忆的内容和连贯性的差异:来自文本分析的见解。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-07-20 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000769
Signy Sheldon, Jay Sheldon, Shirley Zhang, Roni Setton, Gary R Turner, R Nathan Spreng, Matthew D Grilli

Several studies have shown that older adults generate autobiographical memories with fewer specific details than younger adults, a pattern typically attributed to age-relate declines in episodic memory. A relatively unexplored question is how aging affects the content used to represent and recall these memories. We recently proposed that older adults may predominately represent and recall autobiographical memories at the gist level. Emerging from this proposal is the hypothesis that older adults represent memories with a wider array of content topics and recall memories with a distinct narrative style when compared to younger adults. We tested this hypothesis by applying natural language processing approaches to a data set of autobiographical memories described by healthy younger and older adults. We used topic modeling to estimate the distribution (i.e., diversity) of content topics used to represent a memory, and sentence embedding to derive an internal similarity score to estimate the shifts in content when narrating a memory. First, we found that older adults referenced a wider array of content topics (higher content diversity) than younger adults when recalling their autobiographical memories. Second, we found older adults were included more content shifts when narrating their memories than younger adults, suggesting a reduced reliance on choronology to form a coherent memory. Third, we found that the content diversity measures were positively related to specific detail generation for older adults, potentially reflecting age-related compensation for episodic memory difficulties. We discuss how our results shed light on how younger and older adults differ in the way they remember and describe the past. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

几项研究表明,老年人产生的自传式记忆的具体细节比年轻人少,这种模式通常归因于与年龄相关的情景记忆衰退。一个相对未被探索的问题是,衰老是如何影响用来表达和回忆这些记忆的内容的。我们最近提出,老年人可能在主旨水平上主要代表和回忆自传体记忆。从这一提议中产生的假设是,与年轻人相比,老年人代表的记忆具有更广泛的内容主题,并且以独特的叙述风格回忆记忆。我们通过将自然语言处理方法应用于健康的年轻人和老年人描述的自传式记忆数据集来验证这一假设。我们使用主题建模来估计用于表示记忆的内容主题的分布(即多样性),并使用句子嵌入来获得内部相似度评分,以估计叙述记忆时内容的变化。首先,我们发现老年人在回忆自传体记忆时引用的内容主题比年轻人更广泛(内容多样性更高)。其次,我们发现老年人在叙述他们的记忆时比年轻人包含更多的内容变化,这表明他们对时间顺序形成连贯记忆的依赖减少了。第三,我们发现内容多样性测量与老年人的具体细节生成呈正相关,可能反映了与年龄相关的情景记忆困难补偿。我们讨论了我们的研究结果如何揭示了年轻人和老年人在记忆和描述过去的方式上的差异。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c) 2023 APA,版权所有)。
{"title":"Differences in the content and coherence of autobiographical memories between younger and older adults: Insights from text analysis.","authors":"Signy Sheldon, Jay Sheldon, Shirley Zhang, Roni Setton, Gary R Turner, R Nathan Spreng, Matthew D Grilli","doi":"10.1037/pag0000769","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000769","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Several studies have shown that older adults generate autobiographical memories with fewer specific details than younger adults, a pattern typically attributed to age-relate declines in episodic memory. A relatively unexplored question is how aging affects the content used to represent and recall these memories. We recently proposed that older adults may predominately represent and recall autobiographical memories at the gist level. Emerging from this proposal is the hypothesis that older adults represent memories with a wider array of content topics and recall memories with a distinct narrative style when compared to younger adults. We tested this hypothesis by applying natural language processing approaches to a data set of autobiographical memories described by healthy younger and older adults. We used topic modeling to estimate the distribution (i.e., diversity) of content topics used to represent a memory, and sentence embedding to derive an internal similarity score to estimate the shifts in content when narrating a memory. First, we found that older adults referenced a wider array of content topics (higher content diversity) than younger adults when recalling their autobiographical memories. Second, we found older adults were included more content shifts when narrating their memories than younger adults, suggesting a reduced reliance on choronology to form a coherent memory. Third, we found that the content diversity measures were positively related to specific detail generation for older adults, potentially reflecting age-related compensation for episodic memory difficulties. We discuss how our results shed light on how younger and older adults differ in the way they remember and describe the past. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"9893129","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Profiles of activity engagement and depression trajectories as COVID-19 restrictions were relaxed. 随着新冠肺炎限制的放松,活动参与和抑郁轨迹的概况。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-23 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000785
Jonathan L Chia, Andree Hartanto, William Tov

Given elevated depression rates since the onset of the pandemic and potential downstream implications, this research examined the association between activity engagement and depression among middle-aged and older adults postlockdown. This study aimed to (a) identify activity engagement profiles among middle-aged and older adults, (b) understand factors associated with profile memberships, and (c) compare depression trajectories across profiles as COVID-19 restrictions eased over 16 months in Singapore. This longitudinal study involved 6,568 middle-aged and older adults. Latent growth analysis was first conducted to obtain estimates of depression trajectories for each individual. Latent profile analysis was then conducted to identify different activity profiles. Finally, profile characteristics and depression trajectories across these different profiles were compared. Results indicated four profiles that varied in social and physical activity. Although digital activity was negatively associated with depression at baseline, it did not explain depression trajectories as restrictions eased. Over time, depression decreased for all profiles; however, those who were inactive on all activities except digital contact tended to experience more persistent symptoms, compared with those who were highly engaged in physical and outdoor activities. Individuals who were only active digitally tended to experience more prepandemic negative affect, were more introverted and neurotic, less open, agreeable, and conscientious, and had worse health and mobility, lower income, and lower education. Findings highlight how imprecise conceptualizations of activity engagement may obscure subtle activity engagement-depression relations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

考虑到自疫情爆发以来抑郁率的上升以及潜在的下游影响,这项研究考察了封锁后中老年人的活动参与与抑郁之间的关系。这项研究旨在(a)确定中老年人的活动参与情况,(b)了解与个人资料成员资格相关的因素,以及(c)随着新冠肺炎限制措施在新加坡放松16个月,比较个人资料中的抑郁轨迹。这项纵向研究涉及6568名中老年人。首先进行了潜在生长分析,以获得每个个体抑郁轨迹的估计值。然后进行潜在特征分析以识别不同的活动特征。最后,对这些不同剖面的剖面特征和凹陷轨迹进行了比较。结果显示,有四种情况在社交和体育活动方面有所不同。尽管数字活动在基线时与抑郁症呈负相关,但随着限制的放松,它并不能解释抑郁症的轨迹。随着时间的推移,所有剖面的抑郁情绪都有所下降;然而,与那些高度参与体育和户外活动的人相比,那些除数字接触外不参与所有活动的人往往会出现更持久的症状。仅在数字方面活跃的个人往往会经历更多的疫情前负面影响,更内向、更神经质,不那么开放、随和和认真,健康和行动能力较差,收入较低,教育程度较低。研究结果强调了活动参与的不精确概念可能会掩盖微妙的活动参与-抑郁关系。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Profiles of activity engagement and depression trajectories as COVID-19 restrictions were relaxed.","authors":"Jonathan L Chia, Andree Hartanto, William Tov","doi":"10.1037/pag0000785","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000785","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Given elevated depression rates since the onset of the pandemic and potential downstream implications, this research examined the association between activity engagement and depression among middle-aged and older adults postlockdown. This study aimed to (a) identify activity engagement profiles among middle-aged and older adults, (b) understand factors associated with profile memberships, and (c) compare depression trajectories across profiles as COVID-19 restrictions eased over 16 months in Singapore. This longitudinal study involved 6,568 middle-aged and older adults. Latent growth analysis was first conducted to obtain estimates of depression trajectories for each individual. Latent profile analysis was then conducted to identify different activity profiles. Finally, profile characteristics and depression trajectories across these different profiles were compared. Results indicated four profiles that varied in social and physical activity. Although digital activity was negatively associated with depression at baseline, it did not explain depression trajectories as restrictions eased. Over time, depression decreased for all profiles; however, those who were inactive on all activities except digital contact tended to experience more persistent symptoms, compared with those who were highly engaged in physical and outdoor activities. Individuals who were only active digitally tended to experience more prepandemic negative affect, were more introverted and neurotic, less open, agreeable, and conscientious, and had worse health and mobility, lower income, and lower education. Findings highlight how imprecise conceptualizations of activity engagement may obscure subtle activity engagement-depression relations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"49693209","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Limited time horizons lead to the positivity effect in attention, but not to more positive emotions: An investigation of the socioemotional selectivity theory. 有限的时间范围会导致注意力的积极效应,但不会导致更积极的情绪:对社会情绪选择性理论的调查。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000781
Isabella Zsoldos, Pascal Hot

A positivity effect in attention (i.e., an attentional bias in favor of positive over negative stimuli) has been frequently reported in older adults. Based on the postulates of socioemotional selectivity theory (SST), the present study tested whether this positivity effect: (a) depends on the subjective perception of a limited future time perspective (FTP) independently of chronological age, (b) involves controlled processes, and (c) contributes to optimizing positive emotions. Thirty-one older adults (aged 75-93) and 92 younger adults (aged 18-23) were recruited. Young adults were divided into a control group (N = 52) and a group with limited FTP (N = 40), where their subjective perception of the time left to live was experimentally reduced. All participants performed a dot-probe task involving positive, negative and neutral pictures displayed with different presentation durations (500 ms, 1,000 ms). Reaction time bias scores were calculated, and emotional state was measured several times during the task. Analyses revealed attentional biases toward positive (compared to negative) pictures in older adults and young adults with limited FTP, but not in young adults in the control group. These positivity effects appeared from 500 ms of stimulus presentation, did not increase over time, and did not correlate with participants' emotions. These findings support SST predictions that positivity effects occur when individuals perceive a limited FTP, regardless of their actual age. However, our data also suggest that the positivity effect may be a more automatic than controlled process that does not influence emotional state. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

在老年人中,注意力的积极效应(即倾向于积极而非消极刺激的注意力偏差)经常被报道。基于社会情感选择性理论(SST)的假设,本研究测试了这种积极效应是否:(a)取决于对有限的未来时间视角(FTP)的主观感知,与实际年龄无关,(b)涉及受控过程,(c)有助于优化积极情绪。招募了31名老年人(年龄75-93岁)和92名年轻人(年龄18-23岁)。年轻人被分为对照组(N=52)和FTP受限组(N=40),在这两组中,他们对剩余生存时间的主观感知在实验上减少了。所有参与者都执行了一项点探测任务,涉及以不同呈现持续时间(500毫秒、1000毫秒)显示的正面、负面和中性图片。计算反应时间偏差得分,并在任务期间多次测量情绪状态。分析显示,在FTP有限的老年人和年轻人中,对正面(与负面相比)图片的注意力存在偏见,但在对照组的年轻人中没有。这些积极影响出现在500毫秒的刺激表现中,不会随着时间的推移而增加,也与参与者的情绪无关。这些发现支持SST的预测,即当个体感知到有限的FTP时,无论其实际年龄如何,都会产生积极影响。然而,我们的数据也表明,积极效应可能是一个比受控过程更自动的过程,不会影响情绪状态。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Limited time horizons lead to the positivity effect in attention, but not to more positive emotions: An investigation of the socioemotional selectivity theory.","authors":"Isabella Zsoldos, Pascal Hot","doi":"10.1037/pag0000781","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000781","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>A positivity effect in attention (i.e., an attentional bias in favor of positive over negative stimuli) has been frequently reported in older adults. Based on the postulates of socioemotional selectivity theory (SST), the present study tested whether this positivity effect: (a) depends on the subjective perception of a limited future time perspective (FTP) independently of chronological age, (b) involves controlled processes, and (c) contributes to optimizing positive emotions. Thirty-one older adults (aged 75-93) and 92 younger adults (aged 18-23) were recruited. Young adults were divided into a control group (<i>N</i> = 52) and a group with limited FTP (<i>N</i> = 40), where their subjective perception of the time left to live was experimentally reduced. All participants performed a dot-probe task involving positive, negative and neutral pictures displayed with different presentation durations (500 ms, 1,000 ms). Reaction time bias scores were calculated, and emotional state was measured several times during the task. Analyses revealed attentional biases toward positive (compared to negative) pictures in older adults and young adults with limited FTP, but not in young adults in the control group. These positivity effects appeared from 500 ms of stimulus presentation, did not increase over time, and did not correlate with participants' emotions. These findings support SST predictions that positivity effects occur when individuals perceive a limited FTP, regardless of their actual age. However, our data also suggest that the positivity effect may be a more automatic than controlled process that does not influence emotional state. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41216415","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Stability and change of optimism and pessimism in late midlife and old age across three independent studies. 在三项独立研究中,中老年乐观和悲观情绪的稳定性和变化。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Q1 GERONTOLOGY Pub Date : 2024-02-01 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000789
Julia Tetzner, Johanna Drewelies, Sandra Duezel, Ilja Demuth, Gert G Wagner, Margie Lachman, Ulman Lindenberger, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf

Research across a number of different areas in psychology has long shown that optimism and pessimism are predictive of a number of important future life outcomes. Despite a vast literature on the correlates and consequences, we know very little about how optimism and pessimism change across adulthood and old age and the sociodemographic factors that are associated with individual differences in such trajectories. In the present study, we conducted (parallel) analyses of standard items from the Life Orientation Test (Scheier & Carver, 1985) in three comprehensive data sets: Two-wave data from both the Berlin Aging Study II (N = 1,423, aged 60-88; M = 70.4, SD = 3.70) and the Midlife in the U.S. Study (N = 1,810 aged 60-84; M = 69.12, SD = 6.47) as well as cross-sectional data from the Survey of Health, Aging, and Retirement (N = 17,087, aged 60-99; M = 70.19, SD = 7.53). Using latent change-regression models and locally weighted smoothing curves revealed that optimism is on average very stable after age 60, with some evidence in Survey of Health, Aging, and Retirement of lowered optimism in very old age. Consistent across the three independent studies, pessimism evinced on average modest increases, ranging between .25 and .50 SD per 10 years of age. Of the sociodemographic factors examined, higher levels of education revealed the most consistent associations with lower pessimism, whereas gender evinced more study-specific findings. We take our results to demonstrate that age-related trajectories and correlates thereof differ for optimism and pessimism. Older adults appear to preserve into older ages those levels of optimistic expectations they have had at 60 years of age and show only modest increases in pessimism. We discuss possible reasons for these findings. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

心理学多个不同领域的研究早已表明,乐观和悲观可预测未来生活的一系列重要结果。尽管有大量文献对其相关性和后果进行了研究,但我们对乐观和悲观情绪在成年期和老年期的变化以及与个体差异相关的社会人口因素知之甚少。在本研究中,我们通过三个综合数据集对生活取向测验(Scheier & Carver,1985 年)的标准项目进行了(平行)分析:柏林老龄化研究 II(N = 1,423 人,60-88 岁;M = 70.4,SD = 3.70)和美国中年研究(N = 1,810 人,60-84 岁;M = 69.12,SD = 6.47)的两波数据,以及健康、老龄化和退休调查(N = 17,087 人,60-99 岁;M = 70.19,SD = 7.53)的横截面数据。通过使用潜在变化回归模型和局部加权平滑曲线发现,平均而言,60 岁以后的乐观情绪非常稳定,但《健康、老龄化和退休调查》中有一些证据表明,老年期的乐观情绪会降低。三项独立研究一致表明,悲观情绪平均增幅不大,每 10 年年龄增长 0.25 至 0.50 SD 不等。在所研究的社会人口因素中,教育水平越高,悲观情绪越低,而性别则与研究结果有更多的关联。我们的研究结果表明,与年龄相关的轨迹及其相关因素对乐观和悲观的影响是不同的。老年人在 60 岁时的乐观预期水平似乎一直保持到老年,而悲观情绪仅略有增加。我们将讨论这些发现的可能原因。(PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, 版权所有)。
{"title":"Stability and change of optimism and pessimism in late midlife and old age across three independent studies.","authors":"Julia Tetzner, Johanna Drewelies, Sandra Duezel, Ilja Demuth, Gert G Wagner, Margie Lachman, Ulman Lindenberger, Nilam Ram, Denis Gerstorf","doi":"10.1037/pag0000789","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000789","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Research across a number of different areas in psychology has long shown that optimism and pessimism are predictive of a number of important future life outcomes. Despite a vast literature on the correlates and consequences, we know very little about how optimism and pessimism change across adulthood and old age and the sociodemographic factors that are associated with individual differences in such trajectories. In the present study, we conducted (parallel) analyses of standard items from the Life Orientation Test (Scheier & Carver, 1985) in three comprehensive data sets: Two-wave data from both the Berlin Aging Study II (N = 1,423, aged 60-88; M = 70.4, SD = 3.70) and the Midlife in the U.S. Study (N = 1,810 aged 60-84; M = 69.12, SD = 6.47) as well as cross-sectional data from the Survey of Health, Aging, and Retirement (N = 17,087, aged 60-99; M = 70.19, SD = 7.53). Using latent change-regression models and locally weighted smoothing curves revealed that optimism is on average very stable after age 60, with some evidence in Survey of Health, Aging, and Retirement of lowered optimism in very old age. Consistent across the three independent studies, pessimism evinced on average modest increases, ranging between .25 and .50 SD per 10 years of age. Of the sociodemographic factors examined, higher levels of education revealed the most consistent associations with lower pessimism, whereas gender evinced more study-specific findings. We take our results to demonstrate that age-related trajectories and correlates thereof differ for optimism and pessimism. Older adults appear to preserve into older ages those levels of optimistic expectations they have had at 60 years of age and show only modest increases in pessimism. We discuss possible reasons for these findings. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC11406507/pdf/","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139736408","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"OA","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Public events knowledge in an age-heterogeneous sample: Reminiscence bump or bummer? 年龄异质样本中的公共事件知识:回忆颠簸还是颠簸?
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-02-01 Epub Date: 2023-11-02 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000786
Priscilla Achaa-Amankwaa, Diana Steger, Oliver Wilhelm, Ulrich Schroeders

The reminiscence bump describes an increased recollection of autobiographic experiences made in adolescence and early adulthood. It is unclear if this phenomenon can also be found in declarative knowledge of past public events. To answer this question, we assessed public events knowledge (PEK) about the past 6 decades with a 120-item knowledge test across six domains in a sample of 1,012 Germans that were sampled uniformly across the ages of 30-80 years. General and domain-specific PEK scores were analyzed as a function of age-at-event. Scores were lower for public events preceding participants' birth and stayed stable from the age-at-event of 5-10 years onward. There was no significant peak in PEK in adolescence or early adulthood, arguing against an extension of the reminiscence effect to factual knowledge. We examined associations between PEK and relevant variables such as crystallized intelligence (Gc), news consumption, and openness to experience with structural equation models. Strong associations between PEK and Gc were established, whereas the associations of PEK with news consumption and openness were mainly driven by their link to declarative knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).

回忆突起描述了对青春期和成年早期自传体经历的回忆增加。目前尚不清楚这种现象是否也可以在过去公共事件的陈述性知识中找到。为了回答这个问题,我们对1012名年龄在30-80岁之间的德国人进行了六个领域的120项知识测试,评估了过去60年的公共事件知识(PEK)。将一般和领域特异性PEK评分作为活动年龄的函数进行分析。参与者出生前的公共活动得分较低,从5-10岁起保持稳定。在青春期或成年早期,PEK没有显著的峰值,反对将回忆效应扩展到事实知识。我们用结构方程模型研究了PEK与相关变量之间的关系,如结晶智力(Gc)、新闻消费和体验开放度。PEK和Gc之间建立了强烈的关联,而PEK与新闻消费和开放性的关联主要是由它们与陈述性知识的联系驱动的。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Public events knowledge in an age-heterogeneous sample: Reminiscence bump or bummer?","authors":"Priscilla Achaa-Amankwaa, Diana Steger, Oliver Wilhelm, Ulrich Schroeders","doi":"10.1037/pag0000786","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000786","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>The <i>reminiscence bump</i> describes an increased recollection of autobiographic experiences made in adolescence and early adulthood. It is unclear if this phenomenon can also be found in declarative knowledge of past public events. To answer this question, we assessed public events knowledge (PEK) about the past 6 decades with a 120-item knowledge test across six domains in a sample of 1,012 Germans that were sampled uniformly across the ages of 30-80 years. General and domain-specific PEK scores were analyzed as a function of age-at-event. Scores were lower for public events preceding participants' birth and stayed stable from the age-at-event of 5-10 years onward. There was no significant peak in PEK in adolescence or early adulthood, arguing against an extension of the reminiscence effect to factual knowledge. We examined associations between PEK and relevant variables such as crystallized intelligence (Gc), news consumption, and openness to experience with structural equation models. Strong associations between PEK and Gc were established, whereas the associations of PEK with news consumption and openness were mainly driven by their link to declarative knowledge. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-02-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"71428051","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Supplemental Material for Audiovisual Speech Perception in Noise in Younger and Older Bilinguals 青年和老年双语者噪音中的视听语音感知补充材料
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2024-01-25 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000799.supp
{"title":"Supplemental Material for Audiovisual Speech Perception in Noise in Younger and Older Bilinguals","authors":"","doi":"10.1037/pag0000799.supp","DOIUrl":"https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000799.supp","url":null,"abstract":"","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2024-01-25","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"139597121","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
Long-term aging trajectories of the accumulation of disease burden as predictors of daily affect dynamics and stressor reactivity. 疾病负担积累的长期衰老轨迹作为日常情感动态和压力源反应的预测因素。
IF 3.7 1区 心理学 Pub Date : 2023-12-01 Epub Date: 2023-10-12 DOI: 10.1037/pag0000779
Denis Gerstorf, Oliver K Schilling, Theresa Pauly, Martin Katzorreck, Anna J Lücke, Hans-Werner Wahl, Ute Kunzmann, Christiane A Hoppmann, Nilam Ram

Multiple-timescale studies provide new opportunities to examine how developmental processes that evolve at different cadences are intertwined. Developmental theories of emotion regulation suggest that the long-term, slowly evolving age-related accumulation of disease burden should shape short-term, faster evolving (daily) affective experiences. To empirically examine this proposition, we combined data from 123 old adults (65-69 years, 47% women) and 32 very old adults (85-88 years, 59% women) who provided 20 + year within-person longitudinal data on physician-rated morbidity and subsequently also completed repeated daily-life assessments of stress and affect six times a day over 7 consecutive days as they were going about their daily-life routines. Results from models that simultaneously articulate growth and intraindividual variability processes (in a dynamic structural equation modeling framework) revealed that individual differences in long-term aging trajectories of the accumulation of disease burden were indeed predictive of differences in three facets of affective dynamics that manifest in everyday life. In particular-over and above mean levels of disease burden-older adults whose disease burden had increased more over the past 20 years had higher base level of negative affect in their daily lives, more emotional reactivity to the experience of daily stressors, and more moment-to-moment fluctuations in negative affect that was unrelated to stressors (affective systemic noise). We highlight that developmental processes evolving over vastly different timescales are intertwined, and speculate how new knowledge about those relations can inform developmental theories of emotion regulation and daily-life functioning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).

多个时间尺度的研究提供了新的机会来研究以不同节奏进化的发展过程是如何交织在一起的。情绪调节的发展理论表明,与年龄相关的疾病负担的长期、缓慢演变的积累应该塑造短期、快速演变的(日常)情感体验。为了实证地检验这个命题,我们综合了123名老年人(65-69岁,47%为女性)和32名高龄成年人(85-88岁,59%为女性)的数据,他们提供了20多年的医生评定的发病率纵向数据,随后在日常生活中连续7天每天6次完成了对压力和影响的重复日常生活评估。同时阐明生长和个体内变异过程的模型(在动态结构方程建模框架中)的结果表明,疾病负担积累的长期衰老轨迹中的个体差异确实可以预测日常生活中表现出的情感动力学三个方面的差异。特别是超过平均疾病负担水平的老年人,他们的疾病负担在过去20年中增加得更多,他们在日常生活中的负面影响的基础水平更高,对日常压力源的体验有更多的情绪反应,与压力源无关的负面影响(情感系统噪音)也有更多的瞬间波动。我们强调,在截然不同的时间尺度上进化的发展过程是相互交织的,并推测关于这些关系的新知识如何为情绪调节和日常生活功能的发展理论提供信息。(PsycInfo数据库记录(c)2023 APA,保留所有权利)。
{"title":"Long-term aging trajectories of the accumulation of disease burden as predictors of daily affect dynamics and stressor reactivity.","authors":"Denis Gerstorf, Oliver K Schilling, Theresa Pauly, Martin Katzorreck, Anna J Lücke, Hans-Werner Wahl, Ute Kunzmann, Christiane A Hoppmann, Nilam Ram","doi":"10.1037/pag0000779","DOIUrl":"10.1037/pag0000779","url":null,"abstract":"<p><p>Multiple-timescale studies provide new opportunities to examine how developmental processes that evolve at different cadences are intertwined. Developmental theories of emotion regulation suggest that the long-term, slowly evolving age-related accumulation of disease burden should shape short-term, faster evolving (daily) affective experiences. To empirically examine this proposition, we combined data from 123 old adults (65-69 years, 47% women) and 32 very old adults (85-88 years, 59% women) who provided 20 + year within-person longitudinal data on physician-rated morbidity and subsequently also completed repeated daily-life assessments of stress and affect six times a day over 7 consecutive days as they were going about their daily-life routines. Results from models that simultaneously articulate growth and intraindividual variability processes (in a dynamic structural equation modeling framework) revealed that individual differences in long-term aging trajectories of the accumulation of disease burden were indeed predictive of differences in three facets of affective dynamics that manifest in everyday life. In particular-over and above mean levels of disease burden-older adults whose disease burden had increased more over the past 20 years had higher base level of negative affect in their daily lives, more emotional reactivity to the experience of daily stressors, and more moment-to-moment fluctuations in negative affect that was unrelated to stressors (affective systemic noise). We highlight that developmental processes evolving over vastly different timescales are intertwined, and speculate how new knowledge about those relations can inform developmental theories of emotion regulation and daily-life functioning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).</p>","PeriodicalId":48426,"journal":{"name":"Psychology and Aging","volume":null,"pages":null},"PeriodicalIF":3.7,"publicationDate":"2023-12-01","publicationTypes":"Journal Article","fieldsOfStudy":null,"isOpenAccess":false,"openAccessPdf":"","citationCount":null,"resultStr":null,"platform":"Semanticscholar","paperid":"41216416","PeriodicalName":null,"FirstCategoryId":null,"ListUrlMain":null,"RegionNum":1,"RegionCategory":"心理学","ArticlePicture":[],"TitleCN":null,"AbstractTextCN":null,"PMCID":"","EPubDate":null,"PubModel":null,"JCR":null,"JCRName":null,"Score":null,"Total":0}
引用次数: 0
期刊
Psychology and Aging
全部 Acc. Chem. Res. ACS Applied Bio Materials ACS Appl. Electron. Mater. ACS Appl. Energy Mater. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces ACS Appl. Nano Mater. ACS Appl. Polym. Mater. ACS BIOMATER-SCI ENG ACS Catal. ACS Cent. Sci. ACS Chem. Biol. ACS Chemical Health & Safety ACS Chem. Neurosci. ACS Comb. Sci. ACS Earth Space Chem. ACS Energy Lett. ACS Infect. Dis. ACS Macro Lett. ACS Mater. Lett. ACS Med. Chem. Lett. ACS Nano ACS Omega ACS Photonics ACS Sens. ACS Sustainable Chem. Eng. ACS Synth. Biol. Anal. Chem. BIOCHEMISTRY-US Bioconjugate Chem. BIOMACROMOLECULES Chem. Res. Toxicol. Chem. Rev. Chem. Mater. CRYST GROWTH DES ENERG FUEL Environ. Sci. Technol. Environ. Sci. Technol. Lett. Eur. J. Inorg. Chem. IND ENG CHEM RES Inorg. Chem. J. Agric. Food. Chem. J. Chem. Eng. Data J. Chem. Educ. J. Chem. Inf. Model. J. Chem. Theory Comput. J. Med. Chem. J. Nat. Prod. J PROTEOME RES J. Am. Chem. Soc. LANGMUIR MACROMOLECULES Mol. Pharmaceutics Nano Lett. Org. Lett. ORG PROCESS RES DEV ORGANOMETALLICS J. Org. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. J. Phys. Chem. A J. Phys. Chem. B J. Phys. Chem. C J. Phys. Chem. Lett. Analyst Anal. Methods Biomater. Sci. Catal. Sci. Technol. Chem. Commun. Chem. Soc. Rev. CHEM EDUC RES PRACT CRYSTENGCOMM Dalton Trans. Energy Environ. Sci. ENVIRON SCI-NANO ENVIRON SCI-PROC IMP ENVIRON SCI-WAT RES Faraday Discuss. Food Funct. Green Chem. Inorg. Chem. Front. Integr. Biol. J. Anal. At. Spectrom. J. Mater. Chem. A J. Mater. Chem. B J. Mater. Chem. C Lab Chip Mater. Chem. Front. Mater. Horiz. MEDCHEMCOMM Metallomics Mol. Biosyst. Mol. Syst. Des. Eng. Nanoscale Nanoscale Horiz. Nat. Prod. Rep. New J. Chem. Org. Biomol. Chem. Org. Chem. Front. PHOTOCH PHOTOBIO SCI PCCP Polym. Chem.
×
引用
GB/T 7714-2015
复制
MLA
复制
APA
复制
导出至
BibTeX EndNote RefMan NoteFirst NoteExpress
×
0
微信
客服QQ
Book学术公众号 扫码关注我们
反馈
×
意见反馈
请填写您的意见或建议
请填写您的手机或邮箱
×
提示
您的信息不完整,为了账户安全,请先补充。
现在去补充
×
提示
您因"违规操作"
具体请查看互助需知
我知道了
×
提示
现在去查看 取消
×
提示
确定
Book学术官方微信
Book学术文献互助
Book学术文献互助群
群 号:481959085
Book学术
文献互助 智能选刊 最新文献 互助须知 联系我们:info@booksci.cn
Book学术提供免费学术资源搜索服务,方便国内外学者检索中英文文献。致力于提供最便捷和优质的服务体验。
Copyright © 2023 Book学术 All rights reserved.
ghs 京公网安备 11010802042870号 京ICP备2023020795号-1